首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
A multisample investigation of links between individual differences in emotion dysregulation and perceived helpfulness of interpersonal emotion regulation interactions. 对情绪失调的个体差异与人际情绪调节互动的帮助感知之间的联系进行多样本调查。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001415
Benjamin A Swerdlow, Sheri L Johnson

Prior theory and research offer competing predictions for associations between intrapersonal emotion (dys)regulation and interpersonal emotion regulation (IER). One possibility is that difficulties recognizing, accepting, or managing one's emotions might tend to interfere with seeking or benefiting from IER. Alternatively, people who struggle to regulate their emotions by themselves might nevertheless be able to outsource regulatory functions or capitalize on regulatory support effectively, such that benefits of IER might be preserved or even amplified. We conducted secondary analyses of five samples (Ns = 90-381) collected between 2016 and 2020 to examine links between individual differences in intrapersonal emotion (dys)regulation and reported desire for, seeking of, and helpfulness of receiving IER. The samples consisted of students at a public university in California (Samples 1-3), romantic couples recruited predominantly from the Greater San Francisco Bay Area community (Sample 4), and adults reporting difficulties with emotion-related impulsivity enrolled in an online intervention to reduce aggression (Sample 5). Methods varied across samples, including questionnaires, autobiographical recall, nightly diaries, and ecological momentary assessment. Across samples, individual differences in emotion dysregulation, cognitive reappraisal, and expressive suppression were more robustly tied to perceived helpfulness of IER than reported desire for IER. More specifically, emotion dysregulation and suppression use were negatively associated with helpfulness, whereas reappraisal use was positively associated with helpfulness; however, some results were inconsistent across samples. We examine these consistencies and inconsistencies considering differences in sample characteristics and methods. We discuss conceptual and practical implications of these findings alongside strengths, limitations, and future directions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的理论和研究为个人内部情绪(失调)调节和人际情绪调节(IER)之间的关联提供了相互竞争的预测。一种可能性是,认识、接受或管理自己情绪的困难可能会干扰寻求或受益于人际情绪调节。另一种可能是,那些难以自我调节情绪的人可能能够有效地外包调节功能或利用调节支持,从而保持甚至扩大人际情绪调节的益处。我们对 2016 年至 2020 年间收集的五个样本(Ns = 90-381)进行了二次分析,以研究个人内部情绪(失调)调节的个体差异与报告的接受 IER 的愿望、寻求和帮助之间的联系。样本包括加利福尼亚州一所公立大学的学生(样本 1-3)、主要从大旧金山湾区社区招募的恋爱情侣(样本 4),以及参加在线干预以减少攻击行为、报告有情绪相关冲动困难的成年人(样本 5)。不同样本的研究方法各不相同,包括问卷调查、自传回忆、夜间日记和生态瞬间评估。在不同的样本中,情绪失调、认知再评价和表达压抑方面的个体差异与感知到的 IER 有益性之间的联系比报告的 IER 欲望之间的联系更为紧密。更具体地说,情绪失调和压抑的使用与有用性呈负相关,而重评的使用与有用性呈正相关;但是,有些结果在不同样本中并不一致。考虑到样本特征和方法的差异,我们对这些一致性和不一致性进行了研究。我们讨论了这些发现的概念和实际意义,以及优势、局限性和未来发展方向。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A multisample investigation of links between individual differences in emotion dysregulation and perceived helpfulness of interpersonal emotion regulation interactions.","authors":"Benjamin A Swerdlow, Sheri L Johnson","doi":"10.1037/emo0001415","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001415","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior theory and research offer competing predictions for associations between intrapersonal emotion (dys)regulation and interpersonal emotion regulation (IER). One possibility is that difficulties recognizing, accepting, or managing one's emotions might tend to interfere with seeking or benefiting from IER. Alternatively, people who struggle to regulate their emotions by themselves might nevertheless be able to outsource regulatory functions or capitalize on regulatory support effectively, such that benefits of IER might be preserved or even amplified. We conducted secondary analyses of five samples (<i>N</i>s = 90-381) collected between 2016 and 2020 to examine links between individual differences in intrapersonal emotion (dys)regulation and reported desire for, seeking of, and helpfulness of receiving IER. The samples consisted of students at a public university in California (Samples 1-3), romantic couples recruited predominantly from the Greater San Francisco Bay Area community (Sample 4), and adults reporting difficulties with emotion-related impulsivity enrolled in an online intervention to reduce aggression (Sample 5). Methods varied across samples, including questionnaires, autobiographical recall, nightly diaries, and ecological momentary assessment. Across samples, individual differences in emotion dysregulation, cognitive reappraisal, and expressive suppression were more robustly tied to perceived helpfulness of IER than reported desire for IER. More specifically, emotion dysregulation and suppression use were negatively associated with helpfulness, whereas reappraisal use was positively associated with helpfulness; however, some results were inconsistent across samples. We examine these consistencies and inconsistencies considering differences in sample characteristics and methods. We discuss conceptual and practical implications of these findings alongside strengths, limitations, and future directions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-channel adaptation reveals shared emotion representation from face and biological motion. 跨通道适应揭示了人脸和生物运动的共同情绪表征。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001409
