首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung最新文献

英文 中文
Social cues tip the scales in emotional processing of complex pictures. 社交线索会影响复杂图片的情感处理。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-21 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02022-y
Veronica Dudarev, Victoria Wardell, James T Enns, Connor M Kerns, Daniela J Palombo

Perception of a picture is influenced by the social information and emotional value it carries for the viewer. There are still many unanswered questions about how social and emotional processing are related, but it is clear they involve overlapping brain areas and are cognitively interconnected. Yet studies of emotion processing using standardized picture datasets typically leave the social content in the pictures free to vary. In a few studies where the social content has been measured, it correlated with emotional arousal and valence. Here we tested the association between social and emotional content orthogonally, by selecting a similar number of pictures in four categories varying in presence of nonverbal social cues (e.g., gestures, facial expression, body language) and emotional content (neutral, negative). Across two studies, participants (N = 698 in total) provided three ratings for each picture: social relevance (defined as the self-reported use of social cues to understand the picture), valence, and arousal. Despite our best effort to balance the presence of social cues between negative and neutral pictures, ratings of social relevance were strongly associated with ratings of arousal and, to a lesser extent, with valence. These findings likely reflect the intertwined nature of social and emotional processing, which has implications for the neurobiology underlying them, how these systems develop, and how picture databases are used in research.

对图片的感知会受到图片所承载的社会信息和情感价值的影响。关于社会处理和情感处理之间的关系,仍有许多未解之谜,但它们显然涉及重叠的大脑区域,并在认知上相互关联。然而,使用标准化图片数据集进行的情绪处理研究通常会让图片中的社交内容自由变化。在少数对社交内容进行测量的研究中,社交内容与情绪唤醒和情绪价值相关。在这里,我们通过在非语言社交线索(如手势、面部表情、肢体语言)和情感内容(中性、负面)不同的四个类别中选择数量相似的图片,正交地测试了社交内容和情感内容之间的关联。在两项研究中,参与者(共 698 人)为每幅图片提供了三个评分:社会相关性(定义为自我报告的使用社会线索来理解图片的情况)、情绪和唤醒。尽管我们尽力在负面和中性图片之间平衡社会线索的存在,但社会相关性的评分仍与唤醒度的评分密切相关,其次才是情绪。这些发现可能反映了社会和情感处理相互交织的性质,这对它们背后的神经生物学、这些系统如何发展以及如何在研究中使用图片数据库都有影响。
{"title":"Social cues tip the scales in emotional processing of complex pictures.","authors":"Veronica Dudarev, Victoria Wardell, James T Enns, Connor M Kerns, Daniela J Palombo","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02022-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02022-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perception of a picture is influenced by the social information and emotional value it carries for the viewer. There are still many unanswered questions about how social and emotional processing are related, but it is clear they involve overlapping brain areas and are cognitively interconnected. Yet studies of emotion processing using standardized picture datasets typically leave the social content in the pictures free to vary. In a few studies where the social content has been measured, it correlated with emotional arousal and valence. Here we tested the association between social and emotional content orthogonally, by selecting a similar number of pictures in four categories varying in presence of nonverbal social cues (e.g., gestures, facial expression, body language) and emotional content (neutral, negative). Across two studies, participants (N = 698 in total) provided three ratings for each picture: social relevance (defined as the self-reported use of social cues to understand the picture), valence, and arousal. Despite our best effort to balance the presence of social cues between negative and neutral pictures, ratings of social relevance were strongly associated with ratings of arousal and, to a lesser extent, with valence. These findings likely reflect the intertwined nature of social and emotional processing, which has implications for the neurobiology underlying them, how these systems develop, and how picture databases are used in research.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2221-2233"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of evaluatively conditioned stimuli in iconic memory. 评价性条件刺激在图标记忆中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02024-w
Alexandra E Clausen, Florian Kattner

