首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Processing最新文献

英文 中文
Describing and understanding the time course of the property listing task. 描述和理解房地产上市任务的时间进程。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01160-2
Enrique Canessa, Sergio E Chaigneau, Sebastián Moreno

To study linguistically coded concepts, researchers often resort to the Property Listing Task (PLT). In a PLT, participants are asked to list properties that describe a concept (e.g., for DOG, subjects may list "is a pet", "has four legs", etc.). When PLT data is collected for many concepts, researchers obtain Conceptual Properties Norms (CPNs), which are used to study semantic content and as a source of control variables. Though the PLT and CPNs are widely used across psychology, only recently a model that describes the listing course of a PLT has been developed and validated. That original model describes the listing course using order of production of properties. Here we go a step beyond and validate the model using response times (RT), i.e., the time from cue onset to property listing. Our results show that RT data exhibits the same regularities observed in the previous model, but now we can also analyze the time course, i.e., dynamics of the PLT. As such, the RT validated model may be applied to study several similar memory retrieval tasks, such as the Free Listing Task, Verbal Fluidity Task, and to research related cognitive processes. To illustrate those kinds of analyses, we present a brief example of the difference in PLT's dynamics between listing properties for abstract versus concrete concepts, which shows that the model may be fruitfully applied to study concepts.

为了研究语言编码概念,研究人员通常会采用 "属性列表任务"(PLT)。在 PLT 中,受试者被要求列出描述某个概念的属性(例如,对于 DOG,受试者可能会列出 "是宠物"、"有四条腿 "等)。当收集到许多概念的 PLT 数据后,研究人员就能获得概念属性规范(CPN),用于研究语义内容并作为控制变量的来源。虽然 PLT 和 CPN 在心理学中得到了广泛应用,但直到最近才开发出一个描述 PLT 列表过程的模型并得到验证。最初的模型使用属性产生的顺序来描述罗列过程。在这里,我们更进一步,使用反应时间(RT)验证了这一模型,即从提示开始到列出属性的时间。我们的结果表明,RT 数据与之前模型中观察到的规律性相同,但现在我们还可以分析时间过程,即 PLT 的动态变化。因此,RT 验证模型可用于研究几种类似的记忆检索任务,如自由列表任务、言语流畅性任务,以及研究相关的认知过程。为了说明这些分析,我们以抽象概念与具体概念的列表属性为例,简要介绍了 PLT 动态性的差异,这表明该模型可有效地应用于概念的研究。
{"title":"Describing and understanding the time course of the property listing task.","authors":"Enrique Canessa, Sergio E Chaigneau, Sebastián Moreno","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01160-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01160-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To study linguistically coded concepts, researchers often resort to the Property Listing Task (PLT). In a PLT, participants are asked to list properties that describe a concept (e.g., for DOG, subjects may list \"is a pet\", \"has four legs\", etc.). When PLT data is collected for many concepts, researchers obtain Conceptual Properties Norms (CPNs), which are used to study semantic content and as a source of control variables. Though the PLT and CPNs are widely used across psychology, only recently a model that describes the listing course of a PLT has been developed and validated. That original model describes the listing course using order of production of properties. Here we go a step beyond and validate the model using response times (RT), i.e., the time from cue onset to property listing. Our results show that RT data exhibits the same regularities observed in the previous model, but now we can also analyze the time course, i.e., dynamics of the PLT. As such, the RT validated model may be applied to study several similar memory retrieval tasks, such as the Free Listing Task, Verbal Fluidity Task, and to research related cognitive processes. To illustrate those kinds of analyses, we present a brief example of the difference in PLT's dynamics between listing properties for abstract versus concrete concepts, which shows that the model may be fruitfully applied to study concepts.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10268634","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the role of singing, semantics, and amusia screening in speech-in-noise perception in musicians and non-musicians. 探讨歌唱、语义和娱乐性筛选在音乐家和非音乐家的噪声感知中的作用。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01165-x
Ariadne Loutrari, Aseel Alqadi, Cunmei Jiang, Fang Liu

Sentence repetition has been the focus of extensive psycholinguistic research. The notion that music training can bolster speech perception in adverse auditory conditions has been met with mixed results. In this work, we sought to gauge the effect of babble noise on immediate repetition of spoken and sung phrases of varying semantic content (expository, narrative, and anomalous), initially in 100 English-speaking monolinguals with and without music training. The two cohorts also completed some non-musical cognitive tests and the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA). When disregarding MBEA results, musicians were found to significantly outperform non-musicians in terms of overall repetition accuracy. Sung targets were recalled significantly better than spoken ones across groups in the presence of babble noise. Sung expository targets were recalled better than spoken expository ones, and semantically anomalous content was recalled more poorly in noise. Rerunning the analysis after eliminating thirteen participants who were diagnosed with amusia showed no significant group differences. This suggests that the notion of enhanced speech perception-in noise or otherwise-in musicians needs to be evaluated with caution. Musicianship aside, this study showed for the first time that sung targets presented in babble noise seem to be recalled better than spoken ones. We discuss the present design and the methodological approach of screening for amusia as factors which may partially account for some of the mixed results in the field.

