首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Out of sight but in mind: Experimentally activating partner representations in daily life buffers against common stressors. 视而不见,却铭记于心:通过实验激活日常生活中的伙伴表征,缓冲常见的压力因素。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001419
Vivian Zayas, Betul Urganci, Steve Strycharz

Lab experiments have shown that reminders of romantic partners buffer against stressors. Yet, tightly controlled experiments do not mimic what transpires in people's actual lives. Thus, an important question is as follows: To what extent do reminders of romantic partners confer affective benefits when they occur "in the wild" as people experience their daily activities? To capture people's emotional experience in real time, two studies, each spanning 3 months, used event-contingent ecological momentary assessments with a within-subject experimental manipulation. Prior to encountering a stressful event (taking an exam), participants received either a supportive text message from their partner or no message (Studies 1 and 2), or a supportive text message from the research team (Study 2). Receiving supportive partner messages, compared to no messages or messages from the research team, led to less negative affect and greater positive affect, and to less negative affect and greater positive affect about the exam itself. Receiving supportive partner messages had no statistically significant effects on subjective stress. Interestingly, the quality of the partner messages, as coded by independent raters, did not significantly predict the magnitude of the affective benefits. These findings suggest that receiving any supportive partner message, and not necessarily more subtle differences in the quality of the message, may be the key ingredient for these benefits to occur. The present work advances understanding of how the symbolic presence of partners confers affective regulatory benefits in everyday life. Implications for emotion regulation and the utility of integrating perspectives from adult attachment are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

实验室实验表明,对浪漫伴侣的回忆可以缓冲压力。然而,严格控制的实验并不能模拟人们的实际生活。因此,一个重要的问题如下:当人们在日常活动中 "自然而然 "地回忆起浪漫伴侣时,这种回忆能在多大程度上产生情感益处?为了实时捕捉人们的情感体验,有两项研究(每项研究为期 3 个月)采用了事件偶发生态学瞬间评估和被试内实验操作。在遇到压力事件(参加考试)之前,参与者会收到一条来自伴侣的支持性短信,或者没有收到任何短信(研究 1 和研究 2),或者收到一条来自研究团队的支持性短信(研究 2)。与没有收到短信或收到研究小组的短信相比,收到伴侣的支持性短信会使参与者对考试本身产生较少的负面情绪和较多的积极情绪,以及较少的负面情绪和较多的积极情绪。收到伙伴的支持性信息对主观压力没有明显的统计学影响。有趣的是,由独立评分者编码的伙伴信息的质量并不能显著预测情感益处的大小。这些研究结果表明,收到任何支持性伴侣信息,而不一定是信息质量上的微妙差异,可能是产生这些益处的关键因素。本研究加深了人们对伙伴的象征性存在如何在日常生活中带来情绪调节益处的理解。本研究还讨论了情绪调节的意义以及整合成人依恋观点的实用性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Out of sight but in mind: Experimentally activating partner representations in daily life buffers against common stressors.","authors":"Vivian Zayas, Betul Urganci, Steve Strycharz","doi":"10.1037/emo0001419","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001419","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Lab experiments have shown that reminders of romantic partners buffer against stressors. Yet, tightly controlled experiments do not mimic what transpires in people's actual lives. Thus, an important question is as follows: To what extent do reminders of romantic partners confer affective benefits when they occur \"in the wild\" as people experience their daily activities? To capture people's emotional experience in real time, two studies, each spanning 3 months, used event-contingent ecological momentary assessments with a within-subject experimental manipulation. Prior to encountering a stressful event (taking an exam), participants received either a supportive text message from their partner or no message (Studies 1 and 2), or a supportive text message from the research team (Study 2). Receiving supportive partner messages, compared to no messages or messages from the research team, led to less negative affect and greater positive affect, and to less negative affect and greater positive affect about the exam itself. Receiving supportive partner messages had no statistically significant effects on subjective stress. Interestingly, the quality of the partner messages, as coded by independent raters, did not significantly predict the magnitude of the affective benefits. These findings suggest that receiving any supportive partner message, and not necessarily more subtle differences in the quality of the message, may be the key ingredient for these benefits to occur. The present work advances understanding of how the symbolic presence of partners confers affective regulatory benefits in everyday life. Implications for emotion regulation and the utility of integrating perspectives from adult attachment are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142607101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental emotion socialization and parent-child attachment security: A meta-analytic review. 父母情感社会化与亲子依恋安全:荟萃分析综述。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001457
Gabriela A Rodrigues, Stephanie M Waslin, Travis K Nair, Kathryn A Kerns, Laura E Brumariu

