首页 > 最新文献

Judgment and Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Hypothesized drivers of the bias blind spot—cognitive sophistication, introspection bias, and conversational processes 偏见盲点的假设驱动因素——认知复杂性、内省偏见和对话过程
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009475
D. Mandel, Robert N. Collins, A. C. Walker, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, Evan F. Risko
Individuals often assess themselves as being less susceptible to common biases compared to others. This bias blind spot (BBS) is thought to represent a metacognitive error. In this research, we tested three explanations for the effect: The cognitive sophistication hypothesis posits that individuals who display the BBS more strongly are actually less biased than others. The introspection bias hypothesis posits that the BBS occurs because people rely on introspection more when assessing themselves compared to others. The conversational processes hypothesis posits that the effect is largely a consequence of the pragmatic aspects of the experimental situation rather than true metacognitive error. In two experiments (N = 1057) examining 18 social/motivational and cognitive biases, there was strong evidence of the BBS. Among the three hypotheses examined, the conversational processes hypothesis attracted the greatest support, thus raising questions about the extent to which the BBS is a metacognitive effect.
与其他人相比,个人通常认为自己不太容易受到常见偏见的影响。这种偏见盲点(BBS)被认为是一种元认知错误。在这项研究中,我们测试了对这种影响的三种解释:认知成熟度假说认为,在BBS上表现得更强烈的人实际上比其他人更有偏见。内省偏见假说认为,BBS的发生是因为与他人相比,人们在评估自己时更依赖内省。会话过程假说认为,这种影响在很大程度上是实验情境中语用方面的结果,而不是真正的元认知错误。在两个实验(N=1057)中,研究了18种社会/动机和认知偏见,有强有力的证据表明BBS。在所考察的三个假设中,会话过程假设获得了最大的支持,从而引发了人们对BBS在多大程度上是元认知效应的质疑。
{"title":"Hypothesized drivers of the bias blind spot—cognitive sophistication,\u0000 introspection bias, and conversational processes","authors":"D. Mandel, Robert N. Collins, A. C. Walker, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, Evan F. Risko","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009475","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009475","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Individuals often assess themselves as being less susceptible to common\u0000 biases compared to others. This bias blind spot\u0000 (BBS) is thought to represent a metacognitive error. In this research, we\u0000 tested three explanations for the effect: The cognitive sophistication\u0000 hypothesis posits that individuals who display the BBS more strongly are\u0000 actually less biased than others. The introspection bias hypothesis posits\u0000 that the BBS occurs because people rely on introspection more when assessing\u0000 themselves compared to others. The conversational processes hypothesis\u0000 posits that the effect is largely a consequence of the pragmatic aspects of\u0000 the experimental situation rather than true metacognitive error. In two\u0000 experiments (N = 1057) examining 18\u0000 social/motivational and cognitive biases, there was strong evidence of the\u0000 BBS. Among the three hypotheses examined, the conversational processes\u0000 hypothesis attracted the greatest support, thus raising questions about the\u0000 extent to which the BBS is a metacognitive effect.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42902615","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Do people believe that you can have too much money? The relationship between hypothetical lottery wins and expected happiness 人们认为你可以有太多的钱吗?假设彩票中奖与预期幸福之间的关系
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009402
T. Haesevoets, Kim Dierckx, A. Van Hiel
Do people think that there is such a thing as too much money? The present research investigated this question in the context of hypothetical lottery wins. By employing a mental simulation approach, we were able to examine how people respond to increasing envisioned jackpot amounts, and whether there are individual differences in people’s reactions. Across five empirical studies (total N = 1,504), we consistently found that, overall, the relationship between imagined lottery wins and expected happiness is characterized by an inverted U-shaped curve, with expected happiness being highest around an envisioned win of roughly 10 million pounds. Both lower and higher envisioned wins reduced participants’ overall expected happiness. In addition to this overall pattern, we identified three clusters of participants who react differently to expected increases in wealth. These clusters mainly differed in terms of how soon the top of the expected happiness curve was reached, and if and when the curve started to drop. Finally, we also found some interesting cluster differences in terms of participants’ prosocial and proself motivations.
