首页 > 最新文献

Judgment and Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: A meta-analysis 反思性思维预测较低的阴谋信念:一项荟萃分析
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008913
Büşra Elif Yelbuz, Ecesu Madan, Sinan Alper
One of the many established predictors of conspiracy beliefs is reflective thinking, but no meta-analysis so far has examined this relationship. In the current meta-analysis of published and unpublished correlational data (145 samples, 181 effect sizes), we found a significant negative association between reflective thinking and conspiracy beliefs with a medium-level effect size (r = –.189) . Similar levels of correlations were found across different types of measures (self-report vs. performance-based) and conspiracy beliefs (generic vs. specific). Further, no evidence suggested publication bias in this body of work. Suggestions for future research are discussed.
阴谋信念的众多既定预测因素之一是反思思维,但迄今为止还没有荟萃分析来检验这种关系。在目前对已发表和未发表的相关数据(145个样本、181个效应大小)的荟萃分析中,我们发现,反思思维和阴谋信念之间存在显著的负相关,具有中等水平的效应大小(r=–.189)。不同类型的测量(自我报告与基于表现)和阴谋信念(通用与特定)之间存在相似水平的相关性。此外,没有证据表明这一工作中存在出版偏见。讨论了对未来研究的建议。
{"title":"Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: A meta-analysis","authors":"Büşra Elif Yelbuz, Ecesu Madan, Sinan Alper","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008913","url":null,"abstract":"One of the many established predictors of conspiracy beliefs is reflective thinking, but no meta-analysis so far has examined this relationship. In the current meta-analysis of published and unpublished correlational data (145 samples, 181 effect sizes), we found a significant negative association between reflective thinking and conspiracy beliefs with a medium-level effect size (r = –.189) . Similar levels of correlations were found across different types of measures (self-report vs. performance-based) and conspiracy beliefs (generic vs. specific). Further, no evidence suggested publication bias in this body of work. Suggestions for future research are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42155044","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Debiasing System 1: Training favours logical over stereotypical intuiting 去偏见系统1:训练倾向于逻辑而非刻板的直觉
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008895
Esther Boissin, Serge Caparos, Aikaterini Voudouri, Wim De Neys
Whereas people’s reasoning is often biased by intuitive stereotypical associations, recent debiasing studies suggest that performance can be boosted by short training interventions that stress the underlying problem logic. The nature of this training effect remains unclear. Does training help participants correct erroneous stereotypical intuitions through deliberation? Or does it help them develop correct intuitions? We addressed this issue in four studies with base-rate neglect and conjunction fallacy problems. We used a two-response paradigm in which participants first gave an initial intuitive response, under time pressure and cognitive load, and then gave a final response after deliberation. Studies 1A and 2A showed that training boosted performance and did so as early as the intuitive stage. After training, most participants solved the problems correctly from the outset and no longer needed to correct an initial incorrect answer through deliberation. Studies 1B and 2B indicated that this sound intuiting persisted over at least two months. The findings confirm that a short training can debias reasoning at an intuitive “System 1” stage and get reasoners to favour logical over stereotypical intuitions.
尽管人们的推理往往因直观的刻板印象而有偏见,但最近的去偏见研究表明,强调潜在问题逻辑的短期训练干预可以提高表现。这种训练效果的性质尚不清楚。培训是否有助于参与者通过深思熟虑纠正错误的刻板印象?或者它能帮助他们发展正确的直觉吗?我们在四项关于基本速率忽略和连接谬误问题的研究中讨论了这个问题。我们使用了两个反应的范式,参与者首先在时间压力和认知负荷下给出最初的直觉反应,然后在深思熟虑后给出最终反应。研究1A和2A表明,训练可以提高表现,而且早在直觉阶段就可以提高表现。培训后,大多数参与者从一开始就正确地解决了问题,不再需要通过深思熟虑来纠正最初的错误答案。研究1B和2B表明,这种声音直觉至少持续了两个月。研究结果证实,短期训练可以在直觉的“系统1”阶段削弱推理能力,并使推理者倾向于逻辑直觉而非刻板直觉。
{"title":"Debiasing System 1: Training favours logical over stereotypical intuiting","authors":"Esther Boissin, Serge Caparos, Aikaterini Voudouri, Wim De Neys","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008895","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008895","url":null,"abstract":"Whereas people’s reasoning is often biased by intuitive stereotypical associations, recent debiasing studies suggest that performance can be boosted by short training interventions that stress the underlying problem logic. The nature of this training effect remains unclear. Does training help participants correct erroneous stereotypical intuitions through deliberation? Or does it help them develop correct intuitions? We addressed this issue in four studies with base-rate neglect and conjunction fallacy problems. We used a two-response paradigm in which participants first gave an initial intuitive response, under time pressure and cognitive load, and then gave a final response after deliberation. Studies 1A and 2A showed that training boosted performance and did so as early as the intuitive stage. After training, most participants solved the problems correctly from the outset and no longer needed to correct an initial incorrect answer through deliberation. Studies 1B and 2B indicated that this sound intuiting persisted over at least two months. The findings confirm that a short training can debias reasoning at an intuitive “System 1” stage and get reasoners to favour logical over stereotypical intuitions.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47017979","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Drafting strategies in fantasy football: A study of competitive sequential human decision making 梦幻足球的起草策略:竞争顺序人类决策的研究