Tian Yuan, Li Wang, Yi Jiang

Emotions in interpersonal interactions can be communicated simultaneously via various social signals such as face and biological motion (BM). Here, we demonstrate that even though BM and face are very different in visual properties, emotions conveyed by these two types of social signals involve dedicated and common processing mechanisms (N = 168, college students, 2020-2024). By utilizing the visual adaptation paradigm, we found that prolonged exposure to the happy BM biased the emotion perception of the subsequently presented morphed BM toward sad, and vice versus. The observed aftereffect disappeared when the BM adaptors were shown inverted, indicating that it arose from emotional information processing rather than being a result of adaptation to constitutive low-level features. Besides, such an aftereffect was also found for facial expressions and similarly vanished when the face adaptors were inverted. Critically, preexposure to emotional faces also exerted an adaptation aftereffect on the emotion perception of BMs. Furthermore, this cross-channel effect could not only happen from faces to BMs but also from BMs to faces, suggesting that emotion perception from face and BM are potentially driven by common underlying neural substrates. Overall, these findings highlighted a close coupling of BM and face emotion perception and suggested the existence of a dedicated emotional representation that can be shared across these two different types of social signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人际交往中的情绪可以通过脸部和生物动作(BM)等各种社交信号同时传达。在这里,我们证明了尽管生物运动和人脸在视觉特性上有很大不同,但这两类社交信号所传达的情绪涉及到专门的和共同的处理机制(N = 168,大学生,2020-2024 年)。通过使用视觉适应范式,我们发现长时间接触快乐的BM会使随后出现的变形BM的情绪感知偏向悲伤,反之亦然。当BM适配器倒置显示时,观察到的后效应消失了,这表明后效应产生于情绪信息处理,而不是对构成性低级特征的适应结果。此外,在面部表情中也发现了这种后效,当面部适配器倒置时,这种后效也同样消失了。重要的是,预先暴露于情绪化的面孔也会对BM的情绪感知产生适应后效。此外,这种跨通道效应不仅发生在人脸到生物标记物之间,也发生在生物标记物到人脸之间,这表明人脸和生物标记物的情绪感知可能是由共同的潜在神经基质驱动的。总之,这些发现凸显了BM和人脸情绪感知的密切联系,并表明存在一种专用的情绪表征,可以在这两种不同类型的社会信号中共享。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Cross-channel adaptation reveals shared emotion representation from face and biological motion.","authors":"Tian Yuan, Li Wang, Yi Jiang","doi":"10.1037/emo0001409","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001409","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotions in interpersonal interactions can be communicated simultaneously via various social signals such as face and biological motion (BM). Here, we demonstrate that even though BM and face are very different in visual properties, emotions conveyed by these two types of social signals involve dedicated and common processing mechanisms (<i>N</i> = 168, college students, 2020-2024). By utilizing the visual adaptation paradigm, we found that prolonged exposure to the happy BM biased the emotion perception of the subsequently presented morphed BM toward sad, and vice versus. The observed aftereffect disappeared when the BM adaptors were shown inverted, indicating that it arose from emotional information processing rather than being a result of adaptation to constitutive low-level features. Besides, such an aftereffect was also found for facial expressions and similarly vanished when the face adaptors were inverted. Critically, preexposure to emotional faces also exerted an adaptation aftereffect on the emotion perception of BMs. Furthermore, this cross-channel effect could not only happen from faces to BMs but also from BMs to faces, suggesting that emotion perception from face and BM are potentially driven by common underlying neural substrates. Overall, these findings highlighted a close coupling of BM and face emotion perception and suggested the existence of a dedicated emotional representation that can be shared across these two different types of social signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Will you boost my joy or dampen it? Cultural differences in hedonic interpersonal emotion regulation in romantic relationships. 你会增加还是减少我的快乐?恋爱关系中享乐型人际情绪调节的文化差异
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001427
Yue Li, Fiona Ge, Paula R Pietromonaco, Jiyoung Park

A central tenet guiding contemporary research on emotions is that people are fundamentally motivated to feel good and avoid feeling bad. This principle translates from intrapersonal to extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation; people not only strive to achieve the hedonic goal of managing their own emotions, but they also help others reach the same goal-the process called hedonic interpersonal emotion regulation (hedonic IER). Here, we challenge the centrality of this principle in romantic relationships by testing a hypothesis that, compared with European Americans, Asians use hedonic IER less and benefit less from their partners' use of this strategy. Findings across three studies (total N = 2,540) supported this hypothesis. First, European Americans used hedonic IER more than Asians both in positive and negative situations, and, moreover, this cultural difference was mediated by dialectical beliefs about emotions (Study 1). Second, compared with Chinese, European Americans anticipated greater relationship satisfaction in response to their partners' hedonic IER attempts in both positive and negative situations, and this effect was again mediated by dialectical emotion beliefs (Study 2). Third, compared with Asian couples, European American couples perceived that their partners used hedonic IER more in positive situations. Moreover, when European Americans perceived that their partners used hedonic IER more, they showed greater vagal withdrawal during a positive discussion (i.e., physiological reactivity linked to enhanced social sensitivity and engagement), while Asians did not show this association (Study 3). These findings highlight the critical role of sociocultural contexts in shaping IER and its relational consequences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

