首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Dynamics between affect and social acceptance as a function of social anxiety: A person-specific network approach. 作为社交焦虑的函数,情感与社会接受之间的动态关系:特定人群网络方法。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001370
Kelley E Gunther, Audrey Edelman, Daniel Petrie, Hedy Kober, Dylan G Gee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack

Social acceptance and rejection are salient experiences, especially during adolescence. Acceptance and rejection relate to changes in positive and negative affect, although directionality of the relation remains unclear. The ability to regulate affect following social experiences may be part of the etiology of social anxiety disorder. With the importance of social cues in adolescence, as well as adolescence as a key window for the onset of social anxiety, we used daily diary data collected in a sample ranging from 9 to 18 years to examine daily changes in acceptance, rejection, positive affect, and negative affect. Taking a person-centered approach, we constructed networks directionally linking social experiences and affect, which served as behaviors of interest ("nodes") in the network for each individual. From these networks, we extracted recovery times from different nodes, that is, the number of days it took for a node to return to baseline when (a) the node itself was perturbed and (b) when a connected node was perturbed. We examined associations between network metrics and social anxiety, age, gender, and their interaction. We found that the recovery time of positive affect when social acceptance was perturbed was inversely related with social anxiety and age, suggesting benefits of acceptance may be shorter lasting for those with more (vs. less) social anxiety symptoms and for older (vs. younger) adolescents. We conclude that positive affect may be a critical yet understudied piece in understanding why adolescence is a developmental period of increased risk for psychopathology and for understanding the etiology of social anxiety disorder. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

社会接纳和拒绝是一种突出的经历,尤其是在青春期。接受和拒绝与积极和消极情绪的变化有关,但这种关系的方向性仍不明确。社交经历后的情绪调节能力可能是社交焦虑症的病因之一。鉴于社交线索在青春期的重要性,以及青春期是社交焦虑症发病的关键窗口期,我们利用从 9 岁到 18 岁的样本中收集的每日日记数据,研究了接受、拒绝、积极情绪和消极情绪的每日变化。我们采用以人为本的方法,构建了将社交经历和情感定向联系起来的网络,作为每个人在网络中感兴趣的行为("节点")。从这些网络中,我们提取了不同节点的恢复时间,即当(a)节点本身受到干扰和(b)连接节点受到干扰时,节点恢复到基线所需的天数。我们研究了网络指标与社交焦虑、年龄、性别及其交互作用之间的关联。我们发现,当社会接纳受到干扰时,积极情绪的恢复时间与社会焦虑和年龄成反比,这表明对于社会焦虑症状较多(与较少)的人和年龄较大(与较小)的青少年来说,接纳带来的益处可能持续时间较短。我们的结论是,积极情绪可能是理解青春期为何是心理病理学风险增加的发育期以及理解社交焦虑症病因的一个关键但未被充分研究的部分。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Dynamics between affect and social acceptance as a function of social anxiety: A person-specific network approach.","authors":"Kelley E Gunther, Audrey Edelman, Daniel Petrie, Hedy Kober, Dylan G Gee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack","doi":"10.1037/emo0001370","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social acceptance and rejection are salient experiences, especially during adolescence. Acceptance and rejection relate to changes in positive and negative affect, although directionality of the relation remains unclear. The ability to regulate affect following social experiences may be part of the etiology of social anxiety disorder. With the importance of social cues in adolescence, as well as adolescence as a key window for the onset of social anxiety, we used daily diary data collected in a sample ranging from 9 to 18 years to examine daily changes in acceptance, rejection, positive affect, and negative affect. Taking a person-centered approach, we constructed networks directionally linking social experiences and affect, which served as behaviors of interest (\"nodes\") in the network for each individual. From these networks, we extracted recovery times from different nodes, that is, the number of days it took for a node to return to baseline when (a) the node itself was perturbed and (b) when a connected node was perturbed. We examined associations between network metrics and social anxiety, age, gender, and their interaction. We found that the recovery time of positive affect when social acceptance was perturbed was inversely related with social anxiety and age, suggesting benefits of acceptance may be shorter lasting for those with more (vs. less) social anxiety symptoms and for older (vs. younger) adolescents. We conclude that positive affect may be a critical yet understudied piece in understanding why adolescence is a developmental period of increased risk for psychopathology and for understanding the etiology of social anxiety disorder. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"46-56"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What makes us feel good? A data-driven investigation of positive emotion experience. 是什么让我们感觉良好?对积极情绪体验的数据驱动调查。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001417
Roza G Kamiloğlu, İnan Utku Türkmen, Taha Eren Sarnıç, Dana Landman, Disa A Sauter