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008901
M. Lee, Siqi Liu
Drafting is a competitive task in which a set of decision makers choose from a set of resources sequentially, with each resource becoming unavailable once selected. How people make these choices raises basic questions about human decision making, including people’s sensitivity to the statistical regularities of the resource environment, their ability to reason about the behavior of their competitors, and their ability to execute and adapt sophisticated strategies in dynamic situations involving uncertainty. Sports provides one real-world example of drafting behavior, in which a set of teams draft players from an available pool in a well-regulated way. Fantasy sport competitions provide potentially large data sets of drafting behavior. We study fantasy football drafting behavior from the 2017 National Football League (NFL) season based on 1350 leagues hosted by the http://sleeper.app platform. We find people are sensitive to some important environmental regularities in the order in which they draft players, but also present evidence that they use a more narrow range of strategies than is likely optimal in terms of team composition. We find little to no evidence for the use of the complicated but well-documented strategy known as handcuffing, and no evidence of irrational influence from individual-level biases for different NFL teams. We do, however, identify a set of circumstances for which there is clear evidence that people’s choices are strongly influenced by the immediately preceding choice made by a competitor.
起草是一项竞争任务,其中一组决策者依次从一组资源中进行选择,每个资源一旦选择就不可用。人们如何做出这些选择提出了关于人类决策的基本问题,包括人们对资源环境统计规律的敏感性,他们对竞争对手行为的推理能力,以及他们在涉及不确定性的动态情况下执行和适应复杂策略的能力。体育界提供了选秀行为的一个现实例子,在这个例子中,一组球队以一种规范的方式从可用的球员池中挑选球员。幻想的体育比赛提供了潜在的庞大的起草行为数据集。我们基于http://sleeper.app平台上的1350个联赛,研究了2017年美国国家橄榄球联盟(NFL)赛季的梦幻足球选秀行为。我们发现人们对一些重要的环境规律很敏感,在他们挑选球员的顺序上,但也有证据表明,他们使用的策略范围比团队组成的最佳策略范围更窄。我们几乎没有发现任何证据表明使用了复杂但有充分证据的策略,即手铐,也没有证据表明个人层面的偏见对不同的NFL球队产生了非理性影响。然而,我们确实确定了一组情况,在这些情况下,有明确的证据表明,人们的选择受到竞争对手立即做出的选择的强烈影响。
{"title":"Drafting strategies in fantasy football: A study of competitive sequential human decision making","authors":"M. Lee, Siqi Liu","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008901","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008901","url":null,"abstract":"Drafting is a competitive task in which a set of decision makers choose from a set of resources sequentially, with each resource becoming unavailable once selected. How people make these choices raises basic questions about human decision making, including people’s sensitivity to the statistical regularities of the resource environment, their ability to reason about the behavior of their competitors, and their ability to execute and adapt sophisticated strategies in dynamic situations involving uncertainty. Sports provides one real-world example of drafting behavior, in which a set of teams draft players from an available pool in a well-regulated way. Fantasy sport competitions provide potentially large data sets of drafting behavior. We study fantasy football drafting behavior from the 2017 National Football League (NFL) season based on 1350 leagues hosted by the http://sleeper.app platform. We find people are sensitive to some important environmental regularities in the order in which they draft players, but also present evidence that they use a more narrow range of strategies than is likely optimal in terms of team composition. We find little to no evidence for the use of the complicated but well-documented strategy known as handcuffing, and no evidence of irrational influence from individual-level biases for different NFL teams. We do, however, identify a set of circumstances for which there is clear evidence that people’s choices are strongly influenced by the immediately preceding choice made by a competitor.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44667678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The effects of communicating scientific uncertainty on trust and decision making in a public health context 在公共卫生背景下,传播科学不确定性对信任和决策的影响
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008962
C. Schneider, A. Freeman, D. Spiegelhalter, S. van der Linden