In the present study, an attempt was made to replicate results found about the influence of valence on prioritisation and decay in iconic memory. Hereby, the evaluative conditioning effect was used to induce valence for formerly neutral stimuli. The effect is gained by pairing neutral stimuli with either positive, negative, or neutral images in a conditioning phase. Afterwards, the conditioned stimuli acted as targets in an iconic memory test. In the iconic memory test, targets were presented together with seven other stimuli on a circular placement on the screen for a short time. A cue delayed by either 17, 68, 221, 493, or 1003 ms pointed at the target to be reported. Participants rated the targets before and after the conditioning phase. In addition, the affective and neutral images used in the pairing procedure were rated at the end of the experiment. While no significant change in rating could be observed for the conditioned targets, a significant effect of conditioned valence was still present in the response times and the accuracy of the iconic memory test. Participants reacted the quickest in response to a cue for positively conditioned targets compared to neutral or negatively conditioned targets. Accuracy was highest for positively conditioned targets and was lowest for negatively conditioned targets. Unlike in prior studies, slower decay of information in iconic memory for negative targets was not revealed. Further research should be conducted to identify reasons for this inconsistency.

在本研究中,我们试图复制有关情绪对图标记忆中优先级和衰减的影响的结果。因此,评价性条件反射效应被用来诱导以前的中性刺激产生价值。在条件反射阶段,通过将中性刺激与正面、负面或中性图像配对来获得这种效应。之后,条件刺激在图标记忆测试中充当目标。在图标记忆测试中,目标与其他七个刺激物一起出现在屏幕上的圆形位置上,时间很短。延迟 17、68、221、493 或 1003 毫秒的提示分别指向要报告的目标。受试者在调节阶段之前和之后对目标进行评分。此外,配对程序中使用的情感图像和中性图像也在实验结束时进行了评分。虽然对条件目标的评分没有观察到明显的变化,但在反应时间和图标记忆测试的准确性方面,条件情绪仍有明显的影响。与中性或负面条件目标相比,参与者对正面条件目标的提示反应最快。对积极条件目标的准确率最高,而对消极条件目标的准确率最低。与之前的研究不同,没有发现消极目标在图标记忆中的信息衰减较慢。我们应该开展进一步的研究来找出这种不一致的原因。
{"title":"The role of evaluatively conditioned stimuli in iconic memory.","authors":"Alexandra E Clausen, Florian Kattner","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02024-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02024-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the present study, an attempt was made to replicate results found about the influence of valence on prioritisation and decay in iconic memory. Hereby, the evaluative conditioning effect was used to induce valence for formerly neutral stimuli. The effect is gained by pairing neutral stimuli with either positive, negative, or neutral images in a conditioning phase. Afterwards, the conditioned stimuli acted as targets in an iconic memory test. In the iconic memory test, targets were presented together with seven other stimuli on a circular placement on the screen for a short time. A cue delayed by either 17, 68, 221, 493, or 1003 ms pointed at the target to be reported. Participants rated the targets before and after the conditioning phase. In addition, the affective and neutral images used in the pairing procedure were rated at the end of the experiment. While no significant change in rating could be observed for the conditioned targets, a significant effect of conditioned valence was still present in the response times and the accuracy of the iconic memory test. Participants reacted the quickest in response to a cue for positively conditioned targets compared to neutral or negatively conditioned targets. Accuracy was highest for positively conditioned targets and was lowest for negatively conditioned targets. Unlike in prior studies, slower decay of information in iconic memory for negative targets was not revealed. Further research should be conducted to identify reasons for this inconsistency.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2266-2279"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11522187/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not to follow because of distrust: perceived trust modulates the gaze cueing effect. 因不信任而不跟随:感知到的信任会调节凝视提示效应。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02000-4
Xiaowei Ding, Jing Gan, Luzi Xu, Xiaozhi Zhou, Ding-Guo Gao, Yanliang Sun

In social life, people spontaneously form stable trustworthiness impressions from faces. However, the precise role of extracting trustworthiness information remains unclear. This study aims to elucidate whether discerning facial trustworthiness influences social interactions. Specifically, it explores the gaze cueing effect (GCE), wherein individuals exhibit quicker responses to targets appearing in the direction of gaze compared to other locations. Given conflicting perspectives in existing literature regarding the potential modulation of trustworthiness on the GCE, two plausible hypotheses are proposed to explain divergent result patterns. The reflexive hypothesis posits that the GCE operates automatically. In contrast, the flexible hypothesis underscores the potential modulatory role of trustworthiness in the GCE. To provide a comprehensive understanding of whether trustworthiness modulates the GCE, we employed face stimuli incorporating trustworthiness information within Posner' s cue-target task. The findings of Experiment 1 revealed that the perception of trustworthiness indeed influenced the GCE. Specifically, when facial stimuli were perceived as trustworthy, they elicited a more pronounced GCE compared to untrustworthy stimuli. This modulation effect was replicated using a different stimulus set in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, we employed the same stimuli as in Experiment 2, setting the trustworthiness information to baseline as a control experiment. The results demonstrated that the trustworthiness modulation effect disappeared, indicating its specificity to the trustworthiness attribute of the stimuli rather than other characteristics. Collectively, these findings lend support to the flexible hypothesis, highlighting that the extraction of trustworthiness information plays a pivotal role in modulating the GCE, consequently influencing social interactions.