指导当代情绪研究的一个核心原则是,人们的基本动机是感觉良好和避免感觉糟糕。人们不仅努力实现管理自己情绪的享乐目标,而且还帮助他人实现同样的目标--这一过程被称为享乐型人际情绪调节(hedonic IER)。与欧美人相比,亚洲人较少使用享乐型人际情绪调节策略,也较少从伴侣使用这一策略中获益。三项研究(总人数 = 2,540)的结果都支持这一假设。首先,无论是在积极还是消极的情境中,欧美人都比亚洲人更多地使用享乐型 IER,而且,这种文化差异还受到关于情绪的辩证信念的影响(研究 1)。其次,与中国人相比,欧裔美国人在积极和消极情境中都会对伴侣的享乐型IER尝试做出反应,从而预期更高的关系满意度,而这一效应再次受到辩证情绪信念的调节(研究2)。第三,与亚洲夫妇相比,欧美夫妇认为他们的伴侣在积极情境中更多地使用享乐型IER。此外,当欧洲裔美国人认为他们的伴侣更多使用享乐型IER时,他们在积极讨论中表现出更大的迷走神经退缩(即与增强社会敏感性和参与度相关的生理反应),而亚洲人则没有表现出这种关联(研究3)。这些发现凸显了社会文化背景在形成 IER 及其关系后果中的关键作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Will you boost my joy or dampen it? Cultural differences in hedonic interpersonal emotion regulation in romantic relationships.","authors":"Yue Li, Fiona Ge, Paula R Pietromonaco, Jiyoung Park","doi":"10.1037/emo0001427","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001427","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A central tenet guiding contemporary research on emotions is that people are fundamentally motivated to feel good and avoid feeling bad. This principle translates from intrapersonal to extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation; people not only strive to achieve the hedonic goal of managing their own emotions, but they also help others reach the same goal-the process called hedonic interpersonal emotion regulation (hedonic IER). Here, we challenge the centrality of this principle in romantic relationships by testing a hypothesis that, compared with European Americans, Asians use hedonic IER less and benefit less from their partners' use of this strategy. Findings across three studies (total <i>N</i> = 2,540) supported this hypothesis. First, European Americans used hedonic IER more than Asians both in positive and negative situations, and, moreover, this cultural difference was mediated by dialectical beliefs about emotions (Study 1). Second, compared with Chinese, European Americans anticipated greater relationship satisfaction in response to their partners' hedonic IER attempts in both positive and negative situations, and this effect was again mediated by dialectical emotion beliefs (Study 2). Third, compared with Asian couples, European American couples perceived that their partners used hedonic IER more in positive situations. Moreover, when European Americans perceived that their partners used hedonic IER more, they showed greater vagal withdrawal during a positive discussion (i.e., physiological reactivity linked to enhanced social sensitivity and engagement), while Asians did not show this association (Study 3). These findings highlight the critical role of sociocultural contexts in shaping IER and its relational consequences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Putting it into words: Emotion vocabulary, emotion differentiation, and depression among adolescents. 用语言表达:青少年的情绪词汇、情绪分化和抑郁。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001429
Gwyneth A L DeLap, Vera Vine, Angela C Santee, Lisa R Starr

Emotion differentiation (ED; the ability to distinguish discrete internal emotion states) may reflect or benefit from knowledge of linguistic labels. The present study uses natural language processing to examine how emotion vocabulary (EV; diversity of unique emotion terms within active vocabulary) relates to ED and depression in an adolescent sample. We tested two competing preregistered (https://osf.io/4j75w/) models regarding the EV-ED link. In the lexical facilitation hypothesis, we posited that larger EV may inform ED, perhaps resulting in larger EVs being associated with greater ED. In the emotional concision hypothesis, we theorized that ED may reflect narrower emotional experiences that are more succinctly labelled, which could result in larger EV being associated with lower ED. A community sample of adolescents (N = 241, ages 14-17, predominantly White) completed interviews, self-report measures, and ecological momentary assessments as part of a larger study conducted between 2014 and 2016. EV was derived using speech samples from transcribed recordings of life stress interviews. In line with the emotion concision hypothesis, EV and ED were inversely related for negative emotions. Moreover, larger negative EV and lower negative ED were each uniquely associated with depression, casting further doubt on whether diverse negative EVs within spontaneous language are fundamentally adaptive for emotional functioning. Replication in more diverse samples is needed to extend generalizability. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪分化(Emotion Differentiation,ED;区分离散的内部情绪状态的能力)可能反映了语言标签的知识,也可能从语言标签的知识中受益。本研究使用自然语言处理技术来研究青少年样本中的情绪词汇(EV;主动词汇中独特情绪术语的多样性)与情绪分化和抑郁之间的关系。我们测试了有关 EV-ED 联系的两个相互竞争的预注册(https://osf.io/4j75w/)模型。在词汇促进假说中,我们假设较大的EV可能会为ED提供信息,从而导致较大的EV与较大的ED相关联。在情感简洁性假说中,我们推测 ED 可能反映了更简洁的狭义情感体验,这可能导致较大的 EV 与较低的 ED 相关联。作为 2014 年至 2016 年进行的一项大型研究的一部分,社区青少年样本(N = 241,年龄 14-17 岁,主要为白人)完成了访谈、自我报告测量和生态瞬间评估。情绪简洁性是通过转录生活压力访谈录音中的语音样本得出的。与情绪简洁性假设一致,EV 和 ED 与负面情绪成反比。此外,较大的负性 EV 和较低的负性 ED 都与抑郁有独特的关联,这让人进一步怀疑自发语言中多种多样的负性 EV 是否从根本上适应了情绪功能。