Large-scale societal issues such as public health crises highlight the need to communicate scientific information, which is often uncertain, accurately to the public and policy makers. The challenge is to communicate the inherent scientific uncertainty — especially about the underlying quality of the evidence — whilst supporting informed decision making. Little is known about the effects that such scientific uncertainty has on people’s judgments of the information. In three experimental studies (total N=6,489), we investigate the influence of scientific uncertainty about the quality of the evidence on people’s perceived trustworthiness of the information and decision making. We compare the provision of high, low, and ambiguous quality-of-evidence indicators against providing no such cues. Results show an asymmetric relationship: people react more strongly to cues of low quality of evidence than they do to high quality of evidence compared to no cue. While responses to a cue of high quality of evidence are not significantly different from no cue; a cue of low or uncertain quality of evidence is accompanied by lower perceived trustworthiness and lower use of the information in decision making. Cues of uncertain quality of evidence have a similar effect to those of low quality. These effects do not change with the addition of a reason for the indicated quality level. Our findings shed light on the effects of the communication of scientific uncertainty on judgment and decision making, and provide insights for evidence-based communications and informed decision making for policy makers and the public.
公共卫生危机等大规模社会问题凸显了向公众和决策者准确传达科学信息的必要性,而科学信息往往是不确定的。挑战在于传达固有的科学不确定性——尤其是关于证据的基本质量——同时支持知情决策。人们对这种科学不确定性对人们对信息判断的影响知之甚少。在三项实验研究中(总N=6489),我们调查了证据质量的科学不确定性对人们感知的信息可信度和决策的影响。我们将提供高、低和模糊的证据质量指标与不提供此类线索进行比较。结果显示了一种不对称的关系:与没有线索相比,人们对低质量证据的线索的反应比对高质量证据的反应更强烈。而对高质量证据提示的反应与没有提示没有显著差异;证据质量低或不确定的提示伴随着感知可信度的降低和决策中信息的使用率的降低。证据质量不确定的线索与低质量的线索具有相似的效果。这些影响不会随着所指示的质量水平的原因的增加而改变。我们的研究结果揭示了科学不确定性的传播对判断和决策的影响,并为决策者和公众的循证传播和知情决策提供了见解。
{"title":"The effects of communicating scientific uncertainty on trust and decision making in a public health context","authors":"C. Schneider, A. Freeman, D. Spiegelhalter, S. van der Linden","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008962","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008962","url":null,"abstract":"Large-scale societal issues such as public health crises highlight the need to communicate scientific information, which is often uncertain, accurately to the public and policy makers. The challenge is to communicate the inherent scientific uncertainty — especially about the underlying quality of the evidence — whilst supporting informed decision making. Little is known about the effects that such scientific uncertainty has on people’s judgments of the information. In three experimental studies (total N=6,489), we investigate the influence of scientific uncertainty about the quality of the evidence on people’s perceived trustworthiness of the information and decision making. We compare the provision of high, low, and ambiguous quality-of-evidence indicators against providing no such cues. Results show an asymmetric relationship: people react more strongly to cues of low quality of evidence than they do to high quality of evidence compared to no cue. While responses to a cue of high quality of evidence are not significantly different from no cue; a cue of low or uncertain quality of evidence is accompanied by lower perceived trustworthiness and lower use of the information in decision making. Cues of uncertain quality of evidence have a similar effect to those of low quality. These effects do not change with the addition of a reason for the indicated quality level. Our findings shed light on the effects of the communication of scientific uncertainty on judgment and decision making, and provide insights for evidence-based communications and informed decision making for policy makers and the public.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44557945","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Preferences after pan(dem)ics: Time and risk in the shadow of COVID-19 pan(dem)ics之后的偏好:新冠肺炎阴影下的时间和风险
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008925
Xavier Gassmann, Antoine Malézieux, E. Spiegelman, Jean-Christian Tisserand
This paper uses the COVID-19 health crisis to study how individual preferences respond to generalized traumatic events. We review previous literature on natural and man-made disasters. Using incentive-compatible tasks, we simultaneously estimate risk and ambiguity aversion, time discounting, present bias, and prudence parameters before, during, and after the COVID-19 lockdown in France. We find patience, risk aversion, and ambiguity aversion fell during lockdown, then gradually returned toward their initial levels 4 months later. These results have implications for health and economic policies, and deepen our understanding of the responses – and resilience – of economic preferences to traumatic events.