在社会生活中,人们会自发地从人脸中形成稳定的可信度印象。然而,提取可信度信息的确切作用仍不清楚。本研究旨在阐明辨别面部可信度是否会影响社交互动。具体来说,它探讨了注视提示效应(GCE),即个体对注视方向出现的目标比其他位置出现的目标反应更快。鉴于现有文献中关于信任度对 GCE 的潜在调节作用的观点相互冲突,我们提出了两个可信的假设来解释不同的结果模式。反射性假设认为 GCE 是自动运行的。与此相反,灵活假说强调了可信度在 GCE 中的潜在调节作用。为了全面了解可信度是否会调节GCE,我们在波斯纳的线索-目标任务中使用了包含可信度信息的人脸刺激。实验 1 的结果显示,对可信度的感知确实会影响 GCE。具体来说,当面部刺激被认为值得信赖时,它们会比不值得信赖的刺激引起更明显的 GCE。在实验 2 中,我们使用了不同的刺激集复制了这种调节效应。在实验 3 中,我们使用了与实验 2 相同的刺激,将可信度信息设置为基线作为对照实验。结果表明,可信度调节效应消失了,这表明它与刺激物的可信度属性而非其他特征有关。总之,这些研究结果支持了灵活假设,强调了可信度信息的提取在调节 GCE 方面起着关键作用,从而影响社会互动。
{"title":"Not to follow because of distrust: perceived trust modulates the gaze cueing effect.","authors":"Xiaowei Ding, Jing Gan, Luzi Xu, Xiaozhi Zhou, Ding-Guo Gao, Yanliang Sun","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02000-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02000-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In social life, people spontaneously form stable trustworthiness impressions from faces. However, the precise role of extracting trustworthiness information remains unclear. This study aims to elucidate whether discerning facial trustworthiness influences social interactions. Specifically, it explores the gaze cueing effect (GCE), wherein individuals exhibit quicker responses to targets appearing in the direction of gaze compared to other locations. Given conflicting perspectives in existing literature regarding the potential modulation of trustworthiness on the GCE, two plausible hypotheses are proposed to explain divergent result patterns. The reflexive hypothesis posits that the GCE operates automatically. In contrast, the flexible hypothesis underscores the potential modulatory role of trustworthiness in the GCE. To provide a comprehensive understanding of whether trustworthiness modulates the GCE, we employed face stimuli incorporating trustworthiness information within Posner' s cue-target task. The findings of Experiment 1 revealed that the perception of trustworthiness indeed influenced the GCE. Specifically, when facial stimuli were perceived as trustworthy, they elicited a more pronounced GCE compared to untrustworthy stimuli. This modulation effect was replicated using a different stimulus set in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, we employed the same stimuli as in Experiment 2, setting the trustworthiness information to baseline as a control experiment. The results demonstrated that the trustworthiness modulation effect disappeared, indicating its specificity to the trustworthiness attribute of the stimuli rather than other characteristics. Collectively, these findings lend support to the flexible hypothesis, highlighting that the extraction of trustworthiness information plays a pivotal role in modulating the GCE, consequently influencing social interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2195-2210"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493937","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attentional boost effect: research based on source memory and emotional materials. 注意增强效应:基于源记忆和情感材料的研究。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02016-w
Yajie Duan, Tongtong Xin, Yuanhua Yin, Ying Sun, Wei Liu, Ning Chen

