首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Divergence of children's friendships and intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation: Factoring in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation strategy use. 儿童友谊的分化与内在人际情绪调节:考虑外在人际情绪调节策略的使用。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001411
Kyongboon Kwon, Theodore S Lentz, A Michele Lease

As peers become a major part of children's social life, children seek out and provide support for each other when experiencing strong emotions. We examined children's intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (IER; children's emotion regulation support seeking from peers) and extrinsic IER (regulation strategies peers provide to help regulate emotion). We examined the extent to which (a) the peers whom children turn to for intrinsic IER diverge from those they consider close friends and (b) extrinsic IER strategy provided by peers is associated with intrinsic IER seeking. Study participants were 131 (67 girls) fourth and fifth grade children from six classrooms from urban settings in a U.S. Midwest state. Based on a peer nomination procedure, children nominated classmates who are their close friends, peers they turn to when sad or angry (i.e., intrinsic IER), and peers who help them regulate sadness or anger through listening and talking (i.e., extrinsic IER). We used social network analysis methods, with classrooms as a unit of analysis, to examine the pattern of ties in the nomination data. We demonstrated that (a) children's intrinsic IER ties are related to, yet distinct from, close friendship; (b) peers whom children turn to for intrinsic IER differ, to some extent, for sadness versus anger; and (c) extrinsic IER strategy use is significantly associated with intrinsic IER after taking friendship ties into account. The findings suggest that emotion regulatory needs channel children's social interactions, and extrinsic IER strategy accounts for some of the divergence of intrinsic IER ties from friendship. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

随着同伴成为儿童社会生活的重要组成部分,儿童在经历强烈情绪时会寻求同伴的支持并为其提供帮助。我们研究了儿童内在的人际情绪调节(IER;儿童向同伴寻求情绪调节支持)和外在的人际情绪调节(同伴提供的帮助调节情绪的调节策略)。我们研究了(a)儿童寻求内在人际情绪调节的同伴与他们认为亲密的朋友之间的差异程度,以及(b)同伴提供的外在人际情绪调节策略与寻求内在人际情绪调节的关联程度。研究对象是来自美国中西部某州城市环境中六个班级的 131 名四年级和五年级学生(67 名女生)。根据同伴提名程序,孩子们提名了与他们关系密切的同学、他们在悲伤或愤怒时求助的同伴(即内在IER),以及通过倾听和交谈帮助他们调节悲伤或愤怒情绪的同伴(即外在IER)。我们采用社会网络分析方法,以班级为分析单位,研究了提名数据中的联系模式。我们的研究表明:(a)儿童的内在IER联系与亲密的友谊有关,但又不同于亲密的友谊;(b)儿童向哪些同伴寻求内在IER,在某种程度上,悲伤与愤怒的情况有所不同;以及(c)在考虑了友谊联系之后,外在IER策略的使用与内在IER有显著关联。研究结果表明,情绪调节需求引导着儿童的社会交往,而外在 IER 策略在一定程度上解释了内在 IER 关系与友谊关系之间的差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Divergence of children's friendships and intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation: Factoring in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation strategy use.","authors":"Kyongboon Kwon, Theodore S Lentz, A Michele Lease","doi":"10.1037/emo0001411","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001411","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As peers become a major part of children's social life, children seek out and provide support for each other when experiencing strong emotions. We examined children's intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (IER; children's emotion regulation support seeking from peers) and extrinsic IER (regulation strategies peers provide to help regulate emotion). We examined the extent to which (a) the peers whom children turn to for intrinsic IER diverge from those they consider close friends and (b) extrinsic IER strategy provided by peers is associated with intrinsic IER seeking. Study participants were 131 (67 girls) fourth and fifth grade children from six classrooms from urban settings in a U.S. Midwest state. Based on a peer nomination procedure, children nominated classmates who are their close friends, peers they turn to when sad or angry (i.e., intrinsic IER), and peers who help them regulate sadness or anger through listening and talking (i.e., extrinsic IER). We used social network analysis methods, with classrooms as a unit of analysis, to examine the pattern of ties in the nomination data. We demonstrated that (a) children's intrinsic IER ties are related to, yet distinct from, close friendship; (b) peers whom children turn to for intrinsic IER differ, to some extent, for sadness versus anger; and (c) extrinsic IER strategy use is significantly associated with intrinsic IER after taking friendship ties into account. The findings suggest that emotion regulatory needs channel children's social interactions, and extrinsic IER strategy accounts for some of the divergence of intrinsic IER ties from friendship. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298826","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What makes us feel good? A data-driven investigation of positive emotion experience. 是什么让我们感觉良好?对积极情绪体验的数据驱动调查。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001417
Roza G Kamiloğlu, İnan Utku Türkmen, Taha Eren Sarnıç, Dana Landman, Disa A Sauter