人们会认为有太多的钱吗?本研究在假设彩票中奖的背景下调查了这个问题。通过采用心理模拟方法,我们能够研究人们对预期中头奖金额增加的反应,以及人们的反应是否存在个体差异。在五项实证研究中(总N = 1504),我们一致发现,总体而言,想象中彩票中奖和预期幸福之间的关系呈现倒u型曲线,预期幸福在大约1000万英镑的预期中奖时达到最高。预期胜利的高低都会降低参与者的总体预期幸福度。除了这种总体模式之外,我们还确定了三组参与者,他们对预期财富增长的反应不同。这些群体的主要差异在于预期幸福曲线到达顶点的时间,以及曲线是否以及何时开始下降。最后,我们还发现在参与者的亲社会动机和亲自我动机方面存在一些有趣的集群差异。
{"title":"Do people believe that you can have too much money? The relationship\u0000 between hypothetical lottery wins and expected happiness","authors":"T. Haesevoets, Kim Dierckx, A. Van Hiel","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009402","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009402","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Do people think that there is such a thing as too much money? The\u0000 present research investigated this question in the context of hypothetical\u0000 lottery wins. By employing a mental simulation approach, we were able to\u0000 examine how people respond to increasing envisioned jackpot amounts, and\u0000 whether there are individual differences in people’s reactions. Across five\u0000 empirical studies (total N = 1,504), we consistently found that, overall,\u0000 the relationship between imagined lottery wins and expected happiness is\u0000 characterized by an inverted U-shaped curve, with expected happiness being\u0000 highest around an envisioned win of roughly 10 million pounds. Both lower\u0000 and higher envisioned wins reduced participants’ overall expected happiness.\u0000 In addition to this overall pattern, we identified three clusters of\u0000 participants who react differently to expected increases in wealth. These\u0000 clusters mainly differed in terms of how soon the top of the expected\u0000 happiness curve was reached, and if and when the curve started to drop.\u0000 Finally, we also found some interesting cluster differences in terms of\u0000 participants’ prosocial and proself motivations.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47131781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Waiting is painful: The impact of anticipated dread on negative discounting in the loss domain 等待是痛苦的:预期恐惧对损失领域负折扣的影响
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009451
Hong-Yue Sun, Jia-Tao Ma, Lei Zhou, Cheng-Ming Jiang, Shu Li
According to the positive time-discounting assumption of intertemporal decision-making, people prefer to undergo negative events in the future rather than in the present. However, negative discounting has been identified in the intertemporal choice and loss domains, which refers to people’s preference to experience negative events earlier rather than later. Studies have validated and supported the "anticipated dread" as an explanation for negative discounting. This study again explored the effect of anticipated dread on intertemporal choice using content analysis; that is, having participants identify anticipated dread among reasons for negative discounting. This study also validated the effect of anticipated dread on negative discounting by manipulating anticipated dread. This study adds empirical and direct evidence for the role of anticipated dread in negative discounting.
根据跨期决策的正时间折现假设,人们更喜欢在未来而不是现在经历负面事件。然而,在跨期选择和损失领域中已经发现了负面折扣,这是指人们倾向于更早而不是更晚地经历负面事件。研究证实并支持“预期恐惧”作为负面折扣的解释。本研究采用内容分析的方法,再次探讨了预期恐惧对跨期选择的影响;也就是说,让参与者在负折扣的原因中识别预期的恐惧。这项研究还通过操纵预期恐惧来验证预期恐惧对负面折扣的影响。这项研究为预期恐惧在负面折扣中的作用提供了经验和直接的证据。
{"title":"Waiting is painful: The impact of anticipated dread on negative\u0000 discounting in the loss domain","authors":"Hong-Yue Sun, Jia-Tao Ma, Lei Zhou, Cheng-Ming Jiang, Shu Li","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009451","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009451","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 According to the positive time-discounting assumption of intertemporal\u0000 decision-making, people prefer to undergo negative events in the future\u0000 rather than in the present. However, negative discounting has been\u0000 identified in the intertemporal choice and loss domains, which refers to\u0000 people’s preference to experience negative events earlier rather than later.\u0000 Studies have validated and supported the \"anticipated dread\" as an\u0000 explanation for negative discounting. This study again explored the effect\u0000 of anticipated dread on intertemporal choice using content analysis; that\u0000 is, having participants identify anticipated dread among reasons for\u0000 negative discounting. This study also validated the effect of anticipated\u0000 dread on negative discounting by manipulating anticipated dread. This study\u0000 adds empirical and direct evidence for the role of anticipated dread in\u0000 negative discounting.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44931942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Pretrial release judgments and decision fatigue 审前释放判决与判决疲劳
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009384
Ravi Shroff, Konstantinos Vamvourellis