The attentional boost effect (ABE) refers to the phenomenon that stimuli which appear with targets in a detection task are better remembered than those that appear with distractors. Previous studies have consistently reported a robust ABE in item memory, but inconsistent conclusions have been drawn for source memory. Additionally, regarding the impact of emotional stimuli on the ABE, conclusions have also been inconsistent. The aim of this research was to clarify these inconsistencies. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to memorize different emotional background words (primary task), monitor the symbols above the words, press the spacebar when encountering the "+" (secondary task), and remember the size of the emotional background stimuli (as a source feature). Results revealed that the ABE of negative stimuli was stronger in item memory. For source memory, an ABE was observed only for large fonts. In Experiment 2, participants performed the same task as in Experiment 1, except for recalling the color of emotional stimuli instead of their size. Results indicated a stronger ABE for emotional stimuli in item memory, with no ABE observed in source memory. These findings suggest: (1) Item and source memory are regulated by distinct cognitive processes, leading to differential effects of emotionality on ABE in both types of memory. (2) Contrary to previous literature, emotional stimuli, such as negative words, do not consistently diminish the ABE.

注意增强效应(ABE)是指在检测任务中与目标同时出现的刺激物比与干扰物同时出现的刺激物更容易被记住的现象。以往的研究一直报告在项目记忆中存在强烈的注意力增强效应,但在源记忆中却得出了不一致的结论。此外,关于情绪刺激对 ABE 的影响,结论也不一致。本研究旨在澄清这些不一致之处。在实验 1 中,受试者被要求记忆不同情绪背景的单词(主要任务)、监视单词上方的符号、遇到 "+"时按空格键(次要任务)以及记忆情绪背景刺激的大小(作为源特征)。结果显示,在项目记忆中,负面刺激的 ABE 更强。而在源记忆中,只有大字体才会出现 ABE。在实验 2 中,受试者完成了与实验 1 相同的任务,只是回忆情绪刺激的颜色而不是大小。结果表明,在项目记忆中,情绪刺激的 ABE 更强,而在源记忆中没有观察到 ABE。这些发现表明:(1) 单项记忆和源记忆由不同的认知过程调节,导致情绪对这两种类型记忆的 ABE 产生不同的影响。(2)与之前的文献相反,情绪刺激(如负面词语)并不会持续减少 ABE。
{"title":"Attentional boost effect: research based on source memory and emotional materials.","authors":"Yajie Duan, Tongtong Xin, Yuanhua Yin, Ying Sun, Wei Liu, Ning Chen","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02016-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02016-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The attentional boost effect (ABE) refers to the phenomenon that stimuli which appear with targets in a detection task are better remembered than those that appear with distractors. Previous studies have consistently reported a robust ABE in item memory, but inconsistent conclusions have been drawn for source memory. Additionally, regarding the impact of emotional stimuli on the ABE, conclusions have also been inconsistent. The aim of this research was to clarify these inconsistencies. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to memorize different emotional background words (primary task), monitor the symbols above the words, press the spacebar when encountering the \"+\" (secondary task), and remember the size of the emotional background stimuli (as a source feature). Results revealed that the ABE of negative stimuli was stronger in item memory. For source memory, an ABE was observed only for large fonts. In Experiment 2, participants performed the same task as in Experiment 1, except for recalling the color of emotional stimuli instead of their size. Results indicated a stronger ABE for emotional stimuli in item memory, with no ABE observed in source memory. These findings suggest: (1) Item and source memory are regulated by distinct cognitive processes, leading to differential effects of emotionality on ABE in both types of memory. (2) Contrary to previous literature, emotional stimuli, such as negative words, do not consistently diminish the ABE.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2252-2265"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142082201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How do emotions respond to outcome values and influence choice? 情绪如何对结果价值做出反应并影响选择?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02001-3
Aikaterini Grimani, Ayse Yemiscigil, Qing Wang, Georgi Kirilov, Laura Kudrna, Ivo Vlaev

Emotions powerfully, predictably, and pervasively influence decision making. The risk-as-feelings hypothesis states that two kinds of emotions are important in decision-making, anticipatory emotions and anticipated emotions. We empirically investigated whether and how anticipatory and anticipated emotions may change as a function of outcome values and whether anticipatory or anticipated emotions may explain the influence of outcome values on risky choice. To study the effects of value on emotions and choice, we offered people hypothetical large amounts ($100, $200, $300, $400) and incentivized moderate amounts ($10, $20, $30, $40) as prospects in gambles over two consecutive studies. Using a representative sample from the US to ensure the generalizability of the findings, each participant in our two studies made choices in gain and loss domains. Overall, anticipatory and anticipated emotions responded very similarly to changes in value for the sure gains in both studies. The findings also indicated that both anticipatory and anticipated emotions explained the effects of the value on choice for the sure gain and sure losses, while both mediated the effect of framing on choice towards the sure and the gamble option. Although anticipatory emotions mediated a larger portion of the effect, anticipated emotions also show some mediation.