句子重复一直是广泛的心理语言学研究的焦点。音乐训练可以在不利的听觉条件下增强言语感知,这一观点的结果喜忧参半。在这项工作中,我们试图衡量牙牙学语噪音对不同语义内容(解释性、叙事性和反常性)的口语和歌唱短语的即时重复的影响,最初是在100名受过和不受过音乐训练的英语单语人群中。这两组人还完成了一些非音乐认知测试和蒙特利尔Amusia评估组(MBEA)。在忽略MBEA结果的情况下,音乐家在整体重复准确性方面显著优于非音乐家。在有牙牙学语噪音的情况下,唱歌的目标比口语的目标回忆得更好。宋词的说明文比口语的说明文能更好地回忆,而语义异常的内容在噪声中回忆得更差。在排除了13名被诊断为娱乐障碍的参与者后重新运行分析,没有显示出显著的群体差异。这表明,需要谨慎评估音乐家在噪音或其他方面增强语音感知的概念。抛开音乐天赋不谈,这项研究首次表明,在咿呀学语的噪音中出现的歌唱目标似乎比口语目标更容易被回忆起来。我们讨论了目前的设计和筛选娱乐障碍的方法论方法,这些因素可能是该领域一些混合结果的部分原因。
{"title":"Exploring the role of singing, semantics, and amusia screening in speech-in-noise perception in musicians and non-musicians.","authors":"Ariadne Loutrari, Aseel Alqadi, Cunmei Jiang, Fang Liu","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01165-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01165-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sentence repetition has been the focus of extensive psycholinguistic research. The notion that music training can bolster speech perception in adverse auditory conditions has been met with mixed results. In this work, we sought to gauge the effect of babble noise on immediate repetition of spoken and sung phrases of varying semantic content (expository, narrative, and anomalous), initially in 100 English-speaking monolinguals with and without music training. The two cohorts also completed some non-musical cognitive tests and the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA). When disregarding MBEA results, musicians were found to significantly outperform non-musicians in terms of overall repetition accuracy. Sung targets were recalled significantly better than spoken ones across groups in the presence of babble noise. Sung expository targets were recalled better than spoken expository ones, and semantically anomalous content was recalled more poorly in noise. Rerunning the analysis after eliminating thirteen participants who were diagnosed with amusia showed no significant group differences. This suggests that the notion of enhanced speech perception-in noise or otherwise-in musicians needs to be evaluated with caution. Musicianship aside, this study showed for the first time that sung targets presented in babble noise seem to be recalled better than spoken ones. We discuss the present design and the methodological approach of screening for amusia as factors which may partially account for some of the mixed results in the field.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10827916/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond peripersonal boundaries: insights from crossmodal interactions. 超越人际界限:从跨模态互动中获得启示。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01154-0
Gianluca Finotti, Dario Menicagli, Daniele Migliorati, Marcello Costantini, Francesca Ferri

We experience our self as a body located in space. However, how information about self-location is integrated into multisensory processes underlying the representation of the peripersonal space (PPS), is still unclear. Prior studies showed that the presence of visual information related to oneself modulates the multisensory processes underlying PPS. Here, we used the crossmodal congruency effect (CCE) to test whether this top-down modulation depends on the spatial location of the body-related visual information. Participants responded to tactile events on their bodies while trying to ignore a visual distractor presented on the mirror reflection of their body (Self) either in the peripersonal space (Near) or in the extrapersonal space (Far). We found larger CCE when visual events were presented on the mirror reflection in the peripersonal space, as compared to the extrapersonal space. These results suggest that top-down modulation of the multisensory bodily self is only possible within the PPS.