本文利用COVID-19健康危机来研究个体偏好如何对广泛性创伤事件做出反应。我们回顾了以前关于自然灾害和人为灾害的文献。使用激励相容任务,我们同时估计了在法国COVID-19封锁之前、期间和之后的风险和模糊性规避、时间贴现、当前偏差和审慎参数。我们发现,耐心、风险厌恶和模糊厌恶在封锁期间有所下降,然后在4个月后逐渐恢复到初始水平。这些结果对健康和经济政策具有启示意义,并加深了我们对经济偏好对创伤性事件的反应和恢复力的理解。
{"title":"Preferences after pan(dem)ics: Time and risk in the shadow of COVID-19","authors":"Xavier Gassmann, Antoine Malézieux, E. Spiegelman, Jean-Christian Tisserand","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008925","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008925","url":null,"abstract":"This paper uses the COVID-19 health crisis to study how individual preferences respond to generalized traumatic events. We review previous literature on natural and man-made disasters. Using incentive-compatible tasks, we simultaneously estimate risk and ambiguity aversion, time discounting, present bias, and prudence parameters before, during, and after the COVID-19 lockdown in France. We find patience, risk aversion, and ambiguity aversion fell during lockdown, then gradually returned toward their initial levels 4 months later. These results have implications for health and economic policies, and deepen our understanding of the responses – and resilience – of economic preferences to traumatic events.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43643914","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
The advice less taken: The consequences of receiving unexpected advice 建议较少采纳:收到意外建议的后果
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008950
T. Rebholz, Mandy Hütter
Although new information technologies and social networks make a wide variety of opinions and advice easily accessible, one can never be sure to get support on a focal judgment task. Nevertheless, participants in traditional advice taking studies are by default informed in advance about the opportunity to revise their judgment in the light of advice. The expectation of advice, however, may affect the weight assigned to it. The present research therefore investigates whether the advice taking process depends on the expectation of advice in the judge-advisor system (JAS). Five preregistered experiments (total N = 2019) compared low and high levels of advice expectation. While there was no evidence for expectation effects in three experiments with block-wise structure, we obtained support for a positive influence of advice expectation on advice weighting in two experiments implementing sequential advice taking. The paradigmatic disclosure of the full procedure to participants thus constitutes an important boundary condition for the ecological study of advice taking behavior. The results suggest that the conventional JAS procedure fails to capture a class of judgment processes where advice is unexpected and therefore weighted less.
尽管新的信息技术和社交网络使人们很容易获得各种各样的意见和建议,但人们永远无法确保在一项重点判断任务上得到支持。然而,传统咨询研究的参与者默认会提前被告知有机会根据建议修改自己的判断。然而,对建议的期望可能会影响分配给它的权重。因此,本研究调查了法官顾问系统(JAS)中的建议采纳过程是否取决于对建议的预期。五个预先注册的实验(总数N=2019)比较了建议期望的低水平和高水平。虽然在三个具有分块结构的实验中没有证据表明期望效应,但在两个实施顺序建议采纳的实验中,我们获得了对建议期望对建议权重的积极影响的支持。因此,向参与者示范性地披露完整的程序构成了建议采纳行为生态学研究的重要边界条件。结果表明,传统的JAS程序无法捕捉到一类建议出乎意料的判断过程,因此权重较小。
{"title":"The advice less taken: The consequences of receiving unexpected advice","authors":"T. Rebholz, Mandy Hütter","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008950","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008950","url":null,"abstract":"Although new information technologies and social networks make a wide variety of opinions and advice easily accessible, one can never be sure to get support on a focal judgment task. Nevertheless, participants in traditional advice taking studies are by default informed in advance about the opportunity to revise their judgment in the light of advice. The expectation of advice, however, may affect the weight assigned to it. The present research therefore investigates whether the advice taking process depends on the expectation of advice in the judge-advisor system (JAS). Five preregistered experiments (total N = 2019) compared low and high levels of advice expectation. While there was no evidence for expectation effects in three experiments with block-wise structure, we obtained support for a positive influence of advice expectation on advice weighting in two experiments implementing sequential advice taking. The paradigmatic disclosure of the full procedure to participants thus constitutes an important boundary condition for the ecological study of advice taking behavior. The results suggest that the conventional JAS procedure fails to capture a class of judgment processes where advice is unexpected and therefore weighted less.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48530660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing to choose or not 选择选择或不选择
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500008937
Roy Shoval, N. Karsh, Baruch Eitam