需要在更多不同的样本中进行重复研究,以扩大研究的普遍性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Putting it into words: Emotion vocabulary, emotion differentiation, and depression among adolescents.","authors":"Gwyneth A L DeLap, Vera Vine, Angela C Santee, Lisa R Starr","doi":"10.1037/emo0001429","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001429","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion differentiation (ED; the ability to distinguish discrete internal emotion states) may reflect or benefit from knowledge of linguistic labels. The present study uses natural language processing to examine how emotion vocabulary (EV; diversity of unique emotion terms within active vocabulary) relates to ED and depression in an adolescent sample. We tested two competing preregistered (https://osf.io/4j75w/) models regarding the EV-ED link. In the <i>lexical facilitation hypothesis</i>, we posited that larger EV may inform ED, perhaps resulting in larger EVs being associated with greater ED. In the <i>emotional concision hypothesis</i>, we theorized that ED may reflect narrower emotional experiences that are more succinctly labelled, which could result in larger EV being associated with lower ED. A community sample of adolescents (N = 241, ages 14-17, predominantly White) completed interviews, self-report measures, and ecological momentary assessments as part of a larger study conducted between 2014 and 2016. EV was derived using speech samples from transcribed recordings of life stress interviews. In line with the emotion concision hypothesis, EV and ED were inversely related for negative emotions. Moreover, <i>larger</i> negative EV and <i>lower</i> negative ED were each uniquely associated with depression, casting further doubt on whether diverse negative EVs within spontaneous language are fundamentally adaptive for emotional functioning. Replication in more diverse samples is needed to extend generalizability. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Momentary savoring in daily life in an adult life-span sample. 成人寿命样本中日常生活中的瞬间回味。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001423
Claire M Growney, Laura L Carstensen, Tammy English

Savoring moments can foster well-being. Older adults are theorized to prioritize emotional well-being in daily life, which directs their attention to positive aspects of life. In this study, with data collected from 2018 to 2021, 285 adults aged 25-85 completed an experience sampling procedure (six times a day for 10 days) where they reported their experienced emotions, whether they were savoring the moment, and how close they felt to their most recent social partner. They also completed a trait-level questionnaire on psychological well-being. Across the age range, individuals were more likely to savor moments when they were with close social partners. Older people were more likely than younger people to report savoring when experiencing high levels of positive affect. The tendency to savor was also tied to psychological well-being among individuals independent of their age. Findings highlight the relational aspect of savoring in daily contexts and suggest that savoring may contribute to well-being, helping to account for age advantages in well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

品味时刻可以促进幸福感。根据理论,老年人在日常生活中会优先考虑情感幸福,这将引导他们关注生活的积极方面。在这项研究中,285 名年龄在 25-85 岁之间的成年人完成了一项体验取样程序(每天六次,持续 10 天),他们报告了自己的体验情绪、是否在品味当下以及与最近的社交伙伴的亲密程度。他们还填写了一份心理健康特质问卷。从不同年龄段的人来看,当他们与亲密的社交伙伴在一起时,他们更有可能回味那一刻。与年轻人相比,老年人更倾向于在积极情绪高涨时细细品味。细细品味的倾向还与个人的心理健康息息相关,与年龄无关。研究结果凸显了在日常情境中回味的关系方面,并表明回味可能会促进幸福感,有助于解释幸福感的年龄优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Momentary savoring in daily life in an adult life-span sample.","authors":"Claire M Growney, Laura L Carstensen, Tammy English","doi":"10.1037/emo0001423","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001423","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Savoring moments can foster well-being. Older adults are theorized to prioritize emotional well-being in daily life, which directs their attention to positive aspects of life. In this study, with data collected from 2018 to 2021, 285 adults aged 25-85 completed an experience sampling procedure (six times a day for 10 days) where they reported their experienced emotions, whether they were savoring the moment, and how close they felt to their most recent social partner. They also completed a trait-level questionnaire on psychological well-being. Across the age range, individuals were more likely to savor moments when they were with close social partners. Older people were more likely than younger people to report savoring when experiencing high levels of positive affect. The tendency to savor was also tied to psychological well-being among individuals independent of their age. Findings highlight the relational aspect of savoring in daily contexts and suggest that savoring may contribute to well-being, helping to account for age advantages in well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural variation in the motivational correlates of gratitude. 感恩动机相关因素的文化差异。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001425
Allon Vishkin, Min Young Kim, Nevin Solak, Kinga Szymaniak, Cindel J M White, Shinobu Kitayama