我们体验到的自我是一个位于空间中的身体。然而,关于自我位置的信息是如何整合到作为周身空间(PPS)表征基础的多感官过程中的,目前仍不清楚。之前的研究表明,与自身相关的视觉信息的存在会调节 PPS 的基础多感官过程。在这里,我们利用跨模态一致性效应(CCE)来检验这种自上而下的调节是否取决于与身体相关的视觉信息的空间位置。受试者一边对身体上的触觉事件做出反应,一边试图忽略呈现在其身体镜面反射(自我)上的视觉分心物,该分心物既可以出现在个人周围空间(近处),也可以出现在个人外部空间(远处)。我们发现,当视觉事件出现在个人周围空间的镜面反射上时,与个人外部空间相比,CCE 更大。这些结果表明,对多感官身体自我的自上而下的调节只有在 PPS 中才有可能。
{"title":"Beyond peripersonal boundaries: insights from crossmodal interactions.","authors":"Gianluca Finotti, Dario Menicagli, Daniele Migliorati, Marcello Costantini, Francesca Ferri","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01154-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01154-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We experience our self as a body located in space. However, how information about self-location is integrated into multisensory processes underlying the representation of the peripersonal space (PPS), is still unclear. Prior studies showed that the presence of visual information related to oneself modulates the multisensory processes underlying PPS. Here, we used the crossmodal congruency effect (CCE) to test whether this top-down modulation depends on the spatial location of the body-related visual information. Participants responded to tactile events on their bodies while trying to ignore a visual distractor presented on the mirror reflection of their body (Self) either in the peripersonal space (Near) or in the extrapersonal space (Far). We found larger CCE when visual events were presented on the mirror reflection in the peripersonal space, as compared to the extrapersonal space. These results suggest that top-down modulation of the multisensory bodily self is only possible within the PPS.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10827818/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10129649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in absolute identification as a function of autistic trait levels. 作为自闭症特征水平函数的绝对识别的个体差异。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01166-w
Seyed Mohammad Mahdi Moshirian Farahi, Craig Leth-Steensen

The present study aimed to examine the links between a self-report measure known to be discriminative of autism (the AQ-10) and performance on the classic unidimensional absolute identification judgment task with 10 line lengths. The interest in this task is due to the fact that discriminating absolutely between such items is quite perceptually challenging and also that it is not very amenable to generalization. Importantly, there are two currently available views of perceptual learning in autism that suggest that those higher on the autism spectrum might have an advantage on this task. Results showed, however, that for N = 291 typically developing individuals, higher scores on the AQ-10 (and also on a measure of the degree to which individuals self-report having a more spontaneous, activist-type learning style) tended to relate to lower levels of accuracy on this task in contrast to what was expected. One explanation furthered for this result was that those with higher AQ-10 scores may have had more difficulties maintaining the overall stimulus context in memory. Such work adds greatly to knowledge of the nature of the individual differences that can affect performance on this particular task.

本研究旨在检验已知具有自闭症判别力的自我报告量表(AQ-10)与10条线长度的经典一维绝对识别判断任务的表现之间的联系。对这项任务的兴趣是因为在这类项目之间进行绝对的区分在感知上是非常具有挑战性的,而且它不太容易概括。重要的是,目前有两种关于自闭症感知学习的观点表明,自闭症谱系较高的人可能在这项任务中具有优势。然而,结果表明,对于N = 291个典型的发展中的个体,AQ-10(以及个体自我报告具有更自发、积极型学习风格的程度)的得分越高,与预期相比,这项任务的准确性越低。对这一结果的一种进一步解释是,AQ-10分数较高的人可能更难在记忆中维持整体刺激环境。这样的工作极大地增加了对个体差异性质的了解,这些差异可能会影响这项特定任务的表现。
{"title":"Individual differences in absolute identification as a function of autistic trait levels.","authors":"Seyed Mohammad Mahdi Moshirian Farahi, Craig Leth-Steensen","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01166-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01166-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study aimed to examine the links between a self-report measure known to be discriminative of autism (the AQ-10) and performance on the classic unidimensional absolute identification judgment task with 10 line lengths. The interest in this task is due to the fact that discriminating absolutely between such items is quite perceptually challenging and also that it is not very amenable to generalization. Importantly, there are two currently available views of perceptual learning in autism that suggest that those higher on the autism spectrum might have an advantage on this task. Results showed, however, that for N = 291 typically developing individuals, higher scores on the AQ-10 (and also on a measure of the degree to which individuals self-report having a more spontaneous, activist-type learning style) tended to relate to lower levels of accuracy on this task in contrast to what was expected. One explanation furthered for this result was that those with higher AQ-10 scores may have had more difficulties maintaining the overall stimulus context in memory. Such work adds greatly to knowledge of the nature of the individual differences that can affect performance on this particular task.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71427898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does context recollection depend on the base-rate of contextual features? 语境记忆是否取决于语境特征的基率?
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-11 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01153-1
Marek Nieznański, Michał Obidziński, Daria Ford