What does it mean to feel good? Is our experience of gazing in awe at a majestic mountain fundamentally different than erupting with triumph when our favorite team wins the championship? Here, we use a semantic space approach to test which positive emotional experiences are distinct from each other based on in-depth personal narratives of experiences involving 22 positive emotions (n = 165; 3,592 emotional events). A bottom-up computational analysis was applied to the transcribed text, with unsupervised clustering employed to maximize internal granular consistency (i.e., the clusters being maximally different and maximally internally homogeneous). The analysis yielded four emotions that map onto distinct clusters of subjective experiences: amusement, interest, lust, and tenderness. The application of the semantic space approach to in-depth personal accounts yields a nuanced understanding of positive emotional experiences. Moreover, this analytical method allows for the bottom-up development of emotion taxonomies, showcasing its potential for broader applications in the study of subjective experiences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

感觉良好意味着什么?我们对雄伟高山的敬畏之情与我们最喜爱的球队夺冠时的喜悦之情是否有本质区别?在此,我们使用语义空间方法,根据涉及 22 种积极情绪的个人深度叙述(n = 165;3,592 个情绪事件),测试哪些积极情绪体验彼此不同。我们对转录文本进行了自下而上的计算分析,并采用了无监督聚类,以最大限度地提高内部粒度一致性(即聚类具有最大程度的差异和最大程度的内部同质性)。分析得出了四种情绪,分别映射到主观体验的不同聚类上:娱乐、兴趣、欲望和温柔。将语义空间方法应用于深入的个人陈述,可以获得对积极情绪体验的细致入微的理解。此外,这种分析方法允许自下而上地发展情绪分类法,展示了其在主观体验研究中更广泛应用的潜力。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"What makes us feel good? A data-driven investigation of positive emotion experience.","authors":"Roza G Kamiloğlu, İnan Utku Türkmen, Taha Eren Sarnıç, Dana Landman, Disa A Sauter","doi":"10.1037/emo0001417","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What does it mean to feel good? Is our experience of gazing in awe at a majestic mountain fundamentally different than erupting with triumph when our favorite team wins the championship? Here, we use a semantic space approach to test which positive emotional experiences are distinct from each other based on in-depth personal narratives of experiences involving 22 positive emotions (<i>n</i> = 165; 3,592 emotional events). A bottom-up computational analysis was applied to the transcribed text, with unsupervised clustering employed to maximize internal granular consistency (i.e., the clusters being maximally different and maximally internally homogeneous). The analysis yielded four emotions that map onto distinct clusters of subjective experiences: amusement, interest, lust, and tenderness. The application of the semantic space approach to in-depth personal accounts yields a nuanced understanding of positive emotional experiences. Moreover, this analytical method allows for the bottom-up development of emotion taxonomies, showcasing its potential for broader applications in the study of subjective experiences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"271-276"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Hot" affect-related aspects in emotional information processing: The role of facial muscle responses in the direct and indirect processing of emotion categories. 情绪信息处理中与情绪相关的 "热门 "方面:面部肌肉反应在情绪类别的直接和间接处理中的作用。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001414
Michaela Rohr, Timea Folyi, Dirk Wentura

The present study investigated the involvement of facial muscle responses in the indirect and direct processing of emotional facial expressions. Five discrete emotion categories were used, and we assessed both facial muscle and behavioral responses on a trial-by-trial basis. Experiment 1 tested facial muscle activation of clearly visible stimuli in an emotion categorization task. We observed emotion-specific facial muscle responses and corresponding behavioral categorization effects, providing evidence for the specificity of facial muscle activation. By contrast, under masked indirect presentation conditions in which emotional facial expressions were presented as primes in an emotion misattribution procedure, a specific pattern of emotion-congruent and cross-category behavioral misattributions was observed (in line with Rohr et al., 2015, 2018). Multilevel analyses in Study 2 suggest that an emotional reaction feeds into the behavioral decision, as indicated by differential activation of the frontalis lateralis in response to angry faces. Thus, the present study provides evidence that facial muscle responses contribute to behavioral decisions under masked indirect processing conditions. The different pattern of effects in both studies suggests that facial muscle responses index different processes, depending on the processing conditions: sensorimotor simulation in direct processing conditions and emotional reactions in masked indirect processing conditions. We discuss the implications for models that aim to account for facial muscle activity in response to emotional facial expressions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究调查了面部肌肉反应在情绪面部表情的间接和直接加工中的参与情况。我们使用了五个离散的情绪类别,并在逐次试验的基础上评估了面部肌肉和行为反应。实验 1 测试了情绪分类任务中清晰可见的刺激物对面部肌肉的激活作用。我们观察到了特定情绪的面部肌肉反应和相应的行为分类效应,为面部肌肉激活的特异性提供了证据。与此相反,在情绪错误归因程序中,情绪面部表情被作为素材呈现的遮蔽间接呈现条件下,我们观察到了情绪一致和跨类别行为错误归因的特定模式(与 Rohr 等人的研究一致,2015 年、2018 年)。研究 2 中的多层次分析表明,情绪反应会影响行为决策,这一点从额叶外侧肌对愤怒面孔的不同激活可以看出。因此,本研究提供的证据表明,在被遮蔽的间接处理条件下,面部肌肉反应有助于行为决策。这两项研究中不同的效应模式表明,面部肌肉反应会根据处理条件的不同而指示不同的过程:直接处理条件下的感觉运动模拟和掩蔽间接处理条件下的情绪反应。我们讨论了这些研究对旨在解释面部肌肉活动对情绪面部表情反应的模型的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"\"Hot\" affect-related aspects in emotional information processing: The role of facial muscle responses in the direct and indirect processing of emotion categories.","authors":"Michaela Rohr, Timea Folyi, Dirk Wentura","doi":"10.1037/emo0001414","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001414","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated the involvement of facial muscle responses in the indirect and direct processing of emotional facial expressions. Five discrete emotion categories were used, and we assessed both facial muscle and behavioral responses on a trial-by-trial basis. Experiment 1 tested facial muscle activation of clearly visible stimuli in an emotion categorization task. We observed emotion-specific facial muscle responses and corresponding behavioral categorization effects, providing evidence for the specificity of facial muscle activation. By contrast, under masked indirect presentation conditions in which emotional facial expressions were presented as primes in an emotion misattribution procedure, a specific pattern of emotion-congruent and cross-category behavioral misattributions was observed (in line with Rohr et al., 2015, 2018). Multilevel analyses in Study 2 suggest that an emotional reaction feeds into the behavioral decision, as indicated by differential activation of the frontalis lateralis in response to angry faces. Thus, the present study provides evidence that facial muscle responses contribute to behavioral decisions under masked indirect processing conditions. The different pattern of effects in both studies suggests that facial muscle responses index different processes, depending on the processing conditions: sensorimotor simulation in direct processing conditions and emotional reactions in masked indirect processing conditions. We discuss the implications for models that aim to account for facial muscle activity in response to emotional facial expressions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"227-246"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-channel adaptation reveals shared emotion representation from face and biological motion. 跨通道适应揭示了人脸和生物运动的共同情绪表征。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001409
Tian Yuan, Li Wang, Yi Jiang