情绪对决策具有强大的、可预测的和普遍的影响。风险即感觉假说认为,有两种情绪在决策中非常重要,即预期情绪和预期情绪。我们通过实证研究探讨了预期情绪和预期情绪是否会随着结果价值的变化而变化,以及预期情绪和预期情绪是如何解释结果价值对风险选择的影响的。为了研究价值对情绪和选择的影响,我们在连续两次研究中向人们提供了假设的大额金额(100 美元、200 美元、300 美元、400 美元)和激励的中等金额(10 美元、20 美元、30 美元、40 美元)作为赌博的前景。为了确保研究结果的普遍性,我们在两项研究中使用了来自美国的代表性样本,每个参与者都在收益和损失领域做出了选择。总体而言,在这两项研究中,预期情绪和预期情绪对确定收益的价值变化的反应非常相似。研究结果还表明,预期情绪和预期情感都能解释价值对确定收益和确定损失选择的影响,同时两者都能调节框架对确定和赌博选择的影响。虽然预期情绪在较大程度上起到了中介作用,但预期情绪也起到了一定的中介作用。
{"title":"How do emotions respond to outcome values and influence choice?","authors":"Aikaterini Grimani, Ayse Yemiscigil, Qing Wang, Georgi Kirilov, Laura Kudrna, Ivo Vlaev","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02001-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02001-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotions powerfully, predictably, and pervasively influence decision making. The risk-as-feelings hypothesis states that two kinds of emotions are important in decision-making, anticipatory emotions and anticipated emotions. We empirically investigated whether and how anticipatory and anticipated emotions may change as a function of outcome values and whether anticipatory or anticipated emotions may explain the influence of outcome values on risky choice. To study the effects of value on emotions and choice, we offered people hypothetical large amounts ($100, $200, $300, $400) and incentivized moderate amounts ($10, $20, $30, $40) as prospects in gambles over two consecutive studies. Using a representative sample from the US to ensure the generalizability of the findings, each participant in our two studies made choices in gain and loss domains. Overall, anticipatory and anticipated emotions responded very similarly to changes in value for the sure gains in both studies. The findings also indicated that both anticipatory and anticipated emotions explained the effects of the value on choice for the sure gain and sure losses, while both mediated the effect of framing on choice towards the sure and the gamble option. Although anticipatory emotions mediated a larger portion of the effect, anticipated emotions also show some mediation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2234-2250"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11522079/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564837","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: How do emotions respond to outcome values and influence choice? 更正:情绪如何对结果价值做出反应并影响选择?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02027-7
Aikaterini Grimani, Ayse Yemiscigil, Qing Wang, Georgi Kirilov, Laura Kudrna, Ivo Vlaev
{"title":"Correction: How do emotions respond to outcome values and influence choice?","authors":"Aikaterini Grimani, Ayse Yemiscigil, Qing Wang, Georgi Kirilov, Laura Kudrna, Ivo Vlaev","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02027-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02027-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2251"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11522114/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477922","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How well can young adults and children discriminate between odors? 青壮年和儿童分辨气味的能力如何?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6
E Leslie Cameron, Sierra R Follett, Autumn S Rajcevich Schwer

It is widely believed that people can distinguish between many odors although there is limited empirical evidence. Odor discrimination tasks are employed much less often than other measures of olfaction, but, interestingly, performance is typically ~ 75% correct. This less-than-perfect performance is rarely highlighted, although it suggests that people may not be as good at discriminating odors as is commonly believed. Odor discrimination is understudied in children, and although available evidence suggests that it improves with age, children perform better when the task is simpler. In the present study, we explored odor discrimination in children and young adults with a relatively simple same-different task using common and uncommon odors. We found that children perform as well as young adults, but that overall performance was less than perfect and depended on the odors to be discriminated. We found evidence that ability to discriminate between odors improves as the difference in pleasantness of the odors increases. In a second experiment, we tested this directly by exploring whether odors that differ in pleasantness and edibility, two dimensions that appear to be important in olfactory perception, are easier to discriminate than odors that are the same on those dimensions. We found further evidence that odors that differ in pleasantness are easier to discriminate.