Although parental sensitivity is an established determinant of children's attachment security, effect sizes are modest, suggesting other aspects of parenting that might support secure attachment. Parental emotion socialization (ES) has been proposed as a parenting domain that is theoretically linked to secure parent-child attachment. The goal of this meta-analysis was to evaluate the strength of the relations between parental ES and attachment security in children under the age of 18. We conducted three meta-analyses assessing the links of supportive parental ES, nonsupportive parental ES, and parental elaboration with attachment security assessed with behavioral, representational, and questionnaire measures (ks = 9-11 samples; Ns = 576-1,763 participants). The relation between supportive ES and security was significant but very small (r = .06). The relation between nonsupportive ES and security was not significant (r = -.05). Parental elaboration emerged as a key correlate of secure attachment, with a medium effect size (r = .24), similar to the relation between sensitivity and attachment security. The findings underscore the need for further research to elaborate on the role of ES in the development of attachment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管父母的敏感性是儿童依恋安全感的一个既定决定因素,但其效应大小并不明显,这表明养育子女的其他方面可能会支持安全依恋。父母的情感社会化(ES)被认为是理论上与安全亲子依恋相关的养育领域。本荟萃分析旨在评估父母情感社会化与 18 岁以下儿童依恋安全之间的关系强度。我们进行了三项荟萃分析,评估了支持性父母ES、非支持性父母ES和父母精心设计与依恋安全感之间的关系,并通过行为、表象和问卷测量进行了评估(ks = 9-11 个样本;Ns = 576-1,763 名参与者)。支持性 ES 与安全感之间的关系显著,但非常小(r = 0.06)。非支持性 ES 与安全感之间的关系不显著(r = -.05)。父母的精心设计是安全依恋的一个关键相关因素,具有中等效应大小(r = .24),与敏感性和依恋安全之间的关系类似。这些发现强调了进一步研究ES在依恋发展中的作用的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Parental emotion socialization and parent-child attachment security: A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Gabriela A Rodrigues, Stephanie M Waslin, Travis K Nair, Kathryn A Kerns, Laura E Brumariu","doi":"10.1037/emo0001457","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001457","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although parental sensitivity is an established determinant of children's attachment security, effect sizes are modest, suggesting other aspects of parenting that might support secure attachment. Parental emotion socialization (ES) has been proposed as a parenting domain that is theoretically linked to secure parent-child attachment. The goal of this meta-analysis was to evaluate the strength of the relations between parental ES and attachment security in children under the age of 18. We conducted three meta-analyses assessing the links of supportive parental ES, nonsupportive parental ES, and parental elaboration with attachment security assessed with behavioral, representational, and questionnaire measures (<i>k</i>s = 9-11 samples; <i>N</i>s = 576-1,763 participants). The relation between supportive ES and security was significant but very small (<i>r</i> = .06). The relation between nonsupportive ES and security was not significant (<i>r</i> = -.05). Parental elaboration emerged as a key correlate of secure attachment, with a medium effect size (<i>r</i> = .24), similar to the relation between sensitivity and attachment security. The findings underscore the need for further research to elaborate on the role of ES in the development of attachment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142607103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A replication and development of the Short Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire (CMBQ-S). 认知调解信念简明问卷(CMBQ-S)的复制与发展。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001447
Martin J Turner, Katia C Vione, Boban Simonovic, Edward Stupple, Matthew Brooks, David Sheffield

The Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire is a 15-item tool that assesses individuals' emotion beliefs about the cognitive mediation of emotions. It measures two emotion beliefs: stimulus-response generation beliefs and cognitive mediation change beliefs. This study aimed to reduce the number of items and test the validity of a briefer version of the Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire. We combined data from 13 unpublished data sets collected between 2019 and 2023 and reached a final sample of 2,872 participants. While this data set is relatively large and diverse (e.g., participants from 53 nationalities), most were from developed countries, and the data were not fully representative across demographic characteristics, such as age and ethnicity. The data were randomly split by 50%/25%/25% (60%/40% female/male) to conduct one exploratory factor analysis and two confirmatory factor analyses. Using an iterative process in the exploratory factor analysis, seven items were deleted for failing to meet item retention criteria, resulting in an eight-item solution across two factors. Across two confirmatory factor analyses with independent samples, the eight-item and the 15-item solutions were tested. The eight-item model was superior in terms of model fit in both samples. These results were in line with our hypothesis in that an eight-item Short Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire confirmed the validity of the two-factor structure. The present study offers a valid and efficient measure of emotion beliefs that can be used to make a rapid assessment of beliefs about emotions and to support clinical interventions, particularly cognitive behavioral therapy approaches, where cognitive change is fundamental. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