Emotions in interpersonal interactions can be communicated simultaneously via various social signals such as face and biological motion (BM). Here, we demonstrate that even though BM and face are very different in visual properties, emotions conveyed by these two types of social signals involve dedicated and common processing mechanisms (N = 168, college students, 2020-2024). By utilizing the visual adaptation paradigm, we found that prolonged exposure to the happy BM biased the emotion perception of the subsequently presented morphed BM toward sad, and vice versus. The observed aftereffect disappeared when the BM adaptors were shown inverted, indicating that it arose from emotional information processing rather than being a result of adaptation to constitutive low-level features. Besides, such an aftereffect was also found for facial expressions and similarly vanished when the face adaptors were inverted. Critically, preexposure to emotional faces also exerted an adaptation aftereffect on the emotion perception of BMs. Furthermore, this cross-channel effect could not only happen from faces to BMs but also from BMs to faces, suggesting that emotion perception from face and BM are potentially driven by common underlying neural substrates. Overall, these findings highlighted a close coupling of BM and face emotion perception and suggested the existence of a dedicated emotional representation that can be shared across these two different types of social signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

人际交往中的情绪可以通过脸部和生物动作(BM)等各种社交信号同时传达。在这里,我们证明了尽管生物运动和人脸在视觉特性上有很大不同,但这两类社交信号所传达的情绪涉及到专门的和共同的处理机制(N = 168,大学生,2020-2024 年)。通过使用视觉适应范式,我们发现长时间接触快乐的BM会使随后出现的变形BM的情绪感知偏向悲伤,反之亦然。当BM适配器倒置显示时,观察到的后效应消失了,这表明后效应产生于情绪信息处理,而不是对构成性低级特征的适应结果。此外,在面部表情中也发现了这种后效,当面部适配器倒置时,这种后效也同样消失了。重要的是,预先暴露于情绪化的面孔也会对BM的情绪感知产生适应后效。此外,这种跨通道效应不仅发生在人脸到生物标记物之间,也发生在生物标记物到人脸之间,这表明人脸和生物标记物的情绪感知可能是由共同的潜在神经基质驱动的。总之,这些发现凸显了BM和人脸情绪感知的密切联系,并表明存在一种专用的情绪表征,可以在这两种不同类型的社会信号中共享。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Cross-channel adaptation reveals shared emotion representation from face and biological motion.","authors":"Tian Yuan, Li Wang, Yi Jiang","doi":"10.1037/emo0001409","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001409","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotions in interpersonal interactions can be communicated simultaneously via various social signals such as face and biological motion (BM). Here, we demonstrate that even though BM and face are very different in visual properties, emotions conveyed by these two types of social signals involve dedicated and common processing mechanisms (<i>N</i> = 168, college students, 2020-2024). By utilizing the visual adaptation paradigm, we found that prolonged exposure to the happy BM biased the emotion perception of the subsequently presented morphed BM toward sad, and vice versus. The observed aftereffect disappeared when the BM adaptors were shown inverted, indicating that it arose from emotional information processing rather than being a result of adaptation to constitutive low-level features. Besides, such an aftereffect was also found for facial expressions and similarly vanished when the face adaptors were inverted. Critically, preexposure to emotional faces also exerted an adaptation aftereffect on the emotion perception of BMs. Furthermore, this cross-channel effect could not only happen from faces to BMs but also from BMs to faces, suggesting that emotion perception from face and BM are potentially driven by common underlying neural substrates. Overall, these findings highlighted a close coupling of BM and face emotion perception and suggested the existence of a dedicated emotional representation that can be shared across these two different types of social signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"158-173"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Induced negative affect hinders self-referential belief updating in response to social feedback. 诱发的负面情绪会阻碍对社会反馈的自我参照信念更新。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001426
Tobias Kube, Christoph Korn