Field studies in many domains have found evidence of decision fatigue, a phenomenon describing how decision quality can be impaired by the act of making previous decisions. Debate remains, however, over posited psychological mechanisms underlying decision fatigue, and the size of effects in high-stakes settings. We examine an extensive set of pretrial arraignments in a large, urban court system to investigate how judicial release and bail decisions are influenced by the time an arraignment occurs. We find that release rates decline modestly in the hours before lunch and before dinner, and these declines persist after statistically adjusting for an extensive set of observed covariates. However, we find no evidence that arraignment time affects pretrial release rates in the remainder of each decision-making session. Moreover, we find that release rates remain unchanged after a meal break even though judges have the opportunity to replenish their mental and physical resources by resting and eating. In a complementary analysis, we find that the rate at which judges concur with prosecutorial bail requests does not appear to be influenced by either arraignment time or a meal break. Taken together, our results imply that to the extent that decision fatigue plays a role in pretrial release judgments, effects are small and inconsistent with previous explanations implicating psychological depletion processes.
许多领域的实地研究都发现了决策疲劳的证据,这是一种描述决策质量如何因先前做出决策的行为而受损的现象。然而,关于决策疲劳背后的假定心理机制,以及在高风险环境下影响的大小,仍然存在争议。我们研究了一个大型城市法院系统中广泛的审前传讯,以调查司法释放和保释决定如何受到传讯发生时间的影响。我们发现,在午餐前和晚餐前的几个小时内,释放率略有下降,并且在对观察到的大量协变量进行统计调整后,这些下降仍然存在。然而,我们没有发现传讯时间影响每次决策会议剩余时间的审前释放率的证据。此外,我们发现,即使法官有机会通过休息和进食来补充他们的精神和身体资源,进餐休息后释放率仍然保持不变。在补充分析中,我们发现法官同意检察官保释请求的比率似乎不受传讯时间或用餐时间的影响。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,决策疲劳在审前释放判断中发挥作用的程度很小,与先前暗示心理耗竭过程的解释不一致。
{"title":"Pretrial release judgments and decision fatigue","authors":"Ravi Shroff, Konstantinos Vamvourellis","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009384","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009384","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Field studies in many domains have found evidence of decision fatigue, a\u0000 phenomenon describing how decision quality can be impaired by the act of\u0000 making previous decisions. Debate remains, however, over posited\u0000 psychological mechanisms underlying decision fatigue, and the size of\u0000 effects in high-stakes settings. We examine an extensive set of pretrial\u0000 arraignments in a large, urban court system to investigate how judicial\u0000 release and bail decisions are influenced by the time an arraignment occurs.\u0000 We find that release rates decline modestly in the hours before lunch and\u0000 before dinner, and these declines persist after statistically adjusting for\u0000 an extensive set of observed covariates. However, we find no evidence that\u0000 arraignment time affects pretrial release rates in the remainder of each\u0000 decision-making session. Moreover, we find that release rates remain\u0000 unchanged after a meal break even though judges have the opportunity to\u0000 replenish their mental and physical resources by resting and eating. In a\u0000 complementary analysis, we find that the rate at which judges concur with\u0000 prosecutorial bail requests does not appear to be influenced by either\u0000 arraignment time or a meal break. Taken together, our results imply that to\u0000 the extent that decision fatigue plays a role in pretrial release judgments,\u0000 effects are small and inconsistent with previous explanations implicating\u0000 psychological depletion processes.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45227563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The prominence effect in health-care priority setting 在卫生保健重点确定中的突出效应
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009463
Emil Persson, Arvid Erlandsson, P. Slovic, D. Västfjäll, G. Tinghög
People often choose the option that is better on the most subjectively prominent attribute — the prominence effect. We studied the effect of prominence in health care priority setting and hypothesized that values related to health would trump values related to costs in treatment choices, even when individuals themselves evaluated different treatment options as equally good. We conducted pre-registered experiments with a diverse Swedish sample and a sample of international experts on priority setting in health care (n = 1348). Participants, acting in the role of policy makers, revealed their valuation for different medical treatments in hypothetical scenarios. Participants were systematically inconsistent between preferences expressed through evaluation in a matching task and preferences expressed through choice. In line with our hypothesis, a large proportion of participants (General population: 92%, Experts 84% of all choices) chose treatment options that were better on the health dimension (lower health risk) despite having previously expressed indifference between those options and others that were better on the cost dimension. Thus, we find strong evidence of a prominence effect in health-care priority setting. Our findings provide a psychological explanation for why opportunity costs (i.e., the value of choices not exercised) are neglected in health care priority setting.