Episodic recollection is defined by the re-experiencing of contextual and target details of a past event. The base-rate dependency hypothesis assumes that the retrieval of one contextual feature from an integrated episodic trace cues the retrieval of another associated feature, and that the more often a particular configuration of features occurs, the more effective this mutual cueing will be. Alternatively, the conditional probability of one feature given another feature may be neglected in memory for contextual features since they are not directly bound to one another. Three conjoint recognition experiments investigated whether memory for context is sensitive to the base-rates of features. Participants studied frequent versus infrequent configurations of features and, during the test, they were asked to recognise one of these features with (vs. without) another feature reinstated. The results showed that the context recollection parameter, representing the re-experience of contextual features in the dual-recollection model, was higher for frequent than infrequent feature configurations only when the binding of feature information was made easier and the differences in the base-rates were extreme, otherwise no difference was found. Similarly, base-rates of features influenced response guessing only in the condition with salient differences in base-rates. The Bayes factor analyses showed that the evidence from two of our experiments favoured the base-rate neglect hypothesis over the base-rate dependency hypothesis; the opposite result was obtained in the third experiment, but only when high base-rate disproportion and facilitated feature binding conditions were used.

外显回忆是指对过去事件的情境和目标细节的重新体验。基率依赖假说假定,从整合的外显记忆痕迹中检索一个上下文特征会提示检索另一个相关特征,而且特征的特定配置出现的频率越高,这种相互提示就越有效。另外,在记忆上下文特征时,一个特征与另一个特征的条件概率可能会被忽略,因为它们之间没有直接联系。三项联合识别实验研究了上下文记忆是否对特征的基率敏感。受试者学习了经常出现的特征配置和不经常出现的特征配置,在测试过程中,受试者被要求在恢复(或不恢复)另一个特征的情况下识别其中一个特征。结果表明,只有当特征信息的绑定变得更容易且基率差异达到极致时,代表双重回忆模型中上下文特征再体验的上下文回忆参数才会在频繁特征配置中高于不频繁特征配置,否则就没有差异。同样,只有在基数差异显著的条件下,特征的基数才会影响反应猜测。贝叶斯因素分析表明,我们的两个实验的证据表明,基率忽略假说比基率依赖假说更有优势;第三个实验的结果与之相反,但只有在使用高基率失调和促进特征绑定的条件时才会出现这种情况。
{"title":"Does context recollection depend on the base-rate of contextual features?","authors":"Marek Nieznański, Michał Obidziński, Daria Ford","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01153-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01153-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Episodic recollection is defined by the re-experiencing of contextual and target details of a past event. The base-rate dependency hypothesis assumes that the retrieval of one contextual feature from an integrated episodic trace cues the retrieval of another associated feature, and that the more often a particular configuration of features occurs, the more effective this mutual cueing will be. Alternatively, the conditional probability of one feature given another feature may be neglected in memory for contextual features since they are not directly bound to one another. Three conjoint recognition experiments investigated whether memory for context is sensitive to the base-rates of features. Participants studied frequent versus infrequent configurations of features and, during the test, they were asked to recognise one of these features with (vs. without) another feature reinstated. The results showed that the context recollection parameter, representing the re-experience of contextual features in the dual-recollection model, was higher for frequent than infrequent feature configurations only when the binding of feature information was made easier and the differences in the base-rates were extreme, otherwise no difference was found. Similarly, base-rates of features influenced response guessing only in the condition with salient differences in base-rates. The Bayes factor analyses showed that the evidence from two of our experiments favoured the base-rate neglect hypothesis over the base-rate dependency hypothesis; the opposite result was obtained in the third experiment, but only when high base-rate disproportion and facilitated feature binding conditions were used.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10827963/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10194444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use of default option nudge and individual differences in everyday life decisions. 在日常生活决策中使用默认选项轻推和个人差异。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01161-1
Micaela Maria Zucchelli, Elisa Gambetti, Fiorella Giusberti, Raffaella Nori

People often make inefficient decisions for themselves and the community (e.g. they underuse medical screenings or vaccines and they do not vote) also because of their individual characteristics, such as their level of avoidance or anxiety. In recent years, governments have successfully applied strategies, called "nudges", to help people maximizing their decisions in several fields; however, the role of individual characteristics has been poorly explored. The present study investigated whether one kind of nudge, the default option (automatic enrolment in a specific plan), can modulate the influence of such individual differences, promoting favourable decisions in different field, such as the medical and civic ones. One hundred and eighty-three participants completed the Trait Anxiety Inventory, the General Decision-Making Styles Inventory and scenarios about health and civic decisions. Participants have hypothetically been enrolled by default or not enrolled in specific plans and had to decide whether adhere or not to the plan proposed. Result showed that the default option drives anxious and avoidant individuals, who usually refuse to make a choice due to their overestimation of negative events' occurrence, to undergo medical screenings and vaccine and to vote more. Nudge confirmed its effectiveness in favouring better decisions among people according to their individual differences.