认知中介信念问卷是一个由 15 个项目组成的工具,用于评估个人对情绪的认知中介的情绪信念。它测量两种情绪信念:刺激-反应生成信念和认知中介变化信念。本研究旨在减少认知中介信念问卷的项目数量,并测试其简短版本的有效性。我们合并了 2019 年至 2023 年间收集的 13 个未发表数据集的数据,最终样本为 2872 名参与者。虽然这个数据集相对较大且具有多样性(例如,参与者来自 53 个国家),但大多数来自发达国家,而且数据在年龄和种族等人口特征方面不具有完全代表性。数据按 50%/25%/25%(女性/男性各占 60%/40%)随机拆分,以进行一次探索性因子分析和两次确认性因子分析。在探索性因素分析中采用了迭代过程,删除了 7 个不符合项目保留标准的项目,得出了两个因素的 8 个项目解决方案。通过两次独立样本的确认性因素分析,对 8 个项目和 15 个项目的解决方案进行了测试。在两个样本中,8 个项目的模型在模型拟合方面都更胜一筹。这些结果符合我们的假设,即八项认知调解信念简短问卷证实了双因素结构的有效性。本研究提供了一种有效且高效的情绪信念测量方法,可用于对情绪信念进行快速评估,并支持临床干预,尤其是认知行为治疗方法,因为认知改变是根本。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A replication and development of the Short Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire (CMBQ-S).","authors":"Martin J Turner, Katia C Vione, Boban Simonovic, Edward Stupple, Matthew Brooks, David Sheffield","doi":"10.1037/emo0001447","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001447","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire is a 15-item tool that assesses individuals' emotion beliefs about the cognitive mediation of emotions. It measures two emotion beliefs: stimulus-response generation beliefs and cognitive mediation change beliefs. This study aimed to reduce the number of items and test the validity of a briefer version of the Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire. We combined data from 13 unpublished data sets collected between 2019 and 2023 and reached a final sample of 2,872 participants. While this data set is relatively large and diverse (e.g., participants from 53 nationalities), most were from developed countries, and the data were not fully representative across demographic characteristics, such as age and ethnicity. The data were randomly split by 50%/25%/25% (60%/40% female/male) to conduct one exploratory factor analysis and two confirmatory factor analyses. Using an iterative process in the exploratory factor analysis, seven items were deleted for failing to meet item retention criteria, resulting in an eight-item solution across two factors. Across two confirmatory factor analyses with independent samples, the eight-item and the 15-item solutions were tested. The eight-item model was superior in terms of model fit in both samples. These results were in line with our hypothesis in that an eight-item Short Cognitive Mediation Beliefs Questionnaire confirmed the validity of the two-factor structure. The present study offers a valid and efficient measure of emotion beliefs that can be used to make a rapid assessment of beliefs about emotions and to support clinical interventions, particularly cognitive behavioral therapy approaches, where cognitive change is fundamental. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478011","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When is a wandering mind unhappy? The role of thought valence. 徘徊的心灵何时不快乐?思想情绪的作用
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001434
Madeleine Gross, Stephen Raynes, Jonathan W Schooler, Evie Guo, Karen Dobkins

The current research represents one of the first attempts to investigate how various thought qualities that naturally fluctuate across attention states (i.e., mind wandering vs. present-focused attention) impact mood. Of specific interest was whether thought valence may account for previously reported effects of attention state on mood. To examine this, an experience sampling methodology was used to capture participants' (N = 337) attention state (present or mind wandering), thought valence, and mood 6 times per day for 7 days during daily life (all data collected in 2022-2023). Participants further indicated the form of their thoughts (e.g., inner speech), as well as their clarity and interestingness. This design allowed for a conceptual replication and expansion of Killingsworth and Gilbert (2010) in which it was observed that mind wandering leads to relatively poorer mood compared to present-focused attentional states, with the poorest mood for negatively valenced wandering thoughts. Unlike their study, however, we inquired about thought valence for both mind-wandering and present moments. Our findings revealed that the relationship between attention state and mood is substantially accounted for by thought valence, while interestingness and clarity further provided significant, albeit much weaker, indirect effects on mood. Exploratory analyses suggested that the effect of attention state on mood is greatest for older people. Overall, these findings suggest that the commonly reported detrimental impact of mind wandering on mood may largely be accounted for by certain confounding variables. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目前的研究是首次尝试研究在不同注意状态下自然波动的各种思维品质(即思维游离与专注于当下)如何影响情绪。特别令人感兴趣的是,思维价值是否可能解释之前报道的注意力状态对情绪的影响。为了研究这一点,我们采用了一种经验取样方法来捕捉参与者(N = 337)在日常生活中每天6次的注意力状态(当下或思绪游离)、思维价位和情绪(所有数据收集于2022-2023年)。参与者还进一步指出了他们思想的形式(如内心言语),以及思想的清晰度和趣味性。这一设计在概念上复制并扩展了 Killingsworth 和 Gilbert(2010 年)的研究,他们在该研究中观察到,与专注于当下的注意力状态相比,思绪游离会导致相对较差的情绪,而负面情绪的思绪游离会导致最差的情绪。然而,与他们的研究不同的是,我们询问了思绪游荡和当下时刻的思想价值。我们的研究结果表明,注意力状态与情绪之间的关系在很大程度上是由思维价位所决定的,而有趣度和清晰度则进一步对情绪产生了显著的间接影响,尽管这种影响要弱得多。探索性分析表明,注意力状态对老年人情绪的影响最大。