人们通常会在主观上最突出的属性上选择更好的选项——突出效应。我们研究了突出性对医疗保健优先级设置的影响,并假设在治疗选择中,与健康相关的价值观会胜过与成本相关的价值观,即使个人自己认为不同的治疗方案同样好。我们对不同的瑞典样本和国际专家样本(n = 1348)进行了预先注册的实验,以确定卫生保健的优先事项。参与者扮演政策制定者的角色,在假设的情况下透露他们对不同医疗方法的评价。参与者在匹配任务中通过评价表达的偏好和通过选择表达的偏好之间存在系统性的不一致。与我们的假设一致,很大一部分参与者(普通人群:92%,专家占所有选择的84%)选择了在健康方面更好的治疗方案(更低的健康风险),尽管他们之前对这些方案和其他在成本方面更好的方案表示漠不关心。因此,我们发现了强有力的证据,在卫生保健优先事项设置显著影响。我们的研究结果为为什么机会成本(即未行使的选择的价值)在卫生保健优先设置中被忽视提供了心理学解释。
{"title":"The prominence effect in health-care priority setting","authors":"Emil Persson, Arvid Erlandsson, P. Slovic, D. Västfjäll, G. Tinghög","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009463","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009463","url":null,"abstract":"People often choose the option that is better on the most subjectively prominent attribute — the prominence effect. We studied the effect of prominence in health care priority setting and hypothesized that values related to health would trump values related to costs in treatment choices, even when individuals themselves evaluated different treatment options as equally good. We conducted pre-registered experiments with a diverse Swedish sample and a sample of international experts on priority setting in health care (n = 1348). Participants, acting in the role of policy makers, revealed their valuation for different medical treatments in hypothetical scenarios. Participants were systematically inconsistent between preferences expressed through evaluation in a matching task and preferences expressed through choice. In line with our hypothesis, a large proportion of participants (General population: 92%, Experts 84% of all choices) chose treatment options that were better on the health dimension (lower health risk) despite having previously expressed indifference between those options and others that were better on the cost dimension. Thus, we find strong evidence of a prominence effect in health-care priority setting. Our findings provide a psychological explanation for why opportunity costs (i.e., the value of choices not exercised) are neglected in health care priority setting.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45375031","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social preferences before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in China 中国新冠肺炎疫情发生前后的社会偏好
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009438
King King Li, Yingyi Hong, Bo Huang, Tony Tam
This study compares Chinese people’s trust and trustworthiness, risk attitude, and time preference before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in China. We compare the preferences of subjects in two online experiments with samples drawn from 31 provinces across mainland China before and after the onset of the pandemic. We test two competing hypotheses regarding trust and trustworthiness. On the one hand, the outbreak as a collective threat could enhance in-group cohesion and cooperation and thus increase trust and trustworthiness. On the other hand, to the extent that people expect their future income to decline, they may become more self-protective and self-controlled, and thus less trusting and trustworthy and more risk averse and patient. Comparing before and after the onset, we found that the subjects increased in trustworthiness. After the onset, trust and trustworthiness (and risk aversion and present bias too) were positively correlated with the COVID-19 prevalence rate in the provinces. Subjects with more pessimistic expectations about income change showed more risk aversion and lower discount rates, supporting the speculation concerning self-control.