人们经常为自己和社区做出低效的决定(例如,他们没有充分利用医疗筛查或疫苗,也没有投票),这也是因为他们的个人特征,比如他们的回避或焦虑程度。近年来,政府成功地应用了被称为“推动”的策略,帮助人们在几个领域最大限度地做出决策;然而,对个体特征的作用却很少有人探究。本研究调查了一种推动,即默认选项(自动加入特定计划),是否可以调节这种个体差异的影响,促进不同领域的有利决策,如医疗和公民决策。183名参与者完成了特质焦虑量表、一般决策风格量表以及关于健康和公民决策的情景。参与者假设默认加入或未加入特定计划,必须决定是否遵守提议的计划。结果显示,默认选项会驱使焦虑和逃避的人接受医学筛查和疫苗接种,并投票更多。这些人通常因高估负面事件的发生而拒绝做出选择。Nudge证实了它在根据人们的个体差异做出更好决定方面的有效性。
{"title":"Use of default option nudge and individual differences in everyday life decisions.","authors":"Micaela Maria Zucchelli, Elisa Gambetti, Fiorella Giusberti, Raffaella Nori","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01161-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01161-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often make inefficient decisions for themselves and the community (e.g. they underuse medical screenings or vaccines and they do not vote) also because of their individual characteristics, such as their level of avoidance or anxiety. In recent years, governments have successfully applied strategies, called \"nudges\", to help people maximizing their decisions in several fields; however, the role of individual characteristics has been poorly explored. The present study investigated whether one kind of nudge, the default option (automatic enrolment in a specific plan), can modulate the influence of such individual differences, promoting favourable decisions in different field, such as the medical and civic ones. One hundred and eighty-three participants completed the Trait Anxiety Inventory, the General Decision-Making Styles Inventory and scenarios about health and civic decisions. Participants have hypothetically been enrolled by default or not enrolled in specific plans and had to decide whether adhere or not to the plan proposed. Result showed that the default option drives anxious and avoidant individuals, who usually refuse to make a choice due to their overestimation of negative events' occurrence, to undergo medical screenings and vaccine and to vote more. Nudge confirmed its effectiveness in favouring better decisions among people according to their individual differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41120455","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual images of disgusting creatures facilitated attentional orienting and delayed attentional disengagement. 恶心生物的视觉图像促进了注意力定向和延迟注意力脱离。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01162-0
Risako Shirai, Katsumi Watanabe

Numerous studies have suggested that threatening stimuli induce a spatial attention bias; however, only a few studies have investigated spatial attention biases for disgusting stimuli. Moreover, past studies generally reported that the spatial attention bias to disgusting images is not robustly in normal individuals. We hypothesized that this was due to the unfamiliar of the images, so we prepared the creature's images that were clearly categorized as disgusting and examined the effects of disgusting images on spatial attention bias. A disgusting or an emotionally neutral image was paired and presented with an (emotionally neutral) filler image. After a temporal interval, a target appeared at either the position where a disgusting or a neutral image was presented (valid condition) or where a filler image was presented (invalid condition). Participants pressed a key corresponding to the target's position as quickly and accurately as possible. We varied the position-response correspondence among three experiments. The results showed that the RTs in the invalid condition was longer for the disgusting images than for the neutral images when the position of a disgusting image was not naturally associated with the left-right hand position. We interpreted the results in that that disgusting images generally slowed down attentional disengagement process but the manual responses were inhibited for the position where a disgusting image appeared when the locations of keys and targets were congruent. The present results suggest that disgusting images affect not only attentional processes but also manual responses related to the selection and initiation of responses.