尽管经验证据有限,但人们普遍认为人可以区分多种气味。与其他嗅觉测量方法相比,气味辨别任务的使用频率要低得多,但有趣的是,正确率通常在 75% 左右。这种不尽如人意的表现很少被强调,但它表明人们对气味的辨别能力可能并不像人们通常认为的那样好。对儿童气味辨别能力的研究不足,尽管现有证据表明,儿童的辨别能力会随着年龄的增长而提高,但当任务较为简单时,儿童的表现会更好。在本研究中,我们通过一个相对简单的使用常见和不常见气味的 "同-异 "任务,对儿童和青少年的气味辨别能力进行了探索。我们发现,儿童的表现与青壮年一样好,但总体表现并不完美,这取决于要辨别的气味。我们发现有证据表明,对不同气味的辨别能力会随着气味的愉悦程度差异的增加而提高。在第二个实验中,我们直接测试了这一点,探讨了在宜人性和可食性这两个似乎对嗅觉感知很重要的维度上存在差异的气味是否比在这两个维度上相同的气味更容易分辨。我们发现有进一步的证据表明,愉悦度不同的气味更容易辨别。
{"title":"How well can young adults and children discriminate between odors?","authors":"E Leslie Cameron, Sierra R Follett, Autumn S Rajcevich Schwer","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is widely believed that people can distinguish between many odors although there is limited empirical evidence. Odor discrimination tasks are employed much less often than other measures of olfaction, but, interestingly, performance is typically ~ 75% correct. This less-than-perfect performance is rarely highlighted, although it suggests that people may not be as good at discriminating odors as is commonly believed. Odor discrimination is understudied in children, and although available evidence suggests that it improves with age, children perform better when the task is simpler. In the present study, we explored odor discrimination in children and young adults with a relatively simple same-different task using common and uncommon odors. We found that children perform as well as young adults, but that overall performance was less than perfect and depended on the odors to be discriminated. We found evidence that ability to discriminate between odors improves as the difference in pleasantness of the odors increases. In a second experiment, we tested this directly by exploring whether odors that differ in pleasantness and edibility, two dimensions that appear to be important in olfactory perception, are easier to discriminate than odors that are the same on those dimensions. We found further evidence that odors that differ in pleasantness are easier to discriminate.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2360-2375"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strategy variability in computational estimation and its association with mathematical achievement. 计算估算中的策略变化及其与数学成绩的关系。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w
Reba Koenen, Sashank Varma

Computational estimation requires a breadth of strategies and selection of the relevant strategy given a problem's features. We used the new Test of Estimation Strategies (TES), composed of 20 arithmetic problems (e.g., 144 x 0.38), to investigate variability in strategy use in young adults. The TES targets the five estimation strategies that adults use most frequently, which fall into two Classes. The three Class One strategies are general-purpose and taught in schools. Proceed Algorithmically entails applying an algorithm (e.g., shifting a decimal place). Round One and Round Two are defined as rounding one or both operands, respectively. The two Class Two strategies are more advanced, requiring application of conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Known-and-Nice is used when a participant relies on a well-known mathematical fact (e.g., 25 × 4 = 100) to form an estimate. Fractions uses a fraction or percentage in the estimation process (e.g., 943 x 0.48 is about 50% or half of 900). We divided our sample of adult participants into two groups (i.e., high, average) based on their estimation performance on the TES. The high-performance group used a broader range of strategies and more frequently applied the most relevant strategy given a problem's features. Overall estimation accuracy was correlated with mathematical achievement, as were strategy breadth and strategy relevance. However, none of these associations survived first controlling for verbal achievement. Participants' strategy reports suggested that the TES problems were generally successful in eliciting the five target strategies and provided evidence for a new strategy, Partitioning. These findings provide a basis for future instructional studies to improve students' computational estimation.