总之,这些研究结果表明,通常报道的思维游离对情绪的不利影响可能在很大程度上是由某些混杂变量造成的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"When is a wandering mind unhappy? The role of thought valence.","authors":"Madeleine Gross, Stephen Raynes, Jonathan W Schooler, Evie Guo, Karen Dobkins","doi":"10.1037/emo0001434","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001434","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The current research represents one of the first attempts to investigate how various thought qualities that naturally fluctuate across attention states (i.e., mind wandering vs. present-focused attention) impact mood. Of specific interest was whether thought valence may account for previously reported effects of attention state on mood. To examine this, an experience sampling methodology was used to capture participants' (<i>N</i> = 337) attention state (present or mind wandering), thought valence, and mood 6 times per day for 7 days during daily life (all data collected in 2022-2023). Participants further indicated the form of their thoughts (e.g., inner speech), as well as their clarity and interestingness. This design allowed for a conceptual replication and expansion of Killingsworth and Gilbert (2010) in which it was observed that mind wandering leads to relatively poorer mood compared to present-focused attentional states, with the poorest mood for negatively valenced wandering thoughts. Unlike their study, however, we inquired about thought valence for both mind-wandering and present moments. Our findings revealed that the relationship between attention state and mood is substantially accounted for by thought valence, while interestingness and clarity further provided significant, albeit much weaker, indirect effects on mood. Exploratory analyses suggested that the effect of attention state on mood is greatest for older people. Overall, these findings suggest that the commonly reported detrimental impact of mind wandering on mood may largely be accounted for by certain confounding variables. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People in relationally mobile cultures report higher well-being. 在关系流动的文化中,人们的幸福感更高。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001439
Liuqing Wei, Alexander Scott English, Thomas Talhelm, Yan Zhang, Xuyun Tan, Jiong Zhu, Junxiu Wang

In cultures with high relational mobility, relationships are free and flexible. People can make new friends easily, and they have the freedom to leave unsatisfying relationships. In cultures with low relational mobility, relationships are more fixed, and people have less freedom to leave relationships. We argue that people experience higher well-being if they have the freedom to exit toxic relationships and find new partners easily. In Study 1, we ran a controlled comparison by testing people all within the same nation. We measured well-being and relational mobility in a representative sample of 22,669 people across China. People reported greater well-being in relationally mobile prefectures. Study 2 found this same relationship across 74,657 people in 34 cultures. Study 3 used a cross-lagged design to give more insight into the direction of the relationship. The results showed that relational mobility predicted later subjective well-being, but not the opposite direction. Overall, these data suggest the cultural environments of relational mobility make people happy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在关系流动性高的文化中,人际关系是自由和灵活的。人们可以很容易地结交新朋友,也有离开不满意关系的自由。在关系流动性低的文化中,人际关系较为固定,人们离开关系的自由度较低。我们认为,如果人们能够自由地脱离有毒的人际关系,并轻松地找到新的伴侣,那么他们就会获得更高的幸福感。在研究 1 中,我们对同一国家的人进行了对照比较。我们测量了中国 22669 个代表性样本的幸福感和关系流动性。结果显示,在关系流动性较强的省份,人们的幸福感更高。研究 2 在 34 种文化中的 74 657 人中发现了同样的关系。研究 3 采用了交叉滞后设计,以便更深入地了解这种关系的方向。结果表明,关系流动性可以预测以后的主观幸福感,但不是相反。总之,这些数据表明,关系流动性的文化环境能使人幸福。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"People in relationally mobile cultures report higher well-being.","authors":"Liuqing Wei, Alexander Scott English, Thomas Talhelm, Yan Zhang, Xuyun Tan, Jiong Zhu, Junxiu Wang","doi":"10.1037/emo0001439","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001439","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In cultures with high relational mobility, relationships are free and flexible. People can make new friends easily, and they have the freedom to leave unsatisfying relationships. In cultures with low relational mobility, relationships are more fixed, and people have less freedom to leave relationships. We argue that people experience higher well-being if they have the freedom to exit toxic relationships and find new partners easily. In Study 1, we ran a controlled comparison by testing people all within the same nation. We measured well-being and relational mobility in a representative sample of 22,669 people across China. People reported greater well-being in relationally mobile prefectures. Study 2 found this same relationship across 74,657 people in 34 cultures. Study 3 used a cross-lagged design to give more insight into the direction of the relationship. The results showed that relational mobility predicted later subjective well-being, but not the opposite direction. Overall, these data suggest the cultural environments of relational mobility make people happy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual experience shapes bodily representation of emotion. 视觉体验塑造了身体的情绪表征。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001440
Giada Lettieri, Roberta P Calce, Eléonore Giraudet, Olivier Collignon

Philosophers and experimentalists have long debated whether bodily representation of emotion is grounded in our sensory experience. Indeed, we are used to observe emotional reactions expressed through the bodies of others, yet it is still unknown whether this observation influences how we experience affective states in our own bodies. To delve into this question, we developed a naturalistic haptic task and asked a group of early (n = 20) and late (n = 20) blind, as well as sighted individuals (n = 20) to indicate where in the body they perceive changes associated with affective states. Our results show that visual experience shapes bodily representation of emotion. Blind and sighted individuals attribute different importance to body regions in relation to specific emotional states, as sighted people focus more on visceral sensations, while blind report as more relevant the mouth and the hand areas. We also observe differences in the coherence of bodily maps of specific emotions, such as aggressiveness, for which early and late blind are homogenous in reporting the mouth, while sighted subjects demonstrate a scattered pattern of activation across the body. Finally, our findings show that blind people rely on a different organization of affect, as only sighted categorize bodily maps of emotion through the valence and arousal dimensions. In summary, we demonstrate that sensory experience impacts the bodily representation of affect by modulating the relevance that different body parts have in emotional reactions, modifying the weights attributed to interoceptive and exteroceptive signals, and changing how emotions are conceptualized in the body. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来,哲学家和实验学家一直在争论情绪的身体表征是否基于我们的感官体验。事实上,我们习惯于观察他人通过身体表达的情绪反应,但这种观察是否会影响我们如何在自己的身体中体验情绪状态,我们仍然不得而知。为了深入探讨这个问题,我们开发了一个自然触觉任务,要求一组早期盲人(n = 20)、晚期盲人(n = 20)和视力正常者(n = 20)指出他们在身体的哪个部位感知到与情感状态相关的变化。我们的研究结果表明,视觉经验塑造了身体对情绪的表征。盲人和明眼人对与特定情绪状态相关的身体区域的重视程度不同,明眼人更注重内脏感觉,而盲人则认为嘴部和手部更相关。我们还观察到特定情绪的身体图谱的一致性存在差异,例如在攻击性情绪方面,早期和晚期盲人在报告嘴部时是一致的,而视力正常的受试者则表现出全身分散的激活模式。最后,我们的研究结果表明,盲人依赖于不同的情绪组织,因为只有视力正常的人才会通过情绪的价值和唤醒维度对情绪的身体图谱进行分类。总之,我们证明了感官体验通过调节不同身体部位在情绪反应中的相关性、改变内感知和外感知信号的权重以及改变情绪在身体中的概念化方式,对身体的情绪表征产生了影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Visual experience shapes bodily representation of emotion.","authors":"Giada Lettieri, Roberta P Calce, Eléonore Giraudet, Olivier Collignon","doi":"10.1037/emo0001440","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001440","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Philosophers and experimentalists have long debated whether bodily representation of emotion is grounded in our sensory experience. Indeed, we are used to observe emotional reactions expressed through the bodies of others, yet it is still unknown whether this observation influences how we experience affective states in our own bodies. To delve into this question, we developed a naturalistic haptic task and asked a group of early (<i>n</i> = 20) and late (<i>n</i> = 20) blind, as well as sighted individuals (<i>n</i> = 20) to indicate where in the body they perceive changes associated with affective states. Our results show that visual experience shapes bodily representation of emotion. Blind and sighted individuals attribute different importance to body regions in relation to specific emotional states, as sighted people focus more on visceral sensations, while blind report as more relevant the mouth and the hand areas. We also observe differences in the coherence of bodily maps of specific emotions, such as aggressiveness, for which early and late blind are homogenous in reporting the mouth, while sighted subjects demonstrate a scattered pattern of activation across the body. Finally, our findings show that blind people rely on a different organization of affect, as only sighted categorize bodily maps of emotion through the valence and arousal dimensions. In summary, we demonstrate that sensory experience impacts the bodily representation of affect by modulating the relevance that different body parts have in emotional reactions, modifying the weights attributed to interoceptive and exteroceptive signals, and changing how emotions are conceptualized in the body. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Empathic concern promotes social support-seeking: A cross-cultural study. 移情关怀促进寻求社会支持:一项跨文化研究。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001451
Shaofeng Zheng, Rina Tanaka, Keiko Ishii

Previous research has suggested that empathic concern may affect cultural differences in social support-seeking. However, neither the mechanisms through which empathic concern promotes support-seeking nor the explanations for cultural differences in empathic concern are clear. This study attempted to address these questions by conducting three studies in Japan and the United States. The results showed that Japanese participants reported having lower trait-empathic concern and seeking less social support in dealing with stress than European Americans. Study 1 found that trait-empathic concern mediated the cultural differences in support-seeking by increasing beliefs about others' prosocial willingness. Using a controlled set of stressful scenarios, Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1. Additionally, Study 2 showed that Japanese participants reported greater endorsement of the causal repressive suffering construal than European Americans, partly accounting for cultural differences in trait-empathic concern. Using an experimental design, Study 3 showed that primed empathic concern increased support-seeking in coping with follow-up stress across cultures. These findings contribute to our understanding of the role of empathic concern in support-seeking and cultural differences in empathic concern. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,移情关注可能会影响寻求社会支持的文化差异。然而,移情关注促进寻求支持的机制和移情关注文化差异的解释都不清楚。本研究试图通过在日本和美国进行的三项研究来解决这些问题。结果显示,与欧洲裔美国人相比,日本人的特质移情关注较低,在应对压力时寻求的社会支持也较少。研究 1 发现,特质-情感关怀通过增加对他人亲社会意愿的信念,在寻求支持的文化差异中起到了中介作用。研究 2 采用了一组受控的压力情景,重复了研究 1 的结果。此外,研究2还表明,与欧美人相比,日本人更认可因果压抑痛苦构念,这在一定程度上解释了特质-移情关怀的文化差异。通过实验设计,研究 3 表明,在应对后续压力时,不同文化背景下的共情关注会增加寻求支持的程度。