To what degree do people prefer to choose for themselves and what drives this preference? Is it memory-based and results from a life-long association between choices and better outcomes, or is the process of choice itself reinforcing? In a new paradigm, across 6 experiments, participants experienced both ’Own Choice’ and ’Computer Picks’ conditions with identical outcomes before selecting which condition to re-experience in the final part of the experiment. Consistent with previous work, an overwhelming majority ( 83%) preferred own-choice. Several variations of the paradigm reveal that (1) Preference For Choice (PFC) is reduced when thinking about the task without actually choosing in it, (2) PFC is substantially reduced by choice-unrelated cognitive load, and (3) Preference For Choice is further diminished when selection is based on criteria other than one’s preferences. Across experiments, participants’ self-rated enjoyment predicted a significant portion of their PFC, while their perceived gains had little to no predictive value. If PFC stems solely from past reinforcement learning (i.e., memory) then neither performing another few scores of choices nor adding cognitive load to that sequence of choices would be expected to dramatically affect it. Hence, our findings suggest that a significant part of this preference stems from the process of choice itself, and that the experience it confers can itself be reinforcing. We discuss the implications of the proposed mechanism for PFC, which leads us to the prediction that PFC may be muted or even reversed under specific conditions and what this means for when the ‘opposite’ effect – sticking with the default – will occur.
人们更喜欢在多大程度上为自己选择,是什么驱动了这种偏好?它是基于记忆的,是选择和更好的结果之间的终身联系的结果,还是选择的过程本身在强化?在一个新的范式中,在6个实验中,参与者在选择在实验的最后部分重新体验哪种条件之前,经历了“自己选择”和“计算机选择”两种结果相同的条件。与之前的工作一致,绝大多数人(83%)更喜欢自己的选择。该范式的几种变体表明,(1)当思考任务而没有实际选择时,选择偏好(PFC)会降低,(2)与选择无关的认知负荷会显著降低PFC,(3)当选择基于个人偏好以外的标准时,选择的偏好会进一步降低。在整个实验中,参与者的自我评价快乐预测了他们PFC的很大一部分,而他们感知的收益几乎没有预测价值。如果PFC完全源于过去的强化学习(即记忆),那么无论是再做几次选择,还是在选择序列中增加认知负荷,都不会对其产生显著影响。因此,我们的研究结果表明,这种偏好的很大一部分源于选择过程本身,它所赋予的体验本身也可能是强化的。我们讨论了所提出的PFC机制的含义,这使我们预测,在特定条件下,PFC可能会被抑制甚至逆转,以及当“相反”效应(坚持默认)发生时,这意味着什么。
{"title":"Choosing to choose or not","authors":"Roy Shoval, N. Karsh, Baruch Eitam","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500008937","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500008937","url":null,"abstract":"To what degree do people prefer to choose for themselves and what drives this preference? Is it memory-based and results from a life-long association between choices and better outcomes, or is the process of choice itself reinforcing? In a new paradigm, across 6 experiments, participants experienced both ’Own Choice’ and ’Computer Picks’ conditions with identical outcomes before selecting which condition to re-experience in the final part of the experiment. Consistent with previous work, an overwhelming majority ( 83%) preferred own-choice. Several variations of the paradigm reveal that (1) Preference For Choice (PFC) is reduced when thinking about the task without actually choosing in it, (2) PFC is substantially reduced by choice-unrelated cognitive load, and (3) Preference For Choice is further diminished when selection is based on criteria other than one’s preferences. Across experiments, participants’ self-rated enjoyment predicted a significant portion of their PFC, while their perceived gains had little to no predictive value. If PFC stems solely from past reinforcement learning (i.e., memory) then neither performing another few scores of choices nor adding cognitive load to that sequence of choices would be expected to dramatically affect it. Hence, our findings suggest that a significant part of this preference stems from the process of choice itself, and that the experience it confers can itself be reinforcing. We discuss the implications of the proposed mechanism for PFC, which leads us to the prediction that PFC may be muted or even reversed under specific conditions and what this means for when the ‘opposite’ effect – sticking with the default – will occur.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46120661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The day after the disaster: Risk-taking following large- and small-scale disasters in a microworld 灾难发生后的第二天:微观世界中发生大规模和小规模灾难后的风险承担
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500003569
Garston Liang, T. Rakow, E. Yechiam, B. Newell
Using data from seven microworld experiments (N = 841), we investigated how participants reacted to simulated disasters with different risk profiles in a microworld. Our central focus was to investigate how the scale of a disaster affected the choices and response times of these reactions. We find that one-off large-scale disasters prompted stronger reactions to move away from the affected region than recurrent small-scale adverse events, despite the overall risk of a disaster remaining constant across both types of events. A subset of participants are persistent risk-takers who repeatedly put themselves in harm’s way, despite having all the experience and information required to avoid a disaster. Furthermore, while near-misses prompted a small degree of precautionary movement to reduce one’s subsequent risk exposure, directly experiencing the costs of the disaster substantially increased the desire to move away from the affected region. Together, the results point to ways in which laboratory risk-taking tasks can be used to inform the kinds of communication and interventions that seek to mitigate people’s exposure to risk.