When people receive feedback from others, this is an opportunity for them to update their self-views. People with mental health problems (e.g., depression), however, often have difficulty using social feedback to update negative beliefs about themselves. To better understand when and how difficulties with integrating social feedback manifest, we investigated how current affect influences social feedback processing. Our preregistered hypothesis was that negative affect hinders change in participants' self-views in response to social feedback. In a nonclinical sample of little diversity (N = 117) in 2023, participants were invited to a laboratory examination in groups of three-five people. After indicating how they thought about themselves in terms of a number of personality traits (e.g., friendly), participants played a popular parlor game together for 45 min. Subsequently, they indicated how they perceived the other players in terms of their personality. Before receiving anonymous feedback, suggesting that the others perceived them as a highly likeable person, participants underwent the induction of negative versus positive affect versus a neutral control procedure. The results show that the induction of negative affect before receiving social feedback hindered its integration into participants' self-views, relative to the induction of positive affect. Changes in participants' self-views remained relatively stable also 1 day later, except for the control group, in which it slightly declined. These findings confirm that negative affect can indeed hamper the integration of (positive) social feedback. Since negative affect is prevalent in many mental disorders, this might contribute to their problems with social feedback processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

当人们收到来自他人的反馈时,这是他们更新自我看法的一个机会。然而,有心理健康问题(如抑郁症)的人往往很难利用社交反馈来更新对自己的负面看法。为了更好地了解整合社会反馈的困难何时以及如何表现出来,我们研究了当前的情绪如何影响社会反馈处理。我们预先设定的假设是,消极情绪会阻碍参与者根据社会反馈改变自我观点。在 2023 年的一个小多样性非临床样本(N = 117)中,参与者被邀请参加三至五人一组的实验室检查。在指出他们对自己的一些人格特质(如友好)的看法后,参与者一起玩了 45 分钟流行的客厅游戏。随后,他们指出了自己对其他玩家的性格看法。在收到匿名反馈(暗示他人认为自己是一个非常讨人喜欢的人)之前,参与者接受了消极与积极情绪的诱导以及中性对照程序。结果表明,与诱导积极情绪相比,在接受社会反馈之前诱导消极情绪会阻碍其融入参与者的自我观点。1 天后,参与者自我观点的变化仍保持相对稳定,但对照组除外,他们的自我观点略有下降。这些发现证实,消极情绪确实会阻碍(积极的)社会反馈的整合。由于消极情绪在许多精神障碍患者中普遍存在,这可能是导致他们在社会反馈处理方面出现问题的原因之一。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Induced negative affect hinders self-referential belief updating in response to social feedback.","authors":"Tobias Kube, Christoph Korn","doi":"10.1037/emo0001426","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001426","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When people receive feedback from others, this is an opportunity for them to update their self-views. People with mental health problems (e.g., depression), however, often have difficulty using social feedback to update negative beliefs about themselves. To better understand when and how difficulties with integrating social feedback manifest, we investigated how current affect influences social feedback processing. Our preregistered hypothesis was that negative affect hinders change in participants' self-views in response to social feedback. In a nonclinical sample of little diversity (<i>N</i> = 117) in 2023, participants were invited to a laboratory examination in groups of three-five people. After indicating how they thought about themselves in terms of a number of personality traits (e.g., friendly), participants played a popular parlor game together for 45 min. Subsequently, they indicated how they perceived the other players in terms of their personality. Before receiving anonymous feedback, suggesting that the others perceived them as a highly likeable person, participants underwent the induction of negative versus positive affect versus a neutral control procedure. The results show that the induction of negative affect before receiving social feedback hindered its integration into participants' self-views, relative to the induction of positive affect. Changes in participants' self-views remained relatively stable also 1 day later, except for the control group, in which it slightly declined. These findings confirm that negative affect can indeed hamper the integration of (positive) social feedback. Since negative affect is prevalent in many mental disorders, this might contribute to their problems with social feedback processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"174-185"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judging emotion in natural images of crowds. 判断人群自然图像中的情绪
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001358
Susan Hao, David Whitney, Sonia J Bishop