这些发现有助于我们理解移情关注在寻求支持中的作用以及移情关注的文化差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Empathic concern promotes social support-seeking: A cross-cultural study.","authors":"Shaofeng Zheng, Rina Tanaka, Keiko Ishii","doi":"10.1037/emo0001451","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001451","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has suggested that empathic concern may affect cultural differences in social support-seeking. However, neither the mechanisms through which empathic concern promotes support-seeking nor the explanations for cultural differences in empathic concern are clear. This study attempted to address these questions by conducting three studies in Japan and the United States. The results showed that Japanese participants reported having lower trait-empathic concern and seeking less social support in dealing with stress than European Americans. Study 1 found that trait-empathic concern mediated the cultural differences in support-seeking by increasing beliefs about others' prosocial willingness. Using a controlled set of stressful scenarios, Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1. Additionally, Study 2 showed that Japanese participants reported greater endorsement of the causal repressive suffering construal than European Americans, partly accounting for cultural differences in trait-empathic concern. Using an experimental design, Study 3 showed that primed empathic concern increased support-seeking in coping with follow-up stress across cultures. These findings contribute to our understanding of the role of empathic concern in support-seeking and cultural differences in empathic concern. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children with facial differences experience deficits in emotion skills. 有面部差异的儿童在情绪技能方面存在缺陷。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001432
Robert Brinton Fujiki, Fangyun Zhao, Catharine B Garland, Paula M Niedenthal, Susan L Thibeault

Children with facial differences, such as repaired cleft lip and palate (CLP), may present with reduced capacity for sensorimotor simulation, particularly in the form of facial mimicry. This study examined whether facial mimicry, emotion recognition, and empathy skills are reduced in children with CLP when compared with sex/age-matched controls. A case-control design was utilized. Forty-five children between the ages of 8 and 12 with CLP, and 45 age/sex-matched controls were recruited. Participants completed a facial mimicry task, and facial movements were tracked and quantified using OpenFace. Participants also completed picture and context-based emotion recognition tasks. Picture-based assessment involved identifying emotions from the Dynamic FACES database. Context-based assessment consisted of identifying how a child might feel in various situations. Finally, participants and their parents completed the Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (EmQue-CA). Children with CLP presented with significantly reduced facial mimicry (p = .017), picture-based (p < .001), and context-based emotion recognition scores (p < .001) when compared with controls. Better facial mimicry was associated with better picture-based emotion recognition scores in the control group only (r = .22, p < .01). Children with CLP also had significantly lower child and parent-proxy EmQue-CA scores (p < .001). Greater facial mimicry significantly predicted better parent-proxy EmQue-CA scores (p = .016) but did not predict child scores. Children with CLP presented with reduced facial mimicry, poorer emotion recognition, and empathy skills. These findings have a broader relevance as they suggest children with facial differences may present with reduced facial mimicry and/or deficits in emotion recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有面部差异的儿童,如唇腭裂(CLP)修复后,其感觉运动模拟能力可能会下降,尤其是面部模仿能力。本研究探讨了与性别/年龄匹配的对照组相比,CLP 患儿的面部模仿能力、情绪识别能力和移情能力是否会降低。研究采用了病例对照设计。研究招募了 45 名年龄在 8 到 12 岁之间的 CLP 儿童和 45 名年龄/性别匹配的对照组儿童。参与者完成了一项面部模仿任务,并使用 OpenFace 对面部动作进行了跟踪和量化。参与者还完成了基于图片和情境的情绪识别任务。基于图片的评估包括从动态 FACES 数据库中识别情绪。基于情境的评估包括识别儿童在不同情境下的感受。最后,参与者及其父母完成了儿童和青少年移情问卷(EmQue-CA)。与对照组相比,CLP 患儿的面部模仿能力(p = .017)、基于图片的情绪识别能力(p < .001)和基于情境的情绪识别能力(p < .001)均明显降低。仅在对照组中,面部模仿能力越强,基于图片的情绪识别得分越高(r = .22,p < .01)。CLP患儿的儿童和家长代理EmQue-CA得分也明显较低(p < .001)。面部模仿能力越强,父母代理的 EmQue-CA 得分越高(p = .016),但对儿童得分没有预测作用。CLP患儿的面部模仿能力较弱,情绪识别能力和移情能力较差。这些发现具有更广泛的意义,因为它们表明,面部差异儿童可能会出现面部模仿能力下降和/或情绪识别能力缺陷。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Children with facial differences experience deficits in emotion skills.","authors":"Robert Brinton Fujiki, Fangyun Zhao, Catharine B Garland, Paula M Niedenthal, Susan L Thibeault","doi":"10.1037/emo0001432","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001432","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children with facial differences, such as repaired cleft lip and palate (CLP), may present with reduced capacity for sensorimotor simulation, particularly in the form of facial mimicry. This study examined whether facial mimicry, emotion recognition, and empathy skills are reduced in children with CLP when compared with sex/age-matched controls. A case-control design was utilized. Forty-five children between the ages of 8 and 12 with CLP, and 45 age/sex-matched controls were recruited. Participants completed a facial mimicry task, and facial movements were tracked and quantified using OpenFace. Participants also completed picture and context-based emotion recognition tasks. Picture-based assessment involved identifying emotions from the Dynamic FACES database. Context-based assessment consisted of identifying how a child might feel in various situations. Finally, participants and their parents completed the Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (EmQue-CA). Children