使用来自七个微观世界实验(N=841)的数据,我们调查了参与者在微观世界中对具有不同风险特征的模拟灾难的反应。我们的重点是调查灾难的规模如何影响这些反应的选择和响应时间。我们发现,与反复发生的小规模不良事件相比,一次性的大规模灾难促使人们做出更强烈的反应,远离受影响的地区,尽管这两类事件发生灾难的总体风险保持不变。一部分参与者是坚持不懈的风险承担者,他们一再将自己置于危险之中,尽管他们拥有避免灾难所需的所有经验和信息。此外,虽然未遂事件促使人们采取了一定程度的预防行动,以减少随后的风险暴露,但直接经历灾难的代价大大增加了离开受灾地区的愿望。总之,研究结果指出了实验室冒险任务可以用来为寻求减轻人们风险暴露的沟通和干预提供信息的方式。
{"title":"The day after the disaster: Risk-taking following large- and small-scale disasters in a microworld","authors":"Garston Liang, T. Rakow, E. Yechiam, B. Newell","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500003569","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500003569","url":null,"abstract":"Using data from seven microworld experiments (N = 841), we investigated how participants reacted to simulated disasters with different risk profiles in a microworld. Our central focus was to investigate how the scale of a disaster affected the choices and response times of these reactions. We find that one-off large-scale disasters prompted stronger reactions to move away from the affected region than recurrent small-scale adverse events, despite the overall risk of a disaster remaining constant across both types of events. A subset of participants are persistent risk-takers who repeatedly put themselves in harm’s way, despite having all the experience and information required to avoid a disaster. Furthermore, while near-misses prompted a small degree of precautionary movement to reduce one’s subsequent risk exposure, directly experiencing the costs of the disaster substantially increased the desire to move away from the affected region. Together, the results point to ways in which laboratory risk-taking tasks can be used to inform the kinds of communication and interventions that seek to mitigate people’s exposure to risk.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49414659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Expectations of how machines use individuating information and base-rates 对机器如何使用个性化信息和基本费率的期望
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500003600
Sarah D. English, S. Denison, Ori Friedman
Machines are increasingly used to make decisions. We investigated people’s beliefs about how they do so. In six experiments, participants (total N = 2664) predicted how computer and human judges would decide legal cases on the basis of limited evidence — either individuating information from witness testimony or base-rate information. In Experiments 1 to 4, participants predicted that computer judges would be more likely than human ones to reach a guilty verdict, regardless of which kind of evidence was available. Besides asking about punishment, Experiment 5 also included conditions where the judge had to decide whether to reward suspected helpful behavior. Participants again predicted that computer judges would be more likely than human judges to decide based on the available evidence, but also predicted that computer judges would be relatively more punitive than human ones. Also, whereas participants predicted the human judge would give more weight to individuating than base-rate evidence, they expected the computer judge to be insensitive to the distinction between these kinds of evidence. Finally, Experiment 6 replicated the finding that people expect greater sensitivity to the distinction between individuating and base-rate information from humans than computers, but found that the use of cartoon images, as in the first four studies, prevented this effect. Overall, the findings suggest people expect machines to differ from humans in how they weigh different kinds of information when deciding.