计算估算需要多种策略,并根据问题的特征选择相关策略。我们使用了由 20 个算术问题(如 144 x 0.38)组成的新估算策略测试 (TES),以研究青壮年在策略使用方面的变异性。估算策略测试针对成年人最常用的五种估算策略,这些策略分为两类。第一类的三种策略是通用策略,在学校中教授。按照算法进行估算需要应用一种算法(例如,移动一位小数)。四舍五入(Round One)和四舍五入(Round Two)分别指对一个操作数或两个操作数进行四舍五入。第二类的两种策略更高级,需要应用数学概念知识。当学员根据一个众所周知的数学事实(如 25 × 4 = 100)来进行估算时,就会使用 "已知并不错 "策略。分数在估算过程中使用分数或百分比(例如,943 x 0.48 大约是 900 的 50%或一半)。我们根据成人参与者在 TES 中的估算成绩将其分为两组(即高分组和普通组)。成绩优秀组使用的策略范围更广,并且更经常地根据问题的特点使用最相关的策略。总体估算准确率与数学成绩相关,策略广度和策略相关性也与数学成绩相关。然而,在控制了言语成绩之后,这些相关性都不复存在。参与者的策略报告表明,TES 问题总体上成功地激发了五种目标策略,并为一种新策略--"分割 "提供了证据。这些发现为今后提高学生计算估算能力的教学研究提供了依据。
{"title":"Strategy variability in computational estimation and its association with mathematical achievement.","authors":"Reba Koenen, Sashank Varma","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Computational estimation requires a breadth of strategies and selection of the relevant strategy given a problem's features. We used the new Test of Estimation Strategies (TES), composed of 20 arithmetic problems (e.g., 144 x 0.38), to investigate variability in strategy use in young adults. The TES targets the five estimation strategies that adults use most frequently, which fall into two Classes. The three Class One strategies are general-purpose and taught in schools. Proceed Algorithmically entails applying an algorithm (e.g., shifting a decimal place). Round One and Round Two are defined as rounding one or both operands, respectively. The two Class Two strategies are more advanced, requiring application of conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Known-and-Nice is used when a participant relies on a well-known mathematical fact (e.g., 25 × 4 = 100) to form an estimate. Fractions uses a fraction or percentage in the estimation process (e.g., 943 x 0.48 is about 50% or half of 900). We divided our sample of adult participants into two groups (i.e., high, average) based on their estimation performance on the TES. The high-performance group used a broader range of strategies and more frequently applied the most relevant strategy given a problem's features. Overall estimation accuracy was correlated with mathematical achievement, as were strategy breadth and strategy relevance. However, none of these associations survived first controlling for verbal achievement. Participants' strategy reports suggested that the TES problems were generally successful in eliciting the five target strategies and provided evidence for a new strategy, Partitioning. These findings provide a basis for future instructional studies to improve students' computational estimation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2303-2319"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976895","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association between physical activity and memory interference. 体育锻炼与记忆干扰之间的关系
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-24 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z
Paul D Loprinzi, Myungjin Jung, Monika Undorf

Habitual physical activity has been shown to improve memory performance, yet investigations into its effects concerning memory interference remain limited. Additionally, minimal research has evaluated the association between habitual physical activity behaviors occurring in different contexts (e.g., walking, basketball, swimming) and memory. Based on these gaps in the literature, the present set of six experiments evaluated the association between contextually-different physical activity behaviors (e.g., individual physical activities, physical activities performed in social settings) and memory interference among young adult samples from America and Germany. Across six experiments, we reliably demonstrated that Germans exhibited greater memory performance than Americans. We also reliably demonstrated that contextually-different physical activities are not associated with memory performance or attenuated memory interference.