大量研究表明,威胁性刺激会引起空间注意力偏差;然而,只有少数研究调查了恶心刺激的空间注意偏差。此外,过去的研究普遍报道,在正常个体中,对恶心图像的空间注意力偏见并不强烈。我们假设这是由于对图像的陌生,所以我们准备了被明确归类为恶心的生物图像,并研究了恶心图像对空间注意力偏见的影响。恶心或情感中立的图像与(情感中立的)填充图像配对并呈现。在时间间隔之后,目标出现在呈现恶心或中性图像的位置(有效条件)或呈现填充图像的位置处(无效条件)。参与者尽可能快速准确地按下与目标位置相对应的键。我们在三个实验中改变了位置-响应的对应关系。结果表明,当恶心图像的位置与左右手位置不自然相关时,恶心图像在无效条件下的RT比中性图像更长。我们对结果的解释是,恶心的图像通常会减缓注意力脱离过程,但当按键和目标的位置一致时,恶心图像出现的位置会抑制手动反应。目前的研究结果表明,恶心的图像不仅影响注意力过程,还影响与反应的选择和启动相关的手动反应。
{"title":"Visual images of disgusting creatures facilitated attentional orienting and delayed attentional disengagement.","authors":"Risako Shirai, Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01162-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01162-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Numerous studies have suggested that threatening stimuli induce a spatial attention bias; however, only a few studies have investigated spatial attention biases for disgusting stimuli. Moreover, past studies generally reported that the spatial attention bias to disgusting images is not robustly in normal individuals. We hypothesized that this was due to the unfamiliar of the images, so we prepared the creature's images that were clearly categorized as disgusting and examined the effects of disgusting images on spatial attention bias. A disgusting or an emotionally neutral image was paired and presented with an (emotionally neutral) filler image. After a temporal interval, a target appeared at either the position where a disgusting or a neutral image was presented (valid condition) or where a filler image was presented (invalid condition). Participants pressed a key corresponding to the target's position as quickly and accurately as possible. We varied the position-response correspondence among three experiments. The results showed that the RTs in the invalid condition was longer for the disgusting images than for the neutral images when the position of a disgusting image was not naturally associated with the left-right hand position. We interpreted the results in that that disgusting images generally slowed down attentional disengagement process but the manual responses were inhibited for the position where a disgusting image appeared when the locations of keys and targets were congruent. The present results suggest that disgusting images affect not only attentional processes but also manual responses related to the selection and initiation of responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41143401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion in university students: the mediation of repetitive negative thinking. 完美主义与大学生自我同情人际敏感性的关系:重复性消极思维的中介作用。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01163-z
Zahra Neshat, Azam Farah Bijari, Gholamreza Dehshiri

Self-compassion is a construct of positive psychology related to personality and cognitive factors. Perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity are prevalent personality traits among university students and are associated with low self-compassion. Further research is required to comprehend how these mechanisms work in creating self-compassion. Consequently, the current study investigated the direct and indirect relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion via repetitive negative thinking. To this end, a sample of 450 students studying in Tehran during the 2022 academic year was selected as the study sample. The results indicated that perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity demonstrate a negative direct relationship with self-compassion, while perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity exhibit an indirect relationship with self-compassion via repetitive negative thinking. Based on the findings, it can be concluded that the relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion is not straightforward and that repetitive negative thinking can mediate this relationship. The results can be used to improve methods for increasing self-compassion and paying attention to personality, and cognitive factors can be an important step toward more effective self-compassion interventions.

自我同情是一种与人格和认知因素相关的积极心理学建构。完美主义和人际敏感是大学生普遍存在的人格特征,并与低自我同情有关。需要进一步的研究来理解这些机制是如何在创造自我同情方面发挥作用的。因此,本研究通过重复的消极思维,调查了完美主义与人际敏感和自我同情之间的直接和间接关系。为此,选取了2022学年在德黑兰学习的450名学生作为研究样本。结果表明,完美主义和人际敏感性与自我同情呈负向直接关系,完美主义和人与人之间的敏感性通过重复性消极思维与自我同情呈现间接关系。基于这些发现,可以得出结论,完美主义与具有自我同情的人际敏感性之间的关系并不直接,重复的消极思维可以调节这种关系。研究结果可用于改进提高自我同情和关注个性的方法,认知因素可以成为更有效的自我同情干预的重要一步。
{"title":"The relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion in university students: the mediation of repetitive negative thinking.","authors":"Zahra Neshat, Azam Farah Bijari, Gholamreza Dehshiri","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01163-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01163-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-compassion is a construct of positive psychology related to personality and cognitive factors. Perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity are prevalent personality traits among university students and are associated with low self-compassion. Further research is required to comprehend how these mechanisms work in creating self-compassion. Consequently, the current study investigated the direct and indirect relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion via repetitive negative thinking. To this end, a sample of 450 students studying in Tehran during the 2022 academic year was selected as the study sample. The results indicated that perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity demonstrate a negative direct relationship with self-compassion, while perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity exhibit an indirect relationship with self-compassion via repetitive negative thinking. Based on the findings, it can be concluded that the relationship between perfectionism and interpersonal sensitivity with self-compassion is not straightforward and that repetitive negative thinking can mediate this relationship. The results can be used to improve methods for increasing self-compassion and paying attention to personality, and cognitive factors can be an important step toward more effective self-compassion interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41172728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential effects of intrinsic properties of natural scenes and interference mechanisms on recognition processes in long-term visual memory. 自然场景的内在特性和干扰机制对长期视觉记忆中识别过程的差异影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01164-y
Anastasiia Mikhailova, Sophie Lightfoot, José Santos-Victor, Moreno I Coco