with CLP presented with significantly reduced facial mimicry (<i>p</i> = .017), picture-based (<i>p</i> < .001), and context-based emotion recognition scores (<i>p</i> < .001) when compared with controls. Better facial mimicry was associated with better picture-based emotion recognition scores in the control group only (<i>r</i> = .22, <i>p</i> < .01). Children with CLP also had significantly lower child and parent-proxy EmQue-CA scores (<i>p</i> < .001). Greater facial mimicry significantly predicted better parent-proxy EmQue-CA scores (<i>p</i> = .016) but did not predict child scores. Children with CLP presented with reduced facial mimicry, poorer emotion recognition, and empathy skills. These findings have a broader relevance as they suggest children with facial differences may present with reduced facial mimicry and/or deficits in emotion recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social contexts are associated with higher emotional awareness than nonsocial contexts: Evidence in a sample of people with and without major depressive disorder. 与非社会环境相比,社会环境与更高的情绪意识相关:重度抑郁症患者和非重度抑郁症患者样本中的证据。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001436
Alison B Tuck, Mallory J Feldman, Kristen A Lindquist, Renee J Thompson

Growing evidence suggests that social contexts may prompt qualitatively distinct experiences of emotions than nonsocial contexts. In this study of people's naturalistic daily emotional experiences, we examined in adults with and without major depressive disorder (MDD) whether experiencing emotions in a social context (with others) versus nonsocial context (without others) was associated with greater emotional clarity and attention to one's emotional experience (i.e., emotional awareness). Based on evidence that social stimuli are highly salient to social species, we predicted that interactions with social others-and especially close social others-would be associated with greater emotional awareness. We furthermore expected that individuals with MDD, who tend to have diminished emotional clarity and social deficits, might experience less emotional awareness in social settings than healthy controls. Across a 2-week experience sampling study that concluded in 2019, we assessed emotional awareness when people were interacting with others (vs. not) and interacting with close (vs. nonclose) others among adults with current MDD (n = 53), remitted MDD (n = 80), and healthy controls (n = 87). As expected, attention to emotion and emotional clarity were higher in social versus nonsocial contexts and when interacting with close versus nonclose others. Contrary to expectations that these effects would be weaker among those with current MDD, the current MDD group showed enhanced emotional clarity in social versus nonsocial settings compared to the other two groups. Insofar as emotional clarity is beneficial to well-being, these findings suggest those with MDD may especially benefit from social contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

越来越多的证据表明,与非社会环境相比,社会环境可能会促使人们产生截然不同的情绪体验。在这项关于人们日常自然情绪体验的研究中,我们对患有和未患有重度抑郁症(MDD)的成年人进行了调查,以了解在社交情境(与他人一起)和非社交情境(没有他人)中体验情绪是否与更高的情绪清晰度和对自身情绪体验的关注度(即情绪意识)相关。有证据表明,社会性刺激对社会性物种具有高度显著性,因此我们预测,与社会性他人--尤其是亲密的社会性他人--的互动将与更高的情绪意识相关。此外,我们还预计,与健康对照组相比,情感清晰度和社交能力往往较弱的 MDD 患者在社交环境中的情感意识可能较弱。在2019年结束的一项为期两周的体验取样研究中,我们评估了当前患有多发性抑郁症的成年人(n = 53)、缓解型多发性抑郁症患者(n = 80)和健康对照组(n = 87)在与他人互动(与不互动)和与亲密(与不亲密)他人互动时的情绪意识。不出所料,在社交与非社交情境中,以及在与亲密与非亲密他人互动时,对情绪的关注度和情绪清晰度都更高。与预期不同的是,目前患有 MDD 的人群在社交与非社交环境中的情绪清晰度要高于其他两组人群。就情绪清晰度对幸福感的益处而言,这些研究结果表明,患有 MDD 的人尤其能从社交环境中获益。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Social contexts are associated with higher emotional awareness than nonsocial contexts: Evidence in a sample of people with and without major depressive disorder.","authors":"Alison B Tuck, Mallory J Feldman, Kristen A Lindquist, Renee J Thompson","doi":"10.1037/emo0001436","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001436","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Growing evidence suggests that social contexts may prompt qualitatively distinct experiences of emotions than nonsocial contexts. In this study of people's naturalistic daily emotional experiences, we examined in adults with and without major depressive disorder (MDD) whether experiencing emotions in a social context (with others) versus nonsocial context (without others) was associated with greater emotional clarity and attention to one's emotional experience (i.e., <i>emotional awareness</i>). Based on evidence that social stimuli are highly salient to social species, we predicted that interactions with social others-and especially close social others-would be associated with greater emotional awareness. We furthermore expected that individuals with MDD, who tend to have diminished emotional clarity and social deficits, might experience less emotional awareness in social settings than healthy controls. Across a 2-week experience sampling study that concluded in 2019, we assessed emotional awareness when people were interacting with others (vs. not) and interacting with close (vs. nonclose) others among adults with current MDD (<i>n</i> = 53), remitted MDD (<i>n</i> = 80), and healthy controls (<i>n</i> = 87). As expected, attention to emotion and emotional clarity were higher in social versus nonsocial contexts and when interacting with close versus nonclose others. Contrary to expectations that these effects would be weaker among those with current MDD, the current MDD group showed enhanced emotional clarity in social versus nonsocial settings compared to the other two groups. Insofar as emotional clarity is beneficial to well-being, these findings suggest those with MDD may especially benefit from social contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual sensitivity to labeling stereotyped emotion expressions: Associations with age and subclinical psychopathology symptoms from childhood through early adulthood. 对刻板情绪表达标签的知觉敏感性:从童年到成年早期,年龄与亚临床精神病理学症状的关系。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001441