It has been suggested that humans use summary statistics such as the average of the emotion of individual faces when they rapidly judge group emotion. Previous studies have mainly used faces of actors posing basic emotions, and morphed versions of these faces, against a plain background. In the present study, photographs taken in real-world settings were used to investigate the influence of mean facial emotion, maximal facial emotion, and background context on judgments of group emotion, assessed using dimensional ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance. Background context explained a significant amount of unique variance in group ratings for each dimension. Mean emotion explained additional unique variance for valence ratings, whereas maximal emotion explained additional unique variance for arousal, with dominance showing more mixed results. Removing background context and disrupting the contextual and spatial relationship between faces by randomly replacing faces with ones from other images within the stimulus set increased reliance on mean emotion. However, under all conditions, the maximally arousing face continued to exert an influence on ratings of group arousal, in line with theoretical accounts arguing for a unique bottom-up effect of emotional arousal on attentional competition and postattentive perceptual processing. Together these findings suggest that individuals' reliance on average emotion when judging crowd scenes differs as a function of the dimension of affect. In addition, the presence of background context both directly impacts judgments of crowd emotion and modulates the relative influence of maximal versus mean emotion on these judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

有人认为,人类在快速判断群体情绪时,会使用诸如单个人脸情绪平均值之类的汇总统计数据。以前的研究主要使用的是演员在普通背景下摆出基本情绪的脸部表情和这些脸部表情的变形版本。在本研究中,我们使用在真实世界环境中拍摄的照片来研究平均面部情绪、最大面部情绪和背景环境对群体情绪判断的影响。在每个维度的群体评分中,背景情境都能解释大量的独特变异。平均情绪解释了情绪评分的额外独特方差,而最大情绪解释了唤醒评分的额外独特方差,主导地位的结果则较为复杂。通过随机替换刺激集中其他图像中的人脸来移除背景并破坏人脸之间的上下文和空间关系,会增加对平均情绪的依赖。然而,在所有条件下,唤醒程度最高的面孔都会继续影响对群体唤醒程度的评价,这与情绪唤醒对注意竞争和注意后知觉加工的独特自下而上效应的理论观点是一致的。这些研究结果共同表明,个体在判断人群场景时对平均情绪的依赖因情绪维度的不同而不同。此外,背景情境的存在既会直接影响对人群情绪的判断,也会调节最大情绪与平均情绪对这些判断的相对影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Judging emotion in natural images of crowds.","authors":"Susan Hao, David Whitney, Sonia J Bishop","doi":"10.1037/emo0001358","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001358","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been suggested that humans use summary statistics such as the average of the emotion of individual faces when they rapidly judge group emotion. Previous studies have mainly used faces of actors posing basic emotions, and morphed versions of these faces, against a plain background. In the present study, photographs taken in real-world settings were used to investigate the influence of mean facial emotion, maximal facial emotion, and background context on judgments of group emotion, assessed using dimensional ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance. Background context explained a significant amount of unique variance in group ratings for each dimension. Mean emotion explained additional unique variance for valence ratings, whereas maximal emotion explained additional unique variance for arousal, with dominance showing more mixed results. Removing background context and disrupting the contextual and spatial relationship between faces by randomly replacing faces with ones from other images within the stimulus set increased reliance on mean emotion. However, under all conditions, the maximally arousing face continued to exert an influence on ratings of group arousal, in line with theoretical accounts arguing for a unique bottom-up effect of emotional arousal on attentional competition and postattentive perceptual processing. Together these findings suggest that individuals' reliance on average emotion when judging crowd scenes differs as a function of the dimension of affect. In addition, the presence of background context both directly impacts judgments of crowd emotion and modulates the relative influence of maximal versus mean emotion on these judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"57-69"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Training self-other distinction: Effects on emotion regulation, empathy, and theory of mind. 训练自我与他人的区别:对情绪调节、移情和心智理论的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001442
Juan Carlos Oliveros, Idalmis Santiesteban, José Luis Ulloa

Navigating our social environment requires the ability to distinguish ourselves from others. Previous research suggests that training interventions have the potential to enhance the capacity for self-other distinction (SOD), which then may impact various sociocognitive domains, including imitation-inhibition, visual perspective taking, and empathy. Importantly, empirical research on the role of SOD in emotion regulation remains scarce. In this study, we aim to investigate the impact of training SOD on emotion regulation and also replicate findings on empathy and the attribution of mental states to others. Using a pre-post design, participants (N = 104) were assigned to either the imitation-inhibition or general inhibitory control training. Compared to general inhibitory control training, participants trained to inhibit imitation displayed a significant increase in posttest emotion regulation levels compared to pretest levels, indicating that imitation-inhibition training increased self-reported emotion regulation. Notably, emotional interference remained unaffected by either form of training. Both training interventions resulted in diminished self-reported empathic concern, while only general inhibitory control training led to a reduction in personal distress. Moreover, neither type of training had an impact on self-reported perspective taking or theory of mind performance. This study provides novel empirical evidence of the positive impact of imitation-inhibition training on emotion regulation. Furthermore, our findings make significant contributions to the advancement of research in this area and offer further support for the advantages of behavioral training as a methodological approach to studying sociocognitive abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