本研究比较了新冠肺炎疫情发生前后国人的信任可信度、风险态度和时间偏好。我们比较了两项在线实验中受试者的偏好,这些实验样本来自中国大陆31个省份,在疫情爆发前后。我们测试了关于信任和可信赖性的两个相互竞争的假设。一方面,疫情作为一种集体威胁,可以增强群体内部的凝聚力和合作,从而增加信任和可信度。另一方面,在某种程度上,人们预期他们未来的收入会下降,他们可能会变得更加自我保护和自我控制,从而减少信任和值得信赖,更加厌恶风险和耐心。对比发作前后,我们发现被试的可信度有所提高。发病后,各省的信任和可信度(以及风险厌恶和当前偏见)与COVID-19患病率呈正相关。对收入变化预期更悲观的受试者表现出更多的风险厌恶和更低的贴现率,支持有关自我控制的猜测。
{"title":"Social preferences before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in\u0000 China","authors":"King King Li, Yingyi Hong, Bo Huang, Tony Tam","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009438","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009438","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This study compares Chinese people’s trust and trustworthiness, risk\u0000 attitude, and time preference before and after the onset of the COVID-19\u0000 pandemic in China. We compare the preferences of subjects in two online\u0000 experiments with samples drawn from 31 provinces across mainland China\u0000 before and after the onset of the pandemic. We test two competing hypotheses\u0000 regarding trust and trustworthiness. On the one hand, the outbreak as a\u0000 collective threat could enhance in-group cohesion and cooperation and thus\u0000 increase trust and trustworthiness. On the other hand, to the extent that\u0000 people expect their future income to decline, they may become more\u0000 self-protective and self-controlled, and thus less trusting and trustworthy\u0000 and more risk averse and patient. Comparing before and after the onset, we\u0000 found that the subjects increased in trustworthiness. After the onset, trust\u0000 and trustworthiness (and risk aversion and present bias too) were positively\u0000 correlated with the COVID-19 prevalence rate in the provinces. Subjects with\u0000 more pessimistic expectations about income change showed more risk aversion\u0000 and lower discount rates, supporting the speculation concerning\u0000 self-control.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44386612","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Value-directed information search in partner choice 合作伙伴选择中的价值导向信息搜索
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009426
Hongyi Wang, Jiaxin Ma, Lisheng He
It is a widely held view that people rely on incomplete information to find a relationship partner, resulting in non-compensatory choice heuristics. However, recent experimental work typically finds that partner choice follows compensatory choice strategies. To bridge this gap between theory and experimental evidence, we characterize the mate choice problem by distinguishing the information search process from the evaluation process. In an eye-tracking experiment and a MouseLab experiment, we show that people display strong value-directed search heuristics in response to all types of cues and that the magnitude of value-directed searches increases with cue primacy. Cue primacy also explains the interaction effect of cue type and participant sex on the extent of valued-directed search. We further argue that value-directed searching does not necessarily lead to non-compensatory choice rules but may serve compensatory decision-making. Our results demonstrate that people may adopt remarkably smart search heuristics to find an ideal partner efficiently.
人们普遍认为,人们依靠不完全信息来寻找关系伴侣,这导致了非补偿性选择启发式。然而,最近的实验工作通常发现伴侣选择遵循补偿选择策略。为了弥合理论和实验证据之间的差距,我们通过区分信息搜索过程和评估过程来表征配偶选择问题。在眼动追踪实验和MouseLab实验中,我们发现人们对所有类型的线索都表现出强烈的价值导向搜索启发式,并且价值导向搜索的幅度随着线索首要性的增加而增加。线索首因还解释了线索类型和参与者性别对价值导向搜索程度的交互作用。我们进一步论证了价值导向搜索不一定导致非补偿性选择规则,但可能服务于补偿性决策。我们的研究结果表明,人们可能会采用非常聪明的搜索启发式来有效地找到理想的伴侣。
{"title":"Value-directed information search in partner choice","authors":"Hongyi Wang, Jiaxin Ma, Lisheng He","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009426","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009426","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 It is a widely held view that people rely on incomplete information to\u0000 find a relationship partner, resulting in non-compensatory choice\u0000 heuristics. However, recent experimental work typically finds that partner\u0000 choice follows compensatory choice strategies. To bridge this gap between\u0000 theory and experimental evidence, we characterize the mate choice problem by\u0000 distinguishing the information search process from the evaluation process.\u0000 In an eye-tracking experiment and a MouseLab experiment, we show that people\u0000 display strong value-directed search heuristics in response to all types of\u0000 cues and that the magnitude of value-directed searches increases with cue\u0000 primacy. Cue primacy also explains the interaction effect of cue type and\u0000 participant sex on the extent of valued-directed search. We further argue\u0000 that value-directed searching does not necessarily lead to non-compensatory\u0000 choice rules but may serve compensatory decision-making. Our results\u0000 demonstrate that people may adopt remarkably smart search heuristics to find\u0000 an ideal partner efficiently.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48802959","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
When and why people perform mindless math 人们什么时候以及为什么进行无意识的数学运算
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009396
M. A. Lawson, Richard P. Larrick, Jack B. Soll
In this paper, we show that the presence of numbers in a problem tempts people to perform mathematical operations even when the correct answer requires no math, which we term “mindless math”. In three pre-registered studies across two survey platforms (total N = 3,193), we investigate how mindless math relates to perceived problem difficulty, problem representation, and accuracy. In Study 1, we show that increasing the numeric demands of problems leads to more mindless math (and fewer correct answers). Study 2 shows that this effect is not caused by people being wary of problems that seem too easy. In Study 3, we show that this effect is robust over a wider range of numeric demands, and in the discussion we offer two possible mechanisms that would explain this effect, and the caveat that at even harder levels of numeric demands the effect may invert such that much harder math increases accuracy relative to moderately hard math.
在本文中,我们证明了问题中数字的存在会诱使人们进行数学运算,即使正确答案不需要数学,我们称之为“无脑数学”。在两个调查平台(总N = 3,193)的三个预注册研究中,我们调查了无意识数学与感知问题难度、问题表征和准确性之间的关系。在研究1中,我们表明,增加问题的数字要求会导致更多的不需要大脑的数学(和更少的正确答案)。研究2表明,这种影响不是由人们对看似太容易的问题保持警惕造成的。在研究3中,我们表明这种效应在更广泛的数字需求范围内是稳健的,在讨论中,我们提供了两种可能的机制来解释这种效应,并警告说,在更困难的数字需求水平上,这种效应可能会反转,即相对于中等难度的数学,更困难的数学会提高准确性。
{"title":"When and why people perform mindless math","authors":"M. A. Lawson, Richard P. Larrick, Jack B. Soll","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009396","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009396","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In this paper, we show that the presence of numbers in a problem tempts\u0000 people to perform mathematical operations even when the correct answer\u0000 requires no math, which we term “mindless math”. In three pre-registered\u0000 studies across two survey platforms (total N = 3,193), we investigate how\u0000 mindless math relates to perceived problem difficulty, problem\u0000 representation, and accuracy. In Study 1, we show that increasing the\u0000 numeric demands of problems leads to more mindless math (and fewer correct\u0000 answers). Study 2 shows that this effect is not caused by people being wary\u0000 of problems that seem too easy. In Study 3, we show that this effect is\u0000 robust over a wider range of numeric demands, and in the discussion we offer\u0000 two possible mechanisms that would explain this effect, and the caveat that\u0000 at even harder levels of numeric demands the effect may invert such that\u0000 much harder math increases accuracy relative\u0000 to moderately hard math.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44473986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Scientific contagion heuristic: Judgments about the acceptability of water for religious use after potential scientific treatment 科学传染启发式:对潜在科学处理后宗教用水可接受性的判断
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s193029750000944x
Sumita Mukherjee, P. Mukherjee
We propose the concept of ‘scientific contagion’ — a mental heuristic through which any form of scientific treatment transfers some essence of ‘science’ to the processed substance, thereby affecting its nature and social acceptability. This was tested regarding the potential treatment of water from natural sources before it is used for religious purposes, as many such sources have dangerous pollutants. For an ancient natural well having a religious narrative, most participants judged that the acceptability of water would be reduced for religious purposes but not for drinking if local officials scientifically treat the water. That is not the case if religious rituals are conducted on the water instead (Study 1). If water from a “holy river” is processed scientifically, most participants predicted that it would reduce acceptability for religious use while increasing acceptability for drinking (Study 2). Potential scientific treatment without altering the composition of water from a natural spring also decreased acceptability for religious use but there was no effect on acceptability for drinking or on willingness to pay money for the water (Study 3). A follow-up study comparing acceptability for different kinds of water sources — from a holy well, natural spring, and household tap water sourced from either underground wells or rivers found lower acceptability for religious usage compared to drinking after potential scientific treatment for all these waters, but more so for holy and natural waters (Study 4). These studies establish the phenomena of scientific contagion that could have significant social implications and open future directions.
我们提出了“科学传染”的概念,这是一种心理启发式方法,通过这种方法,任何形式的科学治疗都会将“科学”的一些本质转移到加工物质上,从而影响其性质和社会可接受性。在用于宗教目的之前,对天然水源的水的潜在处理进行了测试,因为许多天然水源都有危险的污染物。对于一口具有宗教叙事的古老自然井,大多数参与者判断,如果地方官员科学地处理水,那么出于宗教目的而非饮用目的的水的可接受性会降低。如果在水上举行宗教仪式,情况就不是这样了(研究1)。如果对来自“圣河”的水进行科学处理,大多数参与者预测,这将降低宗教用途的可接受性,同时提高饮用的可接受度(研究2)。在不改变天然泉水成分的情况下进行潜在的科学处理也降低了宗教用途的可接受性,但对饮用水的可接受程度或支付水费用的意愿没有影响(研究3)。一项后续研究比较了不同类型水源的可接受性——来自圣井、天然泉水和来自地下水井或河流的家庭自来水——发现,与对所有这些水进行潜在科学处理后的饮用水相比,宗教用途的可接受度较低,但圣水和天然水的可接受程度更高(研究4)。这些研究确立了科学传染现象,这些现象可能具有重大的社会影响和开辟未来的方向。
{"title":"Scientific contagion heuristic: Judgments about the acceptability of\u0000 water for religious use after potential scientific treatment","authors":"Sumita Mukherjee, P. Mukherjee","doi":"10.1017/s193029750000944x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s193029750000944x","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 We propose the concept of ‘scientific contagion’ — a mental heuristic\u0000 through which any form of scientific treatment transfers some essence of\u0000 ‘science’ to the processed substance, thereby affecting its nature and\u0000 social acceptability. This was tested regarding the potential treatment of\u0000 water from natural sources before it is used for religious purposes, as many\u0000 such sources have dangerous pollutants. For an ancient natural well having a\u0000 religious narrative, most participants judged that the acceptability of\u0000 water would be reduced for religious purposes but not for drinking if local\u0000 officials scientifically treat the water. That is not the case if religious\u0000 rituals are conducted on the water instead (Study 1). If water from a “holy\u0000 river” is processed scientifically, most participants predicted that it\u0000 would reduce acceptability for religious use while increasing acceptability\u0000 for drinking (Study 2). Potential scientific treatment without altering the\u0000 composition of water from a natural spring also decreased acceptability for\u0000 religious use but there was no effect on acceptability for drinking or on\u0000 willingness to pay money for the water (Study 3). A follow-up study\u0000 comparing acceptability for different kinds of water sources — from a holy\u0000 well, natural spring, and household tap water sourced from either\u0000 underground wells or rivers found lower acceptability for religious usage\u0000 compared to drinking after potential scientific treatment for all these\u0000 waters, but more so for holy and natural waters (Study 4). These studies\u0000 establish the phenomena of scientific contagion that could have significant\u0000 social implications and open future directions.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43864630","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Successful everyday decision making: Combining attributes and associates 成功的日常决策:结合属性和关联
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 Economics, Econometrics and Finance Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500009414
A. Banks, David M. Gamblin
How do people make everyday decisions in order to achieve the most successful outcome? Decision making research typically evaluates choices according to their expected utility. However, this research largely focuses on abstract or hypothetical tasks and rarely investigates whether the outcome is successful and satisfying for the decision maker. Instead, we use an everyday decision making task in which participants describe a personally meaningful decision they are currently facing. We investigate the decision processes used to make this decision, and evaluate how successful and satisfying the outcome of the decision is for them. We examine how well analytic, attribute-based processes explain everyday decision making and predict decision outcomes, and we compare these processes to associative processes elicited through free association. We also examine the characteristics of decisions and individuals that are associated with good decision outcomes. Across three experiments we found that: 1) an analytic decision analysis of everyday decisions is not superior to simpler attribute-based processes in predicting decision outcomes; 2) contrary to research linking associative cognition to biases, free association generates valid cues that predict choice and decision outcomes as effectively as attribute-based approaches; 3) contrary to research favouring either attribute-based or associative processes, combining both attribute-based and associates best explains everyday decisions and most accurately predicts decision outcomes; and 4) individuals with a tendency to attempt analytic thinking do not make more successful everyday decisions. Instead, frequency, simplicity, and knowledge of the decision predict success. We propose that attribute-based and associative processes, in combination, both explain everyday decision making and predict successful decision outcomes.
人们如何在日常生活中做出决定以获得最成功的结果?决策研究通常根据选择的预期效用来评估选择。然而,这项研究主要集中在抽象或假设的任务上,很少调查结果是否成功并让决策者满意。相反,我们使用一项日常决策任务,参与者在其中描述他们目前面临的个人有意义的决定。我们调查了用于做出该决策的决策过程,并评估决策结果对他们来说有多成功和令人满意。我们研究了基于属性的分析过程如何很好地解释日常决策和预测决策结果,并将这些过程与通过自由联想引发的联想过程进行了比较。我们还研究了与良好决策结果相关的决策和个人的特征。在三个实验中,我们发现:1)日常决策的分析决策分析在预测决策结果方面并不优于更简单的基于属性的过程;2) 与将联想认知与偏见联系起来的研究相反,自由联想产生了有效的线索,可以像基于属性的方法一样有效地预测选择和决策结果;3) 与倾向于基于属性或关联过程的研究相反,将基于属性和关联过程相结合,可以最好地解释日常决策,并最准确地预测决策结果;和4)倾向于尝试分析思维的人不会做出更成功的日常决策。相反,决策的频率、简单性和知识可以预测成功。我们提出,基于属性的过程和关联过程相结合,既可以解释日常决策,又可以预测成功的决策结果。
{"title":"Successful everyday decision making: Combining attributes and\u0000 associates","authors":"A. Banks, David M. Gamblin","doi":"10.1017/s1930297500009414","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1930297500009414","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 How do people make everyday decisions in order to achieve the most\u0000 successful outcome? Decision making research typically evaluates choices\u0000 according to their expected utility. However, this research largely focuses\u0000 on abstract or hypothetical tasks and rarely investigates whether the\u0000 outcome is successful and satisfying for the decision maker. Instead, we use\u0000 an everyday decision making task in which participants describe a personally\u0000 meaningful decision they are currently facing. We investigate the decision\u0000 processes used to make this decision, and evaluate how successful and\u0000 satisfying the outcome of the decision is for them. We examine how well\u0000 analytic, attribute-based processes explain everyday decision making and\u0000 predict decision outcomes, and we compare these processes to associative\u0000 processes elicited through free association. We also examine the\u0000 characteristics of decisions and individuals that are associated with good\u0000 decision outcomes. Across three experiments we found that: 1) an analytic\u0000 decision analysis of everyday decisions is not superior to simpler\u0000 attribute-based processes in predicting decision outcomes; 2) contrary to\u0000 research linking associative cognition to biases, free association generates\u0000 valid cues that predict choice and decision outcomes as effectively as\u0000 attribute-based approaches; 3) contrary to research favouring either\u0000 attribute-based or associative processes, combining both attribute-based and\u0000 associates best explains everyday decisions and most accurately predicts\u0000 decision outcomes; and 4) individuals with a tendency to attempt analytic\u0000 thinking do not make more successful everyday decisions. Instead, frequency,\u0000 simplicity, and knowledge of the decision predict success. We propose that\u0000 attribute-based and associative processes, in combination, both explain\u0000 everyday decision making and predict successful decision outcomes.","PeriodicalId":48045,"journal":{"name":"Judgment and Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46076261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Judgment and Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1