Humans display remarkable long-term visual memory (LTVM) processes. Even though images may be intrinsically memorable, the fidelity of their visual representations, and consequently the likelihood of successfully retrieving them, hinges on their similarity when concurrently held in LTVM. In this debate, it is still unclear whether intrinsic features of images (perceptual and semantic) may be mediated by mechanisms of interference generated at encoding, or during retrieval, and how these factors impinge on recognition processes. In the current study, participants (32) studied a stream of 120 natural scenes from 8 semantic categories, which varied in frequencies (4, 8, 16 or 32 exemplars per category) to generate different levels of category interference, in preparation for a recognition test. Then they were asked to indicate which of two images, presented side by side (i.e. two-alternative forced-choice), they remembered. The two images belonged to the same semantic category but varied in their perceptual similarity (similar or dissimilar). Participants also expressed their confidence (sure/not sure) about their recognition response, enabling us to tap into their metacognitive efficacy (meta-d'). Additionally, we extracted the activation of perceptual and semantic features in images (i.e. their informational richness) through deep neural network modelling and examined their impact on recognition processes. Corroborating previous literature, we found that category interference and perceptual similarity negatively impact recognition processes, as well as response times and metacognitive efficacy. Moreover, images semantically rich were less likely remembered, an effect that trumped a positive memorability boost coming from perceptual information. Critically, we did not observe any significant interaction between intrinsic features of images and interference generated either at encoding or during retrieval. All in all, our study calls for a more integrative understanding of the representational dynamics during encoding and recognition enabling us to form, maintain and access visual information.

人类表现出显著的长期视觉记忆(LTVM)过程。尽管图像可能本质上是令人难忘的,但它们的视觉表示的保真度,以及成功检索它们的可能性,取决于它们在LTVM中同时保存时的相似性。在这场争论中,尚不清楚图像的内在特征(感知和语义)是否可能由编码时或检索过程中产生的干扰机制介导,以及这些因素如何影响识别过程。在当前的研究中,参与者(32)研究了来自8个语义类别的120个自然场景流,这些场景的频率不同(每个类别4、8、16或32个样本),以产生不同级别的类别干扰,为识别测试做准备。然后,他们被要求指出并排呈现的两个图像中的哪一个(即两个可选的强迫选择),他们记得。这两幅图像属于同一语义范畴,但在感知相似性(相似或不相似)方面存在差异。参与者还表达了他们对自己的识别反应的信心(确定/不确定),使我们能够利用他们的元认知效能(meta-d’)。此外,我们通过深度神经网络建模提取了图像中感知和语义特征的激活(即它们的信息丰富性),并研究了它们对识别过程的影响。通过证实先前的文献,我们发现类别干扰和感知相似性对识别过程、反应时间和元认知效能产生了负面影响。此外,语义丰富的图像不太可能被记住,这一效果胜过了来自感知信息的积极记忆提升。至关重要的是,我们没有观察到图像的内在特征与编码或检索过程中产生的干扰之间有任何显著的相互作用。总之,我们的研究呼吁对编码和识别过程中的表征动力学进行更全面的理解,使我们能够形成、维护和访问视觉信息。
{"title":"Differential effects of intrinsic properties of natural scenes and interference mechanisms on recognition processes in long-term visual memory.","authors":"Anastasiia Mikhailova, Sophie Lightfoot, José Santos-Victor, Moreno I Coco","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01164-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01164-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans display remarkable long-term visual memory (LTVM) processes. Even though images may be intrinsically memorable, the fidelity of their visual representations, and consequently the likelihood of successfully retrieving them, hinges on their similarity when concurrently held in LTVM. In this debate, it is still unclear whether intrinsic features of images (perceptual and semantic) may be mediated by mechanisms of interference generated at encoding, or during retrieval, and how these factors impinge on recognition processes. In the current study, participants (32) studied a stream of 120 natural scenes from 8 semantic categories, which varied in frequencies (4, 8, 16 or 32 exemplars per category) to generate different levels of category interference, in preparation for a recognition test. Then they were asked to indicate which of two images, presented side by side (i.e. two-alternative forced-choice), they remembered. The two images belonged to the same semantic category but varied in their perceptual similarity (similar or dissimilar). Participants also expressed their confidence (sure/not sure) about their recognition response, enabling us to tap into their metacognitive efficacy (meta-d'). Additionally, we extracted the activation of perceptual and semantic features in images (i.e. their informational richness) through deep neural network modelling and examined their impact on recognition processes. Corroborating previous literature, we found that category interference and perceptual similarity negatively impact recognition processes, as well as response times and metacognitive efficacy. Moreover, images semantically rich were less likely remembered, an effect that trumped a positive memorability boost coming from perceptual information. Critically, we did not observe any significant interaction between intrinsic features of images and interference generated either at encoding or during retrieval. All in all, our study calls for a more integrative understanding of the representational dynamics during encoding and recognition enabling us to form, maintain and access visual information.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41216096","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of classical music on learning and memory in rats: a systematic review and meta-analysis. 古典音乐对大鼠学习记忆的影响:系统综述和荟萃分析。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01167-9
Clarissa Trzesniak, Ana C L Biscaro, Amanda V Sardeli, Iara S L Faria, Cesar R Sartori, Luciano Magalhães Vitorino, Rodolfo S Faria