驾驭我们的社会环境需要有能力将自己与他人区分开来。以往的研究表明,训练干预有可能提高自我与他人区分(SOD)的能力,进而影响各种社会认知领域,包括模仿抑制、视觉透视和移情。重要的是,有关 SOD 在情绪调节中的作用的实证研究仍然很少。在本研究中,我们旨在探讨训练 SOD 对情绪调节的影响,同时复制有关移情和将心理状态归因于他人的研究结果。采用前-后设计,参与者(N = 104)被分配到模仿-抑制或一般抑制控制训练中。与一般抑制控制训练相比,接受模仿抑制训练的参与者在测验后的情绪调节水平比测验前有显著提高,这表明模仿抑制训练提高了自我报告的情绪调节能力。值得注意的是,情绪干扰不受这两种训练形式的影响。这两种训练干预都导致了自我报告的移情关注的减少,而只有一般抑制控制训练导致了个人痛苦的减少。此外,这两种训练对自我报告的透视能力或心智理论表现都没有影响。这项研究为模仿-抑制训练对情绪调节的积极影响提供了新的实证证据。此外,我们的研究结果为推动该领域的研究做出了重要贡献,并进一步证明了行为训练作为研究社会认知能力的一种方法的优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Training self-other distinction: Effects on emotion regulation, empathy, and theory of mind.","authors":"Juan Carlos Oliveros, Idalmis Santiesteban, José Luis Ulloa","doi":"10.1037/emo0001442","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Navigating our social environment requires the ability to distinguish ourselves from others. Previous research suggests that training interventions have the potential to enhance the capacity for self-other distinction (SOD), which then may impact various sociocognitive domains, including imitation-inhibition, visual perspective taking, and empathy. Importantly, empirical research on the role of SOD in emotion regulation remains scarce. In this study, we aim to investigate the impact of training SOD on emotion regulation and also replicate findings on empathy and the attribution of mental states to others. Using a pre-post design, participants (<i>N</i> = 104) were assigned to either the imitation-inhibition or general inhibitory control training. Compared to general inhibitory control training, participants trained to inhibit imitation displayed a significant increase in posttest emotion regulation levels compared to pretest levels, indicating that imitation-inhibition training increased self-reported emotion regulation. Notably, emotional interference remained unaffected by either form of training. Both training interventions resulted in diminished self-reported empathic concern, while only general inhibitory control training led to a reduction in personal distress. Moreover, neither type of training had an impact on self-reported perspective taking or theory of mind performance. This study provides novel empirical evidence of the positive impact of imitation-inhibition training on emotion regulation. Furthermore, our findings make significant contributions to the advancement of research in this area and offer further support for the advantages of behavioral training as a methodological approach to studying sociocognitive abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"210-226"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Motivated to feel better and doing something about it: Cross-cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation during COVID-19. 有动力感觉更好并为此做些什么:COVID-19 期间动机情绪调节的跨文化差异。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001403
Lisya Kaspi, Danfei Hu, Allon Vishkin, Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Yuri Miyamoto, Jan Cieciuch, Akiva Cohen, Yukiko Uchida, Min Young Kim, Xiaoqin Wang, Jiang Qiu, Michaela Riediger, Antje Rauers, Yaniv Hanoch, Maya Tamir