机器越来越多地被用来做决定。我们调查了人们对自己是如何做到这一点的看法。在六个实验中,参与者(总数N=2664)预测了计算机和人类法官将如何在有限证据的基础上决定法律案件——要么是证人证词中的个性化信息,要么是基本费率信息。在实验1至4中,参与者预测,无论有哪种证据,计算机法官都比人类法官更有可能做出有罪判决。除了询问惩罚,实验5还包括法官必须决定是否奖励涉嫌帮助行为的条件。参与者再次预测,计算机法官将比人类法官更有可能根据现有证据做出决定,但也预测计算机法官将相对比人类法官更具惩罚性。此外,尽管参与者预测人类法官会比基本证据更重视个性化,但他们预计计算机法官对这类证据之间的区别不敏感。最后,实验6复制了这样一个发现,即人们期望比计算机更敏感地区分来自人类的个性化信息和基本速率信息,但发现与前四项研究一样,卡通图像的使用阻止了这种影响。总的来说,研究结果表明,人们期望机器在做出决定时如何权衡不同种类的信息,这与人类不同。
{"title":"Expectations of how machines use individuating information and base-rates","authors":"Sarah D. English, S. Denison, Ori Friedman","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500003600","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500003600","url":null,"abstract":"Machines are increasingly used to make decisions. We investigated people’s beliefs about how they do so. In six experiments, participants (total N = 2664) predicted how computer and human judges would decide legal cases on the basis of limited evidence — either individuating information from witness testimony or base-rate information. In Experiments 1 to 4, participants predicted that computer judges would be more likely than human ones to reach a guilty verdict, regardless of which kind of evidence was available. Besides asking about punishment, Experiment 5 also included conditions where the judge had to decide whether to reward suspected helpful behavior. Participants again predicted that computer judges would be more likely than human judges to decide based on the available evidence, but also predicted that computer judges would be relatively more punitive than human ones. Also, whereas participants predicted the human judge would give more weight to individuating than base-rate evidence, they expected the computer judge to be insensitive to the distinction between these kinds of evidence. Finally, Experiment 6 replicated the finding that people expect greater sensitivity to the distinction between individuating and base-rate information from humans than computers, but found that the use of cartoon images, as in the first four studies, prevented this effect. Overall, the findings suggest people expect machines to differ from humans in how they weigh different kinds of information when deciding.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41543790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Explaining human sampling rates across different decision domains 解释不同决策领域的人类采样率
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500003557
Didrika S. van de Wouw, Ryan T. McKay, B. Averbeck, N. Furl
Undersampling biases are common in the optimal stopping literature, especially for economic full choice problems. Among these kinds of number-based studies, the moments of the distribution of values that generates the options (i.e., the generating distribution) seem to influence participants’ sampling rate. However, a recent study reported an oversampling bias on a different kind of optimal stopping task: where participants chose potential romantic partners from images of faces (Furl et al., 2019). The authors hypothesised that this oversampling bias might be specific to mate choice. We preregistered this hypothesis and so, here, we test whether sampling rates across different image-based decision-making domains a) reflect different over- or undersampling biases, or b) depend on the moments of the generating distributions (as shown for economic number-based tasks). In two studies (N = 208 and N = 96), we found evidence against the preregistered hypothesis. Participants oversampled to the same degree across domains (compared to a Bayesian ideal observer model), while their sampling rates depended on the generating distribution mean and skewness in a similar way as number-based paradigms. Moreover, optimality model sampling to some extent depended on the the skewness of the generating distribution in a similar way to participants. We conclude that oversampling is not instigated by the mate choice domain and that sampling rate in image-based paradigms, like number-based paradigms, depends on the generating distribution.