David G Weissman, Henna I Vartiainen, Erik C Nook, Hilary K Lambert, Stephanie F Sasse, Leah H Somerville, Katie A McLaughlin

This study investigates (a) age-related differences in how the intensity of stereotyped facial expressions influence the emotion label children, adolescents, and adults assign to that face and (b) how this perceptual sensitivity relates to subclinical symptoms of psychopathology. In 2015-2016, 184 participants aged 4-25 years viewed posed stereotypes of angry, fearful, sad, and happy expressions morphed with neutral expressions at 10%-90% intensity. Thin plate regression smoothing splines were used to chart nonlinear associations between age and the perceptual threshold participants needed to assign the emotion label expected based on cultural consensus. Results suggest that sensitivity to labeling stereotypical happy faces as "happy" peaked by age 4. Sensitivity to perceiving stereotypical angry faces as "angry" increased from ages 4 to 7 and then plateaued. In contrast, sensitivity to perceiving stereotypical fearful and sad faces demonstrated protracted development, not reaching a plateau until ages 15 and 16, respectively. Reduction in selecting the "I don't know" response was the primary driver of these age-related changes. Stereotyped fear expressions required the highest intensity to be labeled as such and showed the most marked change in perceptual threshold across development. Interestingly, lower intensity morphs of stereotypical fear faces were frequently labeled "sad." Furthermore, perceiving lower intensity fear morphs was associated with fewer internalizing and externalizing symptoms in participants aged 7-19. This study describes the development of perceptual sensitivity to labeling stereotypical expressions of emotion according to cultural consensus and shows that how people perceive and categorize ambiguous facial expressions is associated with vulnerability to psychopathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究调查了(a)刻板面部表情的强度如何影响儿童、青少年和成年人赋予该面部的情绪标签的年龄相关差异,以及(b)这种知觉敏感性与亚临床心理病理症状的关系。2015-2016 年,184 名年龄在 4-25 岁之间的参与者观看了愤怒、恐惧、悲伤和快乐表情与中性表情在 10%-90% 强度下的变形。研究人员使用薄板回归平滑样条来绘制年龄与参与者根据文化共识分配预期情绪标签所需的感知阈值之间的非线性关联图。结果表明,将刻板的快乐面孔标记为 "快乐 "的敏感度在 4 岁时达到顶峰。将刻板的愤怒面孔感知为 "愤怒 "的敏感度在 4 到 7 岁期间上升,然后趋于平稳。相比之下,感知恐惧和悲伤的刻板面孔的敏感度则表现出持久的发展,分别直到 15 岁和 16 岁才达到高峰。减少选择 "我不知道 "的反应是这些与年龄有关的变化的主要驱动因素。刻板恐惧表情需要最高的强度才能被标记为刻板恐惧表情,并且在整个发育过程中显示出最明显的知觉阈值变化。有趣的是,刻板恐惧表情的较低强度形态经常被标记为 "悲伤"。此外,在 7-19 岁的参与者中,感知强度较低的恐惧变形与较少的内化和外化症状有关。本研究描述了根据文化共识对刻板情绪表达进行标记的感知敏感性的发展,并表明人们如何感知和归类模棱两可的面部表情与心理病理学的易感性有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Perceptual sensitivity to labeling stereotyped emotion expressions: Associations with age and subclinical psychopathology symptoms from childhood through early adulthood.","authors":"David G Weissman, Henna I Vartiainen, Erik C Nook, Hilary K Lambert, Stephanie F Sasse, Leah H Somerville, Katie A McLaughlin","doi":"10.1037/emo0001441","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001441","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigates (a) age-related differences in how the intensity of stereotyped facial expressions influence the emotion label children, adolescents, and adults assign to that face and (b) how this perceptual sensitivity relates to subclinical symptoms of psychopathology. In 2015-2016, 184 participants aged 4-25 years viewed posed stereotypes of angry, fearful, sad, and happy expressions morphed with neutral expressions at 10%-90% intensity. Thin plate regression smoothing splines were used to chart nonlinear associations between age and the perceptual threshold participants needed to assign the emotion label expected based on cultural consensus. Results suggest that sensitivity to labeling stereotypical happy faces as \"happy\" peaked by age 4. Sensitivity to perceiving stereotypical angry faces as \"angry\" increased from ages 4 to 7 and then plateaued. In contrast, sensitivity to perceiving stereotypical fearful and sad faces demonstrated protracted development, not reaching a plateau until ages 15 and 16, respectively. Reduction in selecting the \"I don't know\" response was the primary driver of these age-related changes. Stereotyped fear expressions required the highest intensity to be labeled as such and showed the most marked change in perceptual threshold across development. Interestingly, lower intensity morphs of stereotypical fear faces were frequently labeled \"sad.\" Furthermore, perceiving lower intensity fear morphs was associated with fewer internalizing and externalizing symptoms in participants aged 7-19. This study describes the development of perceptual sensitivity to labeling stereotypical expressions of emotion according to cultural consensus and shows that how people perceive and categorize ambiguous facial expressions is associated with vulnerability to psychopathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394338","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1