Emotion regulation is linked to adaptive psychological outcomes. To engage in such regulation, people must be motivated to do it. Given that people in different countries vary in how they think about unpleasant emotions, we expected motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions to differ across countries. Furthermore, given that emotion regulation strategies operate in the service of motivation, we expected people who are less motivated to decrease unpleasant emotions to use emotion regulation strategies less across countries. To test these predictions, we conducted two studies during the COVID-19 pandemic: Study 1 in 2020 (N = 1,329) and Study 2 in 2021 (N = 1,279). We assessed the motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions and the use of emotion regulation strategies among members of East Asian countries (i.e., Japan, South Korea, and China) and Western countries (i.e., United States, United Kingdom, and Germany). Because we found substantial variation within these two broader cultural categories, we examined motivation and overall strategy use in emotion regulation at the country level. In both studies, motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was the lowest in Japan and relatively high in the United States. As expected, across countries, weaker motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was associated with using emotion regulation strategies less. We discuss implications of our findings for understanding cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪调节与适应性心理结果有关。要进行这种调节,人们必须有这样做的动机。鉴于不同国家的人对不愉快情绪的看法各不相同,我们预计不同国家的人减少不愉快情绪的动机也不尽相同。此外,鉴于情绪调节策略是为动机服务的,我们预计那些减少不愉快情绪的动机较弱的人在不同国家使用情绪调节策略的程度也会较低。为了验证这些预测,我们在 COVID-19 大流行期间进行了两项研究:研究 1 于 2020 年进行(人数 = 1,329 人),研究 2 于 2021 年进行(人数 = 1,279 人)。我们评估了东亚国家(即日本、韩国和中国)和西方国家(即美国、英国和德国)成员减少不愉快情绪的动机和情绪调节策略的使用情况。由于我们发现在这两个更广泛的文化类别中存在很大差异,因此我们在国家层面上研究了情绪调节的动机和总体策略使用情况。在这两项研究中,日本减少不愉快情绪的动机最低,而美国则相对较高。不出所料,在不同国家,减少不愉快情绪的动机越弱,情绪调节策略的使用就越少。我们讨论了我们的发现对于理解情绪调节动机的文化差异的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Motivated to feel better and doing something about it: Cross-cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation during COVID-19.","authors":"Lisya Kaspi, Danfei Hu, Allon Vishkin, Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Yuri Miyamoto, Jan Cieciuch, Akiva Cohen, Yukiko Uchida, Min Young Kim, Xiaoqin Wang, Jiang Qiu, Michaela Riediger, Antje Rauers, Yaniv Hanoch, Maya Tamir","doi":"10.1037/emo0001403","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001403","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion regulation is linked to adaptive psychological outcomes. To engage in such regulation, people must be motivated to do it. Given that people in different countries vary in how they think about unpleasant emotions, we expected motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions to differ across countries. Furthermore, given that emotion regulation strategies operate in the service of motivation, we expected people who are less motivated to decrease unpleasant emotions to use emotion regulation strategies less across countries. To test these predictions, we conducted two studies during the COVID-19 pandemic: Study 1 in 2020 (<i>N</i> = 1,329) and Study 2 in 2021 (<i>N</i> = 1,279). We assessed the motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions and the use of emotion regulation strategies among members of East Asian countries (i.e., Japan, South Korea, and China) and Western countries (i.e., United States, United Kingdom, and Germany). Because we found substantial variation within these two broader cultural categories, we examined motivation and overall strategy use in emotion regulation at the country level. In both studies, motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was the lowest in Japan and relatively high in the United States. As expected, across countries, weaker motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was associated with using emotion regulation strategies less. We discuss implications of our findings for understanding cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived prolonged stress leads to difficulties in recognizing sadness from voice cues in men but not women. 感觉到的长期压力会导致男性(而非女性)难以从声音线索中识别出悲伤情绪。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001393
Maren Schmidt-Kassow, Alessia-Nadia Günther, Martiel Salim-Latzel, Jochen Kaiser, Silke Paulmann

It has long been known that stress has detrimental effects on cognition (e.g., Alderson & Novack, 2002; Lupien & Lepage, 2001), most notably documented for memory functions (e.g., Schwabe & Wolf, 2013). Interestingly, less is known about the effects of stress on other cognitive functions including language processing. Here, we have examined the effects of self-reported prolonged stress on recognition of emotional language content with a particular emphasis on gender differences. We tested how well 399 participants with different perceived stress levels recognized emotional voice cues. Findings confirm previous results from the emotional prosody literature by demonstrating that women generally outperform men in the vocal emotion recognition task. Crucially, results also revealed that medium levels of perceived stress impair the ability to detect sadness from voice cues in men but not women. These findings were not modulated by task demands (e.g., speeded response) or better acoustic discrimination abilities in women. Results are in line with the idea that perceived stress has a different impact on men versus women and that women have a higher level of experience in voice sadness recognition, potentially due to their predominant role as primary caretakers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

众所周知,压力会对认知产生有害影响(如 Alderson & Novack, 2002; Lupien & Lepage, 2001),其中最明显的是对记忆功能的影响(如 Schwabe & Wolf, 2013)。有趣的是,人们对压力对包括语言处理在内的其他认知功能的影响知之甚少。在此,我们研究了自我报告的长期压力对识别情感语言内容的影响,并特别强调了性别差异。我们测试了 399 名不同压力水平的参与者对情感语音线索的识别能力。研究结果表明,女性在声音情感识别任务中的表现普遍优于男性,从而证实了情感拟声文献之前的研究结果。最重要的是,研究结果还显示,中等程度的感知压力会影响男性从声音线索中识别悲伤情绪的能力,但不会影响女性。这些结果并没有受到任务要求(如快速反应)或女性更强的声音辨别能力的影响。这些结果与以下观点一致:感知到的压力对男性和女性的影响不同,而且女性在声音悲伤识别方面具有更丰富的经验,这可能是由于她们作为主要照顾者的角色占主导地位。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Perceived prolonged stress leads to difficulties in recognizing sadness from voice cues in men but not women.","authors":"Maren Schmidt-Kassow, Alessia-Nadia Günther, Martiel Salim-Latzel, Jochen Kaiser, Silke Paulmann","doi":"10.1037/emo0001393","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001393","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has long been known that stress has detrimental effects on cognition (e.g., Alderson & Novack, 2002; Lupien & Lepage, 2001), most notably documented for memory functions (e.g., Schwabe & Wolf, 2013). Interestingly, less is known about the effects of stress on other cognitive functions including language processing. Here, we have examined the effects of self-reported prolonged stress on recognition of emotional language content with a particular emphasis on gender differences. We tested how well 399 participants with different perceived stress levels recognized emotional voice cues. Findings confirm previous results from the emotional prosody literature by demonstrating that women generally outperform men in the vocal emotion recognition task. Crucially, results also revealed that medium levels of perceived stress impair the ability to detect sadness from voice cues in men but not women. These findings were not modulated by task demands (e.g., speeded response) or better acoustic discrimination abilities in women. Results are in line with the idea that perceived stress has a different impact on men versus women and that women have a higher level of experience in voice sadness recognition, potentially due to their predominant role as primary caretakers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"259-270"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Putting it into words: Emotion vocabulary, emotion differentiation, and depression among adolescents. 用语言表达:青少年的情绪词汇、情绪分化和抑郁。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001429
Gwyneth A L DeLap, Vera Vine, Angela C Santee, Lisa R Starr

Emotion differentiation (ED; the ability to distinguish discrete internal emotion states) may reflect or benefit from knowledge of linguistic labels. The present study uses natural language processing to examine how emotion vocabulary (EV; diversity of unique emotion terms within active vocabulary) relates to ED and depression in an adolescent sample. We tested two competing preregistered (https://osf.io/4j75w/) models regarding the EV-ED link. In the lexical facilitation hypothesis, we posited that larger EV may inform ED, perhaps resulting in larger EVs being associated with greater ED. In the emotional concision hypothesis, we theorized that ED may reflect narrower emotional experiences that are more succinctly labelled, which could result in larger EV being associated with lower ED. A community sample of adolescents (N = 241, ages 14-17, predominantly White) completed interviews, self-report measures, and ecological momentary assessments as part of a larger study conducted between 2014 and 2016. EV was derived using speech samples from transcribed recordings of life stress interviews. In line with the emotion concision hypothesis, EV and ED were inversely related for negative emotions. Moreover, larger negative EV and lower negative ED were each uniquely associated with depression, casting further doubt on whether diverse negative EVs within spontaneous language are fundamentally adaptive for emotional functioning. Replication in more diverse samples is needed to extend generalizability. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪分化(Emotion Differentiation,ED;区分离散的内部情绪状态的能力)可能反映了语言标签的知识,也可能从语言标签的知识中受益。本研究使用自然语言处理技术来研究青少年样本中的情绪词汇(EV;主动词汇中独特情绪术语的多样性)与情绪分化和抑郁之间的关系。我们测试了有关 EV-ED 联系的两个相互竞争的预注册(https://osf.io/4j75w/)模型。在词汇促进假说中,我们假设较大的EV可能会为ED提供信息,从而导致较大的EV与较大的ED相关联。在情感简洁性假说中,我们推测 ED 可能反映了更简洁的狭义情感体验,这可能导致较大的 EV 与较低的 ED 相关联。作为 2014 年至 2016 年进行的一项大型研究的一部分,社区青少年样本(N = 241,年龄 14-17 岁,主要为白人)完成了访谈、自我报告测量和生态瞬间评估。情绪简洁性是通过转录生活压力访谈录音中的语音样本得出的。与情绪简洁性假设一致,EV 和 ED 与负面情绪成反比。此外,较大的负性 EV 和较低的负性 ED 都与抑郁有独特的关联,这让人进一步怀疑自发语言中多种多样的负性 EV 是否从根本上适应了情绪功能。需要在更多不同的样本中进行重复研究,以扩大研究的普遍性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Putting it into words: Emotion vocabulary, emotion differentiation, and depression among adolescents.","authors":"Gwyneth A L DeLap, Vera Vine, Angela C Santee, Lisa R Starr","doi":"10.1037/emo0001429","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001429","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion differentiation (ED; the ability to distinguish discrete internal emotion states) may reflect or benefit from knowledge of linguistic labels. The present study uses natural language processing to examine how emotion vocabulary (EV; diversity of unique emotion terms within active vocabulary) relates to ED and depression in an adolescent sample. We tested two competing preregistered (https://osf.io/4j75w/) models regarding the EV-ED link. In the <i>lexical facilitation hypothesis</i>, we posited that larger EV may inform ED, perhaps resulting in larger EVs being associated with greater ED. In the <i>emotional concision hypothesis</i>, we theorized that ED may reflect narrower emotional experiences that are more succinctly labelled, which could result in larger EV being associated with lower ED. A community sample of adolescents (N = 241, ages 14-17, predominantly White) completed interviews, self-report measures, and ecological momentary assessments as part of a larger study conducted between 2014 and 2016. EV was derived using speech samples from transcribed recordings of life stress interviews. In line with the emotion concision hypothesis, EV and ED were inversely related for negative emotions. Moreover, <i>larger</i> negative EV and <i>lower</i> negative ED were each uniquely associated with depression, casting further doubt on whether diverse negative EVs within spontaneous language are fundamentally adaptive for emotional functioning. Replication in more diverse samples is needed to extend generalizability. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"102-113"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1