欠采样偏差在最优停止文献中很常见,尤其是在经济完全选择问题中。在这些基于数字的研究中,产生选项的值的分布矩(即生成分布)似乎会影响参与者的采样率。然而,最近的一项研究报告称,在另一种最佳停止任务上存在过采样偏差:参与者从人脸图像中选择潜在的浪漫伴侣(Furl等人,2019)。作者假设这种过度采样的偏差可能是择偶特有的。我们预先记录了这一假设,因此,在这里,我们测试了不同基于图像的决策领域的采样率是否a)反映了不同的过采样或欠采样偏差,或者b)取决于生成分布的时刻(如基于经济数字的任务所示)。在两项研究(N=208和N=96)中,我们发现了反对预先登记假说的证据。参与者在不同领域的过采样程度相同(与贝叶斯理想观测器模型相比),而他们的采样率取决于生成的分布均值和偏度,这与基于数字的范式类似。此外,最优性模型的抽样在一定程度上取决于生成分布的偏斜度,这与参和者的方式类似。我们得出结论,过采样不是由择偶域引起的,并且基于图像的范式(如基于数字的范式)中的采样率取决于生成分布。
{"title":"Explaining human sampling rates across different decision domains","authors":"Didrika S. van de Wouw, Ryan T. McKay, B. Averbeck, N. Furl","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500003557","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500003557","url":null,"abstract":"Undersampling biases are common in the optimal stopping literature, especially for economic full choice problems. Among these kinds of number-based studies, the moments of the distribution of values that generates the options (i.e., the generating distribution) seem to influence participants’ sampling rate. However, a recent study reported an oversampling bias on a different kind of optimal stopping task: where participants chose potential romantic partners from images of faces (Furl et al., 2019). The authors hypothesised that this oversampling bias might be specific to mate choice. We preregistered this hypothesis and so, here, we test whether sampling rates across different image-based decision-making domains a) reflect different over- or undersampling biases, or b) depend on the moments of the generating distributions (as shown for economic number-based tasks). In two studies (N = 208 and N = 96), we found evidence against the preregistered hypothesis. Participants oversampled to the same degree across domains (compared to a Bayesian ideal observer model), while their sampling rates depended on the generating distribution mean and skewness in a similar way as number-based paradigms. Moreover, optimality model sampling to some extent depended on the the skewness of the generating distribution in a similar way to participants. We conclude that oversampling is not instigated by the mate choice domain and that sampling rate in image-based paradigms, like number-based paradigms, depends on the generating distribution.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48161926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Judgment and Decision Making
全部 Geobiology Appl. Clay Sci. Geochim. Cosmochim. Acta J. Hydrol. Org. Geochem. Carbon Balance Manage. Contrib. Mineral. Petrol. Int. J. Biometeorol. IZV-PHYS SOLID EART+ J. Atmos. Chem. Acta Oceanolog. Sin. Acta Geophys. ACTA GEOL POL ACTA PETROL SIN ACTA GEOL SIN-ENGL AAPG Bull. Acta Geochimica Adv. Atmos. Sci. Adv. Meteorol. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Am. J. Sci. Am. Mineral. Annu. Rev. Earth Planet. Sci. Appl. Geochem. Aquat. Geochem. Ann. Glaciol. Archaeol. Anthropol. Sci. ARCHAEOMETRY ARCT ANTARCT ALP RES Asia-Pac. J. Atmos. Sci. ATMOSPHERE-BASEL Atmos. Res. Aust. J. Earth Sci. Atmos. Chem. Phys. Atmos. Meas. Tech. Basin Res. Big Earth Data BIOGEOSCIENCES Geostand. Geoanal. Res. GEOLOGY Geosci. J. Geochem. J. Geochem. Trans. Geosci. Front. Geol. Ore Deposits Global Biogeochem. Cycles Gondwana Res. Geochem. Int. Geol. J. Geophys. Prospect. Geosci. Model Dev. GEOL BELG GROUNDWATER Hydrogeol. J. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. Hydrol. Processes Int. J. Climatol. Int. J. Earth Sci. Int. Geol. Rev. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. Int. J. Geomech. Int. J. Geog. Inf. Sci. Isl. Arc J. Afr. Earth. Sci. J. Adv. Model. Earth Syst. J APPL METEOROL CLIM J. Atmos. Oceanic Technol. J. Atmos. Sol. Terr. Phys. J. Clim. J. Earth Sci. J. Earth Syst. Sci. J. Environ. Eng. Geophys. J. Geog. Sci. Mineral. Mag. Miner. Deposita Mon. Weather Rev. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. Nat. Clim. Change Nat. Geosci. Ocean Dyn. Ocean and Coastal Research npj Clim. Atmos. Sci. Ocean Modell. Ocean Sci. Ore Geol. Rev. OCEAN SCI J Paleontol. J. PALAEOGEOGR PALAEOCL PERIOD MINERAL PETROLOGY+ Phys. Chem. Miner. Polar Sci. Prog. Oceanogr. Quat. Sci. Rev. Q. J. Eng. Geol. Hydrogeol. RADIOCARBON Pure Appl. Geophys. Resour. Geol. Rev. Geophys. Sediment. Geol.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1