Gratitude confers a sense of indebtedness to repay the benefactor, which poses a limitation on one's autonomy-an aversive experience in individualist cultures. Yet, gratitude is frequently valued and expressed in individualist cultures such as the United States. One solution to this dilemma is that gratitude has different aspects: It confers a sense of obligation but also strengthens social relations. Thus, gratitude might be associated more strongly with indebtedness in cultural contexts where autonomy is less valued, but it might be associated with a desire to be close to others in cultural contexts where autonomy is more valued. We tested how motivations for being indebted, for connecting to others, and for a hedonic emotional balance predict both gratitude to God and interpersonal gratitude in samples from the United States, India, Israel, Poland, South Korea, and Turkey (N = 2,093). Results revealed substantial cultural variation in how these correlates are associated with gratitude. We discuss how gratitude can inform cultural differences in how relationships are construed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

感恩会让人产生一种要报答恩人的亏欠感,从而限制了一个人的自主性--这在个人主义文化中是一种令人厌恶的体验。然而,在美国等个人主义文化中,感恩却经常受到重视并得到表达。解决这一难题的一个办法是,感恩具有不同的方面:它既能赋予人一种义务感,又能加强社会关系。因此,在不太重视自主性的文化背景下,感恩可能与负债有更强的关联,但在更重视自主性的文化背景下,感恩可能与亲近他人的愿望有关联。我们对来自美国、印度、以色列、波兰、韩国和土耳其的样本(N = 2,093)进行了测试,以了解负债动机、与他人联系的动机和享乐主义情感平衡的动机如何预测对神的感激之情和人际感激之情。研究结果表明,这些相关因素与感恩的关系存在很大的文化差异。我们将讨论如何通过感恩来理解人际关系的文化差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Cultural variation in the motivational correlates of gratitude.","authors":"Allon Vishkin, Min Young Kim, Nevin Solak, Kinga Szymaniak, Cindel J M White, Shinobu Kitayama","doi":"10.1037/emo0001425","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001425","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Gratitude confers a sense of indebtedness to repay the benefactor, which poses a limitation on one's autonomy-an aversive experience in individualist cultures. Yet, gratitude is frequently valued and expressed in individualist cultures such as the United States. One solution to this dilemma is that gratitude has different aspects: It confers a sense of obligation but also strengthens social relations. Thus, gratitude might be associated more strongly with indebtedness in cultural contexts where autonomy is less valued, but it might be associated with a desire to be close to others in cultural contexts where autonomy is more valued. We tested how motivations for being indebted, for connecting to others, and for a hedonic emotional balance predict both gratitude to God and interpersonal gratitude in samples from the United States, India, Israel, Poland, South Korea, and Turkey (<i>N</i> = 2,093). Results revealed substantial cultural variation in how these correlates are associated with gratitude. We discuss how gratitude can inform cultural differences in how relationships are construed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the association among adolescents' emotional clarity, emotion differentiation, and the regulation of negative and positive affect using a daily diary approach. 利用每日日记法研究青少年情绪清晰度、情绪分化以及消极和积极情绪调节之间的关联。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001424
Nicola Hohensee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack

Emotional clarity and emotion differentiation (ED) are two core aspects of the application of emotional knowledge. During adolescence, novel emotional experiences result in temporary decreases of differentiation and clarity. These temporary difficulties might profoundly impact choices of regulatory strategies. And indeed, prior research has shown that lower emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are each associated with higher use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in youth. The two constructs, however, are rarely examined together, and it remains unclear how they are associated in daily life, particularly in children and adolescents. In addition, previous studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. To address these gaps, the present study used an intensive longitudinal design in youth. Between June 2021 and March 2022, 172 children and adolescents (M = 12.99 years) completed a 28-day diary (> 3,500 entries in total) reporting daily affect, emotional clarity, and the use of five emotion regulation strategies in response to negative and positive affect (i.e., rumination, dampening, behavioral avoidance, negative and positive suppression). As predicted, on both between- and within-person levels, higher emotional clarity was associated with decreased use of all maladaptive emotion regulation strategies after adjusting for mean affect intensity. Results for emotion differentiation were mostly nonsignificant. Only higher daily positive emotion differentiation was associated with decreased rumination. In sum, this innovative study explores multiple aspects of emotional knowledge usage and regulation during a critical developmental stage and emphasizes the role of emotional clarity in the regulation of negative and positive affect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪清晰度和情绪分化(ED)是情绪知识应用的两个核心方面。在青春期,新的情绪体验会导致分化和清晰度的暂时下降。这些暂时性的困难可能会对调节策略的选择产生深远影响。事实上,先前的研究已经表明,情绪清晰度和情绪分化的降低都与青少年更多使用所谓的不良情绪调节策略有关。然而,很少有人把这两个概念放在一起研究,也不清楚它们在日常生活中是如何联系在一起的,尤其是在儿童和青少年中。此外,以往的研究主要关注消极情绪的调节,而非积极情绪的调节。为了弥补这些不足,本研究对青少年进行了深入的纵向研究。在 2021 年 6 月至 2022 年 3 月期间,172 名儿童和青少年(中=12.99 岁)完成了为期 28 天的日记(共计超过 3500 条),报告了日常情绪、情绪清晰度以及针对消极和积极情绪所使用的五种情绪调节策略(即反刍、抑制、行为回避、消极和积极抑制)。正如预测的那样,在调整平均情绪强度后,在人与人之间和人与人之间的层面上,情绪清晰度越高,使用所有不良情绪调节策略的次数就越少。情绪分化的结果大多不显著。只有较高的日常积极情绪分化与反刍的减少有关。总之,这项创新性研究探索了关键发展阶段中情绪知识使用和调节的多个方面,并强调了情绪清晰度在调节消极和积极情绪中的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Examining the association among adolescents' emotional clarity, emotion differentiation, and the regulation of negative and positive affect using a daily diary approach.","authors":"Nicola Hohensee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack","doi":"10.1037/emo0001424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional clarity and emotion differentiation (ED) are two core aspects of the application of emotional knowledge. During adolescence, novel emotional experiences result in temporary decreases of differentiation and clarity. These temporary difficulties might profoundly impact choices of regulatory strategies. And indeed, prior research has shown that lower emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are each associated with higher use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in youth. The two constructs, however, are rarely examined together, and it remains unclear how they are associated in daily life, particularly in children and adolescents. In addition, previous studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. To address these gaps, the present study used an intensive longitudinal design in youth. Between June 2021 and March 2022, 172 children and adolescents (<i>M</i> = 12.99 years) completed a 28-day diary (> 3,500 entries in total) reporting daily affect, emotional clarity, and the use of five emotion regulation strategies in response to negative and positive affect (i.e., rumination, dampening, behavioral avoidance, negative and positive suppression). As predicted, on both between- and within-person levels, higher emotional clarity was associated with decreased use of all maladaptive emotion regulation strategies after adjusting for mean affect intensity. Results for emotion differentiation were mostly nonsignificant. Only higher daily positive emotion differentiation was associated with decreased rumination. In sum, this innovative study explores multiple aspects of emotional knowledge usage and regulation during a critical developmental stage and emphasizes the role of emotional clarity in the regulation of negative and positive affect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interpersonal emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in parent-adolescent dyads: A daily-diary investigation. 父母与青少年之间的人际情绪调节和抑郁症状:每日日记调查
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001418
Reuma Gadassi-Polack, Marcia Questel, Haran Sened, Hannah E Marshall, Grace J Chen, Eva J Geiger, Tom Bar Yosef, Jutta Joormann

Deficient parental extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (IER, how people regulate others' emotions) is a known risk factor for adolescent depression. Although IER and depression development are transactional, dyadic processes, previous work has almost exclusively focused on how parental IER is associated with adolescent depression. The association between parental IER and adolescent depression, and the associations between adolescent IER and adolescent and parental depression have received little attention. Moreover, most studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. We address these gaps by examining associations between parent and adolescent IER and depressive symptoms using the actor-partner interdependence model framework. For 28 days, 112 parent-adolescent dyads (12-18-year-old adolescents) completed a dyadic daily diary, reporting their own depressive symptoms and IER strategies employed in response to dyad members' positive and negative affect. Our results, based on 5,442 data points, show that the use of positive- and negative-affect-worsening IER is associated with more depression in the regulator (be it parent or adolescent). Surprisingly, parents' use of more negative-affect-improving IER was associated with higher levels of their own and adolescents' depression. Finally, adolescents' use of positive-affect-improving IER was associated with their own decreased depression. Overall, parents (vs. adolescents) used more negative- and positive-affect-improving extrinsic IER, whereas adolescents used more positive-affect-worsening extrinsic IER. Our results highlight the importance of using dyadic designs in studying depression and IER, as well as the need to consider who is regulating, the valence of the affect regulated, and the type of strategy used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

父母外在人际情绪调节(IER,人们如何调节他人的情绪)不足是青少年抑郁的一个已知风险因素。虽然人际情绪调节与抑郁的发展是一个相互影响的双向过程,但以往的研究几乎都集中在父母的人际情绪调节与青少年抑郁之间的关系上。父母的 IER 与青少年抑郁之间的关系,以及青少年的 IER 与青少年和父母抑郁之间的关系很少受到关注。此外,大多数研究关注的是消极情绪的调节,而不是积极情绪的调节。为了弥补这些不足,我们采用行为者-伙伴相互依存模型框架,研究了父母和青少年 IER 与抑郁症状之间的关联。在为期 28 天的时间里,112 个父母-青少年组合(12-18 岁的青少年)完成了组合每日日记,报告他们自己的抑郁症状以及针对组合成员的积极和消极情绪所采取的 IER 策略。我们根据 5442 个数据点得出的结果表明,使用积极和消极影响恶化的 IER 与调节者(无论是父母还是青少年)抑郁程度的增加有关。令人惊讶的是,父母使用更多的改善负面影响的 IER 与他们自己和青少年更高水平的抑郁有关。最后,青少年使用积极影响改善型 IER 与他们自身抑郁程度的降低有关。总体而言,父母(与青少年相比)使用了更多的消极和积极影响改善型外在IER,而青少年则使用了更多的积极影响恶化型外在IER。我们的研究结果凸显了在研究抑郁和IER时采用双向设计的重要性,以及考虑谁在调节、所调节的情感的价值以及所使用的策略类型的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Interpersonal emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in parent-adolescent dyads: A daily-diary investigation.","authors":"Reuma Gadassi-Polack, Marcia Questel, Haran Sened, Hannah E Marshall, Grace J Chen, Eva J Geiger, Tom Bar Yosef, Jutta Joormann","doi":"10.1037/emo0001418","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001418","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Deficient parental extrinsic <i>interpersonal emotion regulation</i> (IER, how people regulate others' emotions) is a known risk factor for adolescent depression. Although IER and depression development are transactional, dyadic processes, previous work has almost exclusively focused on how parental IER is associated with adolescent depression. The association between parental IER and adolescent depression, and the associations between adolescent IER and adolescent and parental depression have received little attention. Moreover, most studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. We address these gaps by examining associations between parent and adolescent IER and depressive symptoms using the actor-partner interdependence model framework. For 28 days, 112 parent-adolescent dyads (12-18-year-old adolescents) completed a dyadic daily diary, reporting their own depressive symptoms and IER strategies employed in response to dyad members' positive and negative affect. Our results, based on 5,442 data points, show that the use of positive- and negative-affect-worsening IER is associated with more depression in the regulator (be it parent or adolescent). Surprisingly, parents' use of more negative-affect-improving IER was associated with higher levels of their own and adolescents' depression. Finally, adolescents' use of positive-affect-improving IER was associated with their own decreased depression. Overall, parents (vs. adolescents) used more negative- and positive-affect-improving extrinsic IER, whereas adolescents used more positive-affect-worsening extrinsic IER. Our results highlight the importance of using dyadic designs in studying depression and IER, as well as the need to consider who is regulating, the valence of the affect regulated, and the type of strategy used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unpacking the components of positive affect variability: Implications for psychological health across contexts. 解读积极情绪变异的组成部分:不同情境下对心理健康的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001416
David J Disabato, Emily A Gawlik, Pallavi Aurora, Karin G Coifman

Prior research suggests variability of positive affect (PA), or the degree to which an individual's experience of PA is variable rather than stable, is associated with worse psychological health. However, it is unclear whether different aspects of PA variability serve different psychological functions. One possibility is that changes in PA in response to rewarding contexts, or PA reactivity, serve a healthy function, while general instability of PA from one moment to the next serves an unhealthy function. The current investigation separated out PA reactivity to pleasant activities from general PA instability. We tested associations in three experience-sampling studies collected between 2012 and 2020 (N = 323). An internal meta-analysis revealed a significant association between PA reactivity to pleasant activities and less well-being. Moderation by average levels of PA was present but inconsistent across studies. We discuss how PA reactions to rewarding contexts may not necessarily reflect healthy emotion regulation and consider that "mood brightening" effects in daily life may indicate ill-being rather than well-being. Caution is warranted when interpreting the primary findings, as the indirect effect of PA reactivity was significant in only one of the three individual studies, and the effect was only found for the outcome of well-being and not distress. Results can be most confidently generalized to White adults living in the Midwest region of the United States. Future research should test not only the intensity of PA reactivity to rewarding contexts but also how long a person can sustain elevated PA-in relation to psychological health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究表明,积极情绪(PA)的可变性,或者说一个人的积极情绪体验可变而非稳定的程度,与心理健康状况的恶化有关。然而,目前还不清楚积极情感变化的不同方面是否具有不同的心理功能。一种可能的情况是,PA 对奖励情境的响应变化或 PA 反应性具有健康功能,而 PA 从一而终的普遍不稳定性则具有不健康功能。目前的调查将对愉快活动的反应性与一般的反应性不稳定性区分开来。我们在 2012 年至 2020 年间收集的三项经验取样研究中测试了两者之间的关联(N = 323)。一项内部荟萃分析显示,愉快活动的 PA 反应性与幸福感较低之间存在显著关联。平均 PA 水平的调节作用是存在的,但在不同研究中并不一致。我们讨论了对有益情境的 PA 反应不一定能反映健康的情绪调节,并认为日常生活中的 "情绪提振 "效应可能预示着不健康而非幸福。在解释主要研究结果时需要谨慎,因为在三项单独的研究中,只有一项研究的PA反应性的间接效应是显著的,而且该效应只对幸福感而非痛苦的结果有影响。研究结果最有信心推广到生活在美国中西部地区的白人成年人。未来的研究不仅要测试PA对奖励情境的反应强度,还要测试一个人能够维持多长时间的PA升高--这与心理健康有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Unpacking the components of positive affect variability: Implications for psychological health across contexts.","authors":"David J Disabato, Emily A Gawlik, Pallavi Aurora, Karin G Coifman","doi":"10.1037/emo0001416","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research suggests variability of positive affect (PA), or the degree to which an individual's experience of PA is variable rather than stable, is associated with worse psychological health. However, it is unclear whether different aspects of PA variability serve different psychological functions. One possibility is that changes in PA in response to rewarding contexts, or PA reactivity, serve a healthy function, while general instability of PA from one moment to the next serves an unhealthy function. The current investigation separated out PA reactivity to pleasant activities from general PA instability. We tested associations in three experience-sampling studies collected between 2012 and 2020 (<i>N</i> = 323). An internal meta-analysis revealed a significant association between PA reactivity to pleasant activities and <i>less</i> well-being. Moderation by average levels of PA was present but inconsistent across studies. We discuss how PA reactions to rewarding contexts may <i>not</i> necessarily reflect healthy emotion regulation and consider that \"mood brightening\" effects in daily life may indicate ill-being rather than well-being. Caution is warranted when interpreting the primary findings, as the indirect effect of PA reactivity was significant in only one of the three individual studies, and the effect was only found for the outcome of well-being and not distress. Results can be most confidently generalized to White adults living in the Midwest region of the United States. Future research should test not only the intensity of PA reactivity to rewarding contexts but also how long a person can sustain elevated PA-in relation to psychological health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Motivated to feel better and doing something about it: Cross-cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation during COVID-19. 有动力感觉更好并为此做些什么:COVID-19 期间动机情绪调节的跨文化差异。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001403
Lisya Kaspi, Danfei Hu, Allon Vishkin, Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Yuri Miyamoto, Jan Cieciuch, Akiva Cohen, Yukiko Uchida, Min Young Kim, Xiaoqin Wang, Jiang Qiu, Michaela Riediger, Antje Rauers, Yaniv Hanoch, Maya Tamir

Emotion regulation is linked to adaptive psychological outcomes. To engage in such regulation, people must be motivated to do it. Given that people in different countries vary in how they think about unpleasant emotions, we expected motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions to differ across countries. Furthermore, given that emotion regulation strategies operate in the service of motivation, we expected people who are less motivated to decrease unpleasant emotions to use emotion regulation strategies less across countries. To test these predictions, we conducted two studies during the COVID-19 pandemic: Study 1 in 2020 (N = 1,329) and Study 2 in 2021 (N = 1,279). We assessed the motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions and the use of emotion regulation strategies among members of East Asian countries (i.e., Japan, South Korea, and China) and Western countries (i.e., United States, United Kingdom, and Germany). Because we found substantial variation within these two broader cultural categories, we examined motivation and overall strategy use in emotion regulation at the country level. In both studies, motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was the lowest in Japan and relatively high in the United States. As expected, across countries, weaker motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was associated with using emotion regulation strategies less. We discuss implications of our findings for understanding cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪调节与适应性心理结果有关。要进行这种调节,人们必须有这样做的动机。鉴于不同国家的人对不愉快情绪的看法各不相同,我们预计不同国家的人减少不愉快情绪的动机也不尽相同。此外,鉴于情绪调节策略是为动机服务的,我们预计那些减少不愉快情绪的动机较弱的人在不同国家使用情绪调节策略的程度也会较低。为了验证这些预测,我们在 COVID-19 大流行期间进行了两项研究:研究 1 于 2020 年进行(人数 = 1,329 人),研究 2 于 2021 年进行(人数 = 1,279 人)。我们评估了东亚国家(即日本、韩国和中国)和西方国家(即美国、英国和德国)成员减少不愉快情绪的动机和情绪调节策略的使用情况。由于我们发现在这两个更广泛的文化类别中存在很大差异,因此我们在国家层面上研究了情绪调节的动机和总体策略使用情况。在这两项研究中,日本减少不愉快情绪的动机最低,而美国则相对较高。不出所料,在不同国家,减少不愉快情绪的动机越弱,情绪调节策略的使用就越少。我们讨论了我们的发现对于理解情绪调节动机的文化差异的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Motivated to feel better and doing something about it: Cross-cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation during COVID-19.","authors":"Lisya Kaspi, Danfei Hu, Allon Vishkin, Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Yuri Miyamoto, Jan Cieciuch, Akiva Cohen, Yukiko Uchida, Min Young Kim, Xiaoqin Wang, Jiang Qiu, Michaela Riediger, Antje Rauers, Yaniv Hanoch, Maya Tamir","doi":"10.1037/emo0001403","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001403","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion regulation is linked to adaptive psychological outcomes. To engage in such regulation, people must be motivated to do it. Given that people in different countries vary in how they think about unpleasant emotions, we expected motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions to differ across countries. Furthermore, given that emotion regulation strategies operate in the service of motivation, we expected people who are less motivated to decrease unpleasant emotions to use emotion regulation strategies less across countries. To test these predictions, we conducted two studies during the COVID-19 pandemic: Study 1 in 2020 (<i>N</i> = 1,329) and Study 2 in 2021 (<i>N</i> = 1,279). We assessed the motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions and the use of emotion regulation strategies among members of East Asian countries (i.e., Japan, South Korea, and China) and Western countries (i.e., United States, United Kingdom, and Germany). Because we found substantial variation within these two broader cultural categories, we examined motivation and overall strategy use in emotion regulation at the country level. In both studies, motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was the lowest in Japan and relatively high in the United States. As expected, across countries, weaker motivation to decrease unpleasant emotions was associated with using emotion regulation strategies less. We discuss implications of our findings for understanding cultural differences in motivated emotion regulation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1