During the learning process, music can activate important neural areas in the brain, promoting the retention of information and memory formation. However, studies testing music effects on memory had found different improvements, which could be due to the methodological differences across studies. Thus, the purpose of this article was to systematically review the literature and meta-analyze the effects of music on Rattus norvegicus' explicit memory (Maze tests) only in controlled investigations. The seven studies included led to a very homogeneous analysis (I2 = 0%), confirming the consistency of the significant standardized mean difference (SMD) between the memory of animals exposed and not exposed to music (SMD 0.60 (95% CI 0.38; 0.83, p < 0.001)). Exploratory analysis suggests music benefits on memory can be acquired when begun at any age, when tested with the three types of mazes evaluated, with exposure lasting from 8 to 83 days and when the age on test day was either under 30 days or over 30 days. To expand the actual understanding of music effects on memory, future studies should investigate different types of music and animal species, with different sex and health conditions, at different time points.

在学习过程中,音乐可以激活大脑中的重要神经区域,促进信息的保留和记忆的形成。然而,测试音乐对记忆影响的研究发现了不同的改善,这可能是由于研究之间的方法差异。因此,本文的目的是系统地回顾文献,并仅在对照研究中荟萃分析音乐对褐家鼠外显记忆的影响(迷宫测试)。包括的七项研究导致了非常均匀的分析(I2 = 0%),证实了暴露于音乐和未暴露于音乐的动物的记忆之间的显著标准化平均差(SMD)的一致性(SMD 0.60(95%CI 0.38;0.83,p
{"title":"The influence of classical music on learning and memory in rats: a systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Clarissa Trzesniak, Ana C L Biscaro, Amanda V Sardeli, Iara S L Faria, Cesar R Sartori, Luciano Magalhães Vitorino, Rodolfo S Faria","doi":"10.1007/s10339-023-01167-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-023-01167-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>During the learning process, music can activate important neural areas in the brain, promoting the retention of information and memory formation. However, studies testing music effects on memory had found different improvements, which could be due to the methodological differences across studies. Thus, the purpose of this article was to systematically review the literature and meta-analyze the effects of music on Rattus norvegicus' explicit memory (Maze tests) only in controlled investigations. The seven studies included led to a very homogeneous analysis (I<sup>2</sup> = 0%), confirming the consistency of the significant standardized mean difference (SMD) between the memory of animals exposed and not exposed to music (SMD 0.60 (95% CI 0.38; 0.83, p < 0.001)). Exploratory analysis suggests music benefits on memory can be acquired when begun at any age, when tested with the three types of mazes evaluated, with exposure lasting from 8 to 83 days and when the age on test day was either under 30 days or over 30 days. To expand the actual understanding of music effects on memory, future studies should investigate different types of music and animal species, with different sex and health conditions, at different time points.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71427899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Processing
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1