习惯性体育锻炼已被证明能提高记忆力,但对其对记忆干扰的影响的研究仍然有限。此外,很少有研究对不同情境下的习惯性体育锻炼行为(如散步、篮球、游泳)与记忆之间的关联进行评估。基于这些文献空白,本组六项实验评估了来自美国和德国的年轻成人样本中不同情境下的体育活动行为(如个人体育活动、在社交场合进行的体育活动)与记忆干扰之间的关联。通过六项实验,我们可靠地证明德国人比美国人表现出更强的记忆能力。我们还可靠地证明,不同情境下的体育活动与记忆表现或记忆干扰的减弱无关。
{"title":"The association between physical activity and memory interference.","authors":"Paul D Loprinzi, Myungjin Jung, Monika Undorf","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Habitual physical activity has been shown to improve memory performance, yet investigations into its effects concerning memory interference remain limited. Additionally, minimal research has evaluated the association between habitual physical activity behaviors occurring in different contexts (e.g., walking, basketball, swimming) and memory. Based on these gaps in the literature, the present set of six experiments evaluated the association between contextually-different physical activity behaviors (e.g., individual physical activities, physical activities performed in social settings) and memory interference among young adult samples from America and Germany. Across six experiments, we reliably demonstrated that Germans exhibited greater memory performance than Americans. We also reliably demonstrated that contextually-different physical activities are not associated with memory performance or attenuated memory interference.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2280-2291"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142047313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic. Covid-19 大流行期间的时间意识改变。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0
Şerife Leman Runyun, Virginie van Wassenhove, Fuat Balci

Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic had profound effects on human well-being. A handful of studies have focused on how time perception was altered during the COVID-19 pandemic, while no study has tested whether temporal metacognition is also affected by the lockdown. We examined the impact of long-term social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic on the ability to monitor errors in timing performance. We recruited 1232 participants from 12 countries during lockdown, 211 of which were retested "post-pandemic" for within-group comparisons. We also tested a new group of 331 participants during the "post-pandemic" period and compared their data to those of 1232 participants tested during the lockdown (between-group comparison). Participants produced a 3600 ms target interval and assessed the magnitude and direction of their time production error. Both within and between-group comparisons showed reduced metric error monitoring performance during the lockdown, even after controlling for government-imposed stringency indices. A higher level of reported social isolation also predicted reduced temporal error monitoring ability. Participants produced longer duration during lockdown compared to post-lockdown (again controlling for government stringency indices). We reason that these effects may be underlain by altered biological and behavioral rhythms during social isolation experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding these effects is crucial for a more complete characterization of the cognitive consequences of long-term social isolation.

COVID-19 大流行期间的社会隔离对人类福祉产生了深远影响。少数研究关注的是 COVID-19 大流行期间时间感知是如何改变的,但还没有研究测试时间元认知是否也受到封锁的影响。我们研究了 COVID-19 大流行期间长期社会隔离对监测计时错误能力的影响。在封锁期间,我们从 12 个国家招募了 1232 名参与者,其中 211 人在 "大流行后 "接受了重新测试,以进行组内比较。我们还在 "大流行后 "期间测试了一组新的 331 名参与者,并将他们的数据与封锁期间测试的 1232 名参与者的数据进行了比较(组间比较)。参与者制作了一个 3600 毫秒的目标时间间隔,并对其时间制作误差的大小和方向进行了评估。组内比较和组间比较均显示,即使在控制了政府规定的严格指数后,封锁期间的度量误差监测表现仍有所下降。较高的社会隔离水平也预示着较低的时间误差监测能力。与封锁后相比,参与者在封锁期间产生了更长的持续时间(同样控制了政府的严格指数)。我们推断,这些影响可能是由于 COVID-19 大流行期间经历的社会隔离改变了生物和行为节律。了解这些影响对于更全面地描述长期社会隔离的认知后果至关重要。
{"title":"Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic.","authors":"Şerife Leman Runyun, Virginie van Wassenhove, Fuat Balci","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic had profound effects on human well-being. A handful of studies have focused on how time perception was altered during the COVID-19 pandemic, while no study has tested whether temporal metacognition is also affected by the lockdown. We examined the impact of long-term social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic on the ability to monitor errors in timing performance. We recruited 1232 participants from 12 countries during lockdown, 211 of which were retested \"post-pandemic\" for within-group comparisons. We also tested a new group of 331 participants during the \"post-pandemic\" period and compared their data to those of 1232 participants tested during the lockdown (between-group comparison). Participants produced a 3600 ms target interval and assessed the magnitude and direction of their time production error. Both within and between-group comparisons showed reduced metric error monitoring performance during the lockdown, even after controlling for government-imposed stringency indices. A higher level of reported social isolation also predicted reduced temporal error monitoring ability. Participants produced longer duration during lockdown compared to post-lockdown (again controlling for government stringency indices). We reason that these effects may be underlain by altered biological and behavioral rhythms during social isolation experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding these effects is crucial for a more complete characterization of the cognitive consequences of long-term social isolation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2335-2345"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1