What does it mean to feel good? Is our experience of gazing in awe at a majestic mountain fundamentally different than erupting with triumph when our favorite team wins the championship? Here, we use a semantic space approach to test which positive emotional experiences are distinct from each other based on in-depth personal narratives of experiences involving 22 positive emotions (n = 165; 3,592 emotional events). A bottom-up computational analysis was applied to the transcribed text, with unsupervised clustering employed to maximize internal granular consistency (i.e., the clusters being maximally different and maximally internally homogeneous). The analysis yielded four emotions that map onto distinct clusters of subjective experiences: amusement, interest, lust, and tenderness. The application of the semantic space approach to in-depth personal accounts yields a nuanced understanding of positive emotional experiences. Moreover, this analytical method allows for the bottom-up development of emotion taxonomies, showcasing its potential for broader applications in the study of subjective experiences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

感觉良好意味着什么?我们对雄伟高山的敬畏之情与我们最喜爱的球队夺冠时的喜悦之情是否有本质区别?在此,我们使用语义空间方法,根据涉及 22 种积极情绪的个人深度叙述(n = 165;3,592 个情绪事件),测试哪些积极情绪体验彼此不同。我们对转录文本进行了自下而上的计算分析,并采用了无监督聚类,以最大限度地提高内部粒度一致性(即聚类具有最大程度的差异和最大程度的内部同质性)。分析得出了四种情绪,分别映射到主观体验的不同聚类上:娱乐、兴趣、欲望和温柔。将语义空间方法应用于深入的个人陈述,可以获得对积极情绪体验的细致入微的理解。此外,这种分析方法允许自下而上地发展情绪分类法,展示了其在主观体验研究中更广泛应用的潜力。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"What makes us feel good? A data-driven investigation of positive emotion experience.","authors":"Roza G Kamiloğlu, İnan Utku Türkmen, Taha Eren Sarnıç, Dana Landman, Disa A Sauter","doi":"10.1037/emo0001417","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What does it mean to feel good? Is our experience of gazing in awe at a majestic mountain fundamentally different than erupting with triumph when our favorite team wins the championship? Here, we use a semantic space approach to test which positive emotional experiences are distinct from each other based on in-depth personal narratives of experiences involving 22 positive emotions (<i>n</i> = 165; 3,592 emotional events). A bottom-up computational analysis was applied to the transcribed text, with unsupervised clustering employed to maximize internal granular consistency (i.e., the clusters being maximally different and maximally internally homogeneous). The analysis yielded four emotions that map onto distinct clusters of subjective experiences: amusement, interest, lust, and tenderness. The application of the semantic space approach to in-depth personal accounts yields a nuanced understanding of positive emotional experiences. Moreover, this analytical method allows for the bottom-up development of emotion taxonomies, showcasing its potential for broader applications in the study of subjective experiences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Smiling and frowning induced by facial neuromuscular electrical stimulation (fNMES) modulate felt emotion and physiology. 面部神经肌肉电刺激(fNMES)诱发的微笑和皱眉会调节感觉情绪和生理。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001408
Themis Nikolas Efthimiou, Joshua Baker, Arthur Elsenaar, Marc Mehu, Sebastian Korb

According to the facial feedback hypothesis, feedback from facial muscles can initiate and modulate a person's emotional state. This assumption is debated, however, and existing research has arguably suffered from a lack of control over which facial muscles are activated, when, to what degree, and for how long. To overcome these limitations, we carried out a preregistered experiment including 58 participants. Facial neuromuscular electrical stimulation (fNMES) was applied to the bilateral zygomaticus major and depressor anguli oris muscles for 5 s at 100% and 50% of the participants' individual motor threshold. After each trial, participants reported their emotional valence and intensity and levels of experienced discomfort. Facial muscle activations were verified with automatic video coding; heart rate and electrodermