首页 > 最新文献

Social Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Tugging at Their Heartstrings: Partner's Knowledge of Affective Meta-Bases Predicts Use of Emotional Advocacies in Close Relationships 牵动他们的心弦:伴侣对情感元基础的了解预测了亲密关系中情感主张的使用
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.150
Kenneth Tan, Y. See
Traditional studies of attitude change have focused on attempts between strangers, but what about in close relationships? The present article examines whether accuracy regarding a partner's meta-attitudinal bases can influence persuasion attempts. Because meta-bases reflect informationprocessing goals, we hypothesized that given partners with more affective meta-bases, greater accuracy regarding partners' meta-bases would predict use of emotional advocacies and their perceived persuasiveness. Self and partner ratings of meta-bases were assessed, and emotional advocacies as well as cognitive ones were provided to participants to present to their partners. Results revealed that the correspondence between perceptions of partner's affective meta-bases and use of emotional advocacies was greater among those whose partners rated themselves as having more affective meta-bases compared to those whose partners rated themselves as having less affective meta-bases. Results remained significant when controlling for perceived similarity. Implications of meta-bases for understanding interpersonal influence are discussed.
关于态度改变的传统研究主要集中在陌生人之间的尝试,但在亲密关系中呢?本文探讨了合作伙伴的元态度基础的准确性是否会影响说服尝试。由于元基础反映了信息处理目标,我们假设如果伴侣具有更多的情感元基础,那么关于伴侣元基础的准确性就会更高,这将预测他们使用情感倡导和他们感知到的说服力。对元基的自我和伴侣评分进行评估,并向参与者提供情感倡导和认知倡导,供他们向伴侣展示。结果显示,与那些认为自己有更多情感元基础的人相比,那些认为自己有更少情感元基础的人对伴侣情感元基础的感知和情感倡导的使用之间的对应关系更大。当控制感知相似性时,结果仍然显着。讨论了理解人际影响的元基础的含义。
{"title":"Tugging at Their Heartstrings: Partner's Knowledge of Affective Meta-Bases Predicts Use of Emotional Advocacies in Close Relationships","authors":"Kenneth Tan, Y. See","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.150","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.150","url":null,"abstract":"Traditional studies of attitude change have focused on attempts between strangers, but what about in close relationships? The present article examines whether accuracy regarding a partner's meta-attitudinal bases can influence persuasion attempts. Because meta-bases reflect informationprocessing goals, we hypothesized that given partners with more affective meta-bases, greater accuracy regarding partners' meta-bases would predict use of emotional advocacies and their perceived persuasiveness. Self and partner ratings of meta-bases were assessed, and emotional advocacies as well as cognitive ones were provided to participants to present to their partners. Results revealed that the correspondence between perceptions of partner's affective meta-bases and use of emotional advocacies was greater among those whose partners rated themselves as having more affective meta-bases compared to those whose partners rated themselves as having less affective meta-bases. Results remained significant when controlling for perceived similarity. Implications of meta-bases for understanding interpersonal influence are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46238176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shared Reality Effects of Tuning Messages to Multiple Audiences 向多个受众调整信息的共享现实效果
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.172
Federica Pinelli, L. Davachi, E. Higgins
Our study explores how communicating with audiences who hold opposite opinions about a target person can lead to a biased recall of the target's behaviors depending on whom a shared reality is created with. By extending the standard “saying-is-believing” paradigm to the case of two audiences with opposite attitudes toward a target person, we found that communicators evaluatively tune their message to the attitude of each audience. Still, their later recall of the target's behavior is biased toward the audience's attitude only for the audience with whom they created a shared reality. Shared reality creation was manipulated by receiving feedback that, based on the communicator's message, an audience was either able (success) or unable (failure) to successfully identify the target person, with the former creating a shared reality. These results highlight the importance of shared reality creation for subsequent recall when communicating with multiple audiences on a topic.
我们的研究探讨了与对目标人物持相反看法的观众交流如何导致对目标人物行为的偏见回忆,这取决于与谁共同创造了一个共同的现实。通过将标准的“说即信”范式扩展到两个对目标人持相反态度的受众的情况,我们发现传播者会根据每个受众的态度评估地调整他们的信息。尽管如此,他们后来对目标行为的回忆只对与他们创造了共同现实的观众的态度有偏见。共享现实的创建是通过接收反馈来操纵的,根据传播者的信息,受众能够(成功)或不能(失败)成功地识别目标人物,前者创建共享现实。这些结果强调了在与多个受众就一个主题进行交流时,创建共享现实对于后续回忆的重要性。
{"title":"Shared Reality Effects of Tuning Messages to Multiple Audiences","authors":"Federica Pinelli, L. Davachi, E. Higgins","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.172","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.172","url":null,"abstract":"Our study explores how communicating with audiences who hold opposite opinions about a target person can lead to a biased recall of the target's behaviors depending on whom a shared reality is created with. By extending the standard “saying-is-believing” paradigm to the case of two audiences with opposite attitudes toward a target person, we found that communicators evaluatively tune their message to the attitude of each audience. Still, their later recall of the target's behavior is biased toward the audience's attitude only for the audience with whom they created a shared reality. Shared reality creation was manipulated by receiving feedback that, based on the communicator's message, an audience was either able (success) or unable (failure) to successfully identify the target person, with the former creating a shared reality. These results highlight the importance of shared reality creation for subsequent recall when communicating with multiple audiences on a topic.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42181152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who Can Be Fooled? Modeling Facial Impressions of Gullibility 谁会被愚弄?模拟易受骗的面部印象
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.127
Bastian Jaeger, Erdem O. Meral
The success of acts of deceit and exploitation depends on how trusting and naïve (i.e., gullible) targets are. In three preregistered studies, using both theory-driven and data-driven approaches, we examined how people form impressions of gullibility based on targets' facial appearance. We find significant consensus in gullibility impressions, suggesting that people have a somewhat shared representation of what a gullible person looks like (Study 1, n = 294). Gullibility impressions is based on different cues than trustworthiness or dominance impressions, suggesting that they constitute dissociable facial stereotypes (Study 2, n = 403). Examining a wide range of facial features, we find that gullibility impressions are primarily based on resemblance to an angry facial expression. We also find that young, female, and smiling individuals were seen as more gullible (Study 3, n = 209). These findings suggest that gullibility impressions are based on cues linked to low levels of perceived threat.
欺骗和剥削行为的成功取决于目标的信任程度和天真程度。在三项预先注册的研究中,我们使用理论驱动和数据驱动的方法,研究了人们如何根据目标的面部表情形成易受骗印象。我们在易受骗印象中发现了显著的共识,这表明人们对易受骗的人的长相有某种共同的看法(研究1,n=294)。轻信印象与可信度或支配性印象基于不同的线索,这表明它们构成了可分解的面部刻板印象(研究2,n=403)。通过检查各种面部特征,我们发现易受骗的印象主要是基于与愤怒面部表情的相似性。我们还发现,年轻、女性和微笑的人更容易上当受骗(研究3,n=209)。这些发现表明,易受骗印象是基于与低水平感知威胁相关的线索。
{"title":"Who Can Be Fooled? Modeling Facial Impressions of Gullibility","authors":"Bastian Jaeger, Erdem O. Meral","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.127","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.127","url":null,"abstract":"The success of acts of deceit and exploitation depends on how trusting and naïve (i.e., gullible) targets are. In three preregistered studies, using both theory-driven and data-driven approaches, we examined how people form impressions of gullibility based on targets' facial appearance. We find significant consensus in gullibility impressions, suggesting that people have a somewhat shared representation of what a gullible person looks like (Study 1, n = 294). Gullibility impressions is based on different cues than trustworthiness or dominance impressions, suggesting that they constitute dissociable facial stereotypes (Study 2, n = 403). Examining a wide range of facial features, we find that gullibility impressions are primarily based on resemblance to an angry facial expression. We also find that young, female, and smiling individuals were seen as more gullible (Study 3, n = 209). These findings suggest that gullibility impressions are based on cues linked to low levels of perceived threat.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44048841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Inferring Goals and Traits From Behaviors: The Role of Culture, Self-Construal, and Thinking Style 从行为推断目标和特征:文化、自我构成和思维方式的作用
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.184
Ceren Günsoy, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten
People make spontaneous inferences from others' behaviors, such as spontaneous trait inferences (STIs) and spontaneous situation inferences (SSIs). People's behaviors, however, are shaped by their goals as well, which are determined by their internal characteristics (e.g., traits) and by contextual factors (e.g., social roles). In three studies, we examined whether self-construal, culture, and holistic thinking were associated with spontaneous goal inferences (SGIs) and STIs. We found that the more people defined themselves interdependently and viewed the causality of events holistically, the more they made SGIs (Study 1). Moreover, Asian American participants (interdependent) made SGIs but no STIs from people's behaviors, whereas European American participants (independent) made both (Study 2). Finally, participants primed with interdependence made SGIs but no STIs, whereas those primed with independence made both (Study 3). This research is the first to examine SGIs in relation to self-construal, culture, and holistic thinking.
人们从他人的行为中进行自发的推断,如自发的特质推断(STIs)和自发的情境推断(ssi)。然而,人们的行为也受到他们的目标的影响,目标是由他们的内在特征(例如,性格)和环境因素(例如,社会角色)决定的。在三项研究中,我们考察了自我解释、文化和整体思维是否与自发目标推断(sgi)和性传播感染有关。我们发现,越多的人定义自己是相互依赖的,并从整体上看待事件的因果关系,他们就会做出更多的sgi(研究1)。此外,亚裔美国参与者(相互依赖)会做出sgi,但不会从人们的行为中做出sti,而欧洲美国参与者(独立)会做出这两种行为(研究2)。最后,被相互依赖启动的参与者会做出sgi,但不会做出sti。而那些被独立启动的人两者都做到了(研究3)。这项研究首次考察了sgi与自我解释、文化和整体思维的关系。
{"title":"Inferring Goals and Traits From Behaviors: The Role of Culture, Self-Construal, and Thinking Style","authors":"Ceren Günsoy, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.184","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.2.184","url":null,"abstract":"People make spontaneous inferences from others' behaviors, such as spontaneous trait inferences (STIs) and spontaneous situation inferences (SSIs). People's behaviors, however, are shaped by their goals as well, which are determined by their internal characteristics (e.g., traits) and by contextual factors (e.g., social roles). In three studies, we examined whether self-construal, culture, and holistic thinking were associated with spontaneous goal inferences (SGIs) and STIs. We found that the more people defined themselves interdependently and viewed the causality of events holistically, the more they made SGIs (Study 1). Moreover, Asian American participants (interdependent) made SGIs but no STIs from people's behaviors, whereas European American participants (independent) made both (Study 2). Finally, participants primed with interdependence made SGIs but no STIs, whereas those primed with independence made both (Study 3). This research is the first to examine SGIs in relation to self-construal, culture, and holistic thinking.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46569464","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Moral Evaluations of Humor Apply Beyond Just Those Telling the Joke 幽默的道德评价不仅仅适用于讲笑话的人
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-02-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.107
Wei Jee Ong, Kai Chi Yam, Christopher M. Barnes
Humor involves both joke-tellers and listeners, both of whom are subject to observers' evaluations. Past research has suggested a tension between humor and morality such that moral individuals may be less humorous, and humor may promote tolerance of moral violations. Building on this work, we highlight that individuals engaging in humor are themselves subject to inferences of moral character. Joke-tellers are evaluated as less moral people when their jokes are offensive. Individuals who laugh at jokes are similarly evaluated as less moral, but only when the jokes are offensive, not clean. Across two experiments (Studies 1 and 2) using different manipulations, we found support for these effects and the mediating role of perceived norm violations. In Study 3, we further found preliminary evidence depicting nuanced similarities and differences between the effects on moral evaluations and other-person perceptions such as warmth and competence. These findings contribute to understanding of moral judgment in humor.
幽默既涉及讲笑话的人,也涉及听众,他们都要接受观察者的评价。过去的研究表明,幽默和道德之间存在紧张关系,道德个体可能不那么幽默,幽默可能会促进对道德违规行为的容忍。在这项工作的基础上,我们强调,从事幽默的个人本身就受到道德品质的推断。当讲笑话的人讲的笑话令人反感时,他们会被评价为道德败坏的人。对笑话发笑的人也同样被评价为不那么道德,但只有当笑话是冒犯性的,而不是干净的。在使用不同操作的两个实验(研究1和2)中,我们发现了对这些影响以及感知到的规范违反的中介作用的支持。在研究3中,我们进一步发现了初步证据,描述了道德评价与他人感知(如热情和能力)之间的细微相似和差异。这些发现有助于理解幽默中的道德判断。
{"title":"Moral Evaluations of Humor Apply Beyond Just Those Telling the Joke","authors":"Wei Jee Ong, Kai Chi Yam, Christopher M. Barnes","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.107","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.107","url":null,"abstract":"Humor involves both joke-tellers and listeners, both of whom are subject to observers' evaluations. Past research has suggested a tension between humor and morality such that moral individuals may be less humorous, and humor may promote tolerance of moral violations. Building on this work, we highlight that individuals engaging in humor are themselves subject to inferences of moral character. Joke-tellers are evaluated as less moral people when their jokes are offensive. Individuals who laugh at jokes are similarly evaluated as less moral, but only when the jokes are offensive, not clean. Across two experiments (Studies 1 and 2) using different manipulations, we found support for these effects and the mediating role of perceived norm violations. In Study 3, we further found preliminary evidence depicting nuanced similarities and differences between the effects on moral evaluations and other-person perceptions such as warmth and competence. These findings contribute to understanding of moral judgment in humor.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48284466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the Link Between Neutral and Ambivalent Attitudes: Their Association and their Co-Occurrence 中性态度与矛盾态度之间的联系:它们的关联与共现
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-02-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.1
Danfei Hu, Karen Gasper
We examined the link between neutral and ambivalent attitudes. First, we examined whether they are mutually exclusive, in that when one attitude is present, the other is not, or if they co-occur. Second, we examined whether they are negatively associated, such that as ambivalence increases, neutrality decreases. In three studies, participants indicated their positive, negative, neutral, and ambivalent attitudes toward various stimuli. In contrast to both ideas, (objective and subjective) ambivalence and neutrality co-occurred (no mutual exclusivity), and they, at best, depending on how one looked at the association, were only weakly positively or negatively associated (no strong, negative association). Univalent attitudes co-occurred with and were strongly negatively associated with neutral attitudes. These findings conflict with theories and methodologies that assume mutual exclusivity or an inverse association between neutral and ambivalent attitudes, suggesting that researchers should not assume that presence of neutrality suggests a lack of ambivalence.
我们研究了中立态度和矛盾态度之间的联系。首先,我们研究了它们是否相互排斥,即当一种态度存在时,另一种态度不存在,或者它们是否同时存在。其次,我们研究了它们是否存在负相关,比如随着矛盾心理的增加,中立性降低。在三项研究中,参与者表明了他们对各种刺激的积极、消极、中立和矛盾态度。与这两种观点相反,(客观和主观)矛盾心理和中立性同时发生(没有相互排斥),它们充其量只是微弱的正相关或负相关(没有强烈的负相关),这取决于人们如何看待这种关联。单一态度与中性态度共存,并与中性态度呈强烈的负相关。这些发现与假设中立和矛盾态度之间相互排斥或反向关联的理论和方法相冲突,表明研究人员不应假设中立的存在表明缺乏矛盾心理。
{"title":"Examining the Link Between Neutral and Ambivalent Attitudes: Their Association and their Co-Occurrence","authors":"Danfei Hu, Karen Gasper","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.1","url":null,"abstract":"We examined the link between neutral and ambivalent attitudes. First, we examined whether they are mutually exclusive, in that when one attitude is present, the other is not, or if they co-occur. Second, we examined whether they are negatively associated, such that as ambivalence increases, neutrality decreases. In three studies, participants indicated their positive, negative, neutral, and ambivalent attitudes toward various stimuli. In contrast to both ideas, (objective and subjective) ambivalence and neutrality co-occurred (no mutual exclusivity), and they, at best, depending on how one looked at the association, were only weakly positively or negatively associated (no strong, negative association). Univalent attitudes co-occurred with and were strongly negatively associated with neutral attitudes. These findings conflict with theories and methodologies that assume mutual exclusivity or an inverse association between neutral and ambivalent attitudes, suggesting that researchers should not assume that presence of neutrality suggests a lack of ambivalence.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48337283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Unwilling to Un-Blame: Whites Who Dismiss Historical Causes of Societal Disparities Also Dismiss Personal Mitigating Information for Black Offenders 不愿推卸责任:否认社会差异历史原因的白人也否认黑人罪犯的个人缓解信息
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-02-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.55
M. Gill, A. Pizzuto
When will racial bias in blame and punishment emerge? Here, we focus on White people's willingness to “un-blame” Black and White offenders upon learning of their unfortunate life histories or biological impairments. We predicted that personal mitigating narratives of Black (but not White) offenders would be ignored by Whites who are societal-level anti-historicists. Societal-level anti-historicists deny that a history of oppression by Whites has shaped current societal-level intergroup disparities. Thus, our prediction centers on how societal-level beliefs relate to bias against individuals. Our predictions were confirmed in three studies. In one of those studies, we also showed how racial bias in willingness to un-blame can be removed: Societal-level anti-historicists became open to mitigation for Black offenders if they were reminded that the offender began as an innocent baby. Results are discussed in terms of how the rich literature on blame and moral psychology could enrich the study of racial bias.
指责和惩罚中的种族偏见何时会出现?在这里,我们关注的是白人在得知黑人和白人罪犯的不幸生活史或生物损伤后,是否愿意“推卸责任”。我们预测,作为社会层面的反历史主义者,白人会忽视黑人(但不是白人)罪犯的个人减轻叙事。社会层面的反历史主义者否认白人压迫的历史塑造了当前社会层面的群体间差异。因此,我们的预测集中在社会层面的信仰与对个人的偏见之间的关系上。我们的预测在三项研究中得到了证实。在其中一项研究中,我们还展示了如何消除不愿推卸责任的种族偏见:如果提醒黑人罪犯从无辜婴儿开始,社会层面的反历史主义者就会对黑人罪犯采取缓解措施。结果从丰富的指责和道德心理学文献如何丰富种族偏见的研究方面进行了讨论。
{"title":"Unwilling to Un-Blame: Whites Who Dismiss Historical Causes of Societal Disparities Also Dismiss Personal Mitigating Information for Black Offenders","authors":"M. Gill, A. Pizzuto","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.55","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.55","url":null,"abstract":"When will racial bias in blame and punishment emerge? Here, we focus on White people's willingness to “un-blame” Black and White offenders upon learning of their unfortunate life histories or biological impairments. We predicted that personal mitigating narratives of Black (but not White) offenders would be ignored by Whites who are societal-level anti-historicists. Societal-level anti-historicists deny that a history of oppression by Whites has shaped current societal-level intergroup disparities. Thus, our prediction centers on how societal-level beliefs relate to bias against individuals. Our predictions were confirmed in three studies. In one of those studies, we also showed how racial bias in willingness to un-blame can be removed: Societal-level anti-historicists became open to mitigation for Black offenders if they were reminded that the offender began as an innocent baby. Results are discussed in terms of how the rich literature on blame and moral psychology could enrich the study of racial bias.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42727996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Generalized Approach/Avoidance Responses to Degraded Affective Stimuli: An Informational Account 对退化的情感刺激的普遍化接近/回避反应:一个信息解释
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-02-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.29
Nicolas Pillaud, François Ric
Two studies tested whether affective stimuli presented auditorily spontaneously trigger approach/avoidance reactions toward neutral visual stimuli. Contrary to hypotheses, Experiment 1 revealed that when the target was present, participants responded faster after positive (vs. negative) stimuli, and faster to the absence of the target following negative (vs. positive) stimuli, whatever the response modality (i.e., approach/avoidance). Instructions were to approach/avoid stimuli depending on whether a target was presented or not presented. We proposed that affective stimuli were used in this study as information about the presence/absence of the target. In Experiment 2, we replicated the results of Experiment 1 when participants responded to the presence/absence of the target, whereas an approach/avoidance compatibility effect was observed when each response modality was associated with a target. These results indicate that affective stimuli influence approach/avoidance across perceptual modalities and suggest that the link between affective stimuli and behavioral tendencies could be mediated by informational value of affect.
两项研究测试了听觉上呈现的情感刺激是否会自发触发对中性视觉刺激的接近/回避反应。与假设相反,实验1表明,当目标存在时,参与者在积极(与消极)刺激后的反应更快,在消极(与积极)刺激后对没有目标的反应更快——无论反应模式如何(即接近/回避)。指令是根据是否出现目标来接近/避免刺激。我们提出在这项研究中使用情感刺激作为关于目标存在/不存在的信息。在实验2中,当参与者对目标的存在/不存在做出反应时,我们复制了实验1的结果,而当每个反应模式都与目标相关时,观察到了接近/回避兼容性效应。这些结果表明,情感刺激会影响感知模式中的接近/回避,并表明情感刺激和行为倾向之间的联系可能由情感的信息价值介导。
{"title":"Generalized Approach/Avoidance Responses to Degraded Affective Stimuli: An Informational Account","authors":"Nicolas Pillaud, François Ric","doi":"10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.29","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2022.40.1.29","url":null,"abstract":"Two studies tested whether affective stimuli presented auditorily spontaneously trigger approach/avoidance reactions toward neutral visual stimuli. Contrary to hypotheses, Experiment 1 revealed that when the target was present, participants responded faster after positive (vs. negative) stimuli, and faster to the absence of the target following negative (vs. positive) stimuli, whatever the response modality (i.e., approach/avoidance). Instructions were to approach/avoid stimuli depending on whether a target was presented or not presented. We proposed that affective stimuli were used in this study as information about the presence/absence of the target. In Experiment 2, we replicated the results of Experiment 1 when participants responded to the presence/absence of the target, whereas an approach/avoidance compatibility effect was observed when each response modality was associated with a target. These results indicate that affective stimuli influence approach/avoidance across perceptual modalities and suggest that the link between affective stimuli and behavioral tendencies could be mediated by informational value of affect.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42728346","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Master Narratives, Expectations of Change, and Their Effect on Temporal Appraisals 主叙述、对变化的期望及其对时间评价的影响
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.717
J. Hillman, David J. Hauser
People hold narrative expectations for how humans generally change over the course of their lives. In some areas, people expect growth (e.g., wisdom), while in others, people expect stability (e.g., extroversion). However, do people apply those same expectations to the self? In five studies (total N = 1,372), participants rated selves as improving modestly over time in domains where stability should be expected (e.g., extroversion, quick-wittedness). Reported improvement was significantly larger in domains where growth should be expected (e.g., wisdom, rationality) than domains where stability should be expected. Further, in domains where growth should be expected participants reported improvement for selves and others. However, in domains where stability should be expected, participants reported improvement for selves but not others. Hence, participants used narrative expectations to inform projections of change. We discuss implications for future temporal self-appraisal research, heterogeneity of effect sizes in self-appraisal research, and between-culture differences in narratives.
人们对人类在一生中的总体变化抱有叙事期望。在某些领域,人们期待成长(例如智慧),而在另一些领域,人们期望稳定(例如外向)。然而,人们是否也对自己抱有同样的期望?在五项研究中(总N=1372),参与者认为自己在应该稳定的领域(如外向、机智)随着时间的推移会有适度的改善。报告的改善在应预期增长的领域(如智慧、理性)明显大于应预期稳定的领域。此外,在预期增长的领域,参与者报告了自己和他人的进步。然而,在应该期望稳定性的领域,参与者报告了自我的改善,但没有报告其他人的改善。因此,参与者使用叙述性期望来预测变化。我们讨论了对未来时间自我评价研究的影响,自我评价研究中效果大小的异质性,以及叙事中文化差异之间的差异。
{"title":"Master Narratives, Expectations of Change, and Their Effect on Temporal Appraisals","authors":"J. Hillman, David J. Hauser","doi":"10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.717","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.717","url":null,"abstract":"People hold narrative expectations for how humans generally change over the course of their lives. In some areas, people expect growth (e.g., wisdom), while in others, people expect stability (e.g., extroversion). However, do people apply those same expectations to the self? In five studies (total N = 1,372), participants rated selves as improving modestly over time in domains where stability should be expected (e.g., extroversion, quick-wittedness). Reported improvement was significantly larger in domains where growth should be expected (e.g., wisdom, rationality) than domains where stability should be expected. Further, in domains where growth should be expected participants reported improvement for selves and others. However, in domains where stability should be expected, participants reported improvement for selves but not others. Hence, participants used narrative expectations to inform projections of change. We discuss implications for future temporal self-appraisal research, heterogeneity of effect sizes in self-appraisal research, and between-culture differences in narratives.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49404902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Pupil Size Predicts Partner Choices in Online Dating 学生规模预测在线约会中的伴侣选择
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.773
Tila M. Pronk, R. Bogaers, M. Verheijen, Willem W. A. Sleegers
People's choices for specific romantic partners can have far reaching consequences, but very little is known about the process of partner selection. In the current study, we tested whether a measure of physiological arousal, pupillometry (i.e., changes in pupil size), can predict partner choices in an online dating setting. A total of 239 heterosexual participants took part in an online dating task in which they accepted or rejected hypothetical potential partners, while pupil size response was registered using an eye tracker. In line with our main hypothesis, the results indicated a positive association between pupil size and partner acceptance. This association was not found to depend on relationship status, relationship quality, gender, or sociosexual orientation. These findings show that the body (i.e., the pupils) provides an automatic cue of whether a potential partner will be selected as a mate, or rejected.
人们对特定浪漫伴侣的选择可能会产生深远的影响,但对伴侣选择的过程知之甚少。在目前的研究中,我们测试了一种生理唤醒指标,瞳孔测量(即瞳孔大小的变化),是否可以预测在线约会环境中的伴侣选择。共有239名异性恋参与者参加了一项在线约会任务,他们接受或拒绝假设的潜在伴侣,同时使用眼动仪记录瞳孔大小的反应。与我们的主要假设一致,研究结果表明,瞳孔大小与伴侣接受度之间存在正相关。这种关联并不取决于关系状态、关系质量、性别或社会性取向。这些发现表明,身体(即瞳孔)会自动提示潜在的伴侣是会被选为伴侣,还是会被拒绝。
{"title":"Pupil Size Predicts Partner Choices in Online Dating","authors":"Tila M. Pronk, R. Bogaers, M. Verheijen, Willem W. A. Sleegers","doi":"10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.773","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2021.39.6.773","url":null,"abstract":"People's choices for specific romantic partners can have far reaching consequences, but very little is known about the process of partner selection. In the current study, we tested whether a measure of physiological arousal, pupillometry (i.e., changes in pupil size), can predict partner choices in an online dating setting. A total of 239 heterosexual participants took part in an online dating task in which they accepted or rejected hypothetical potential partners, while pupil size response was registered using an eye tracker. In line with our main hypothesis, the results indicated a positive association between pupil size and partner acceptance. This association was not found to depend on relationship status, relationship quality, gender, or sociosexual orientation. These findings show that the body (i.e., the pupils) provides an automatic cue of whether a potential partner will be selected as a mate, or rejected.","PeriodicalId":48050,"journal":{"name":"Social Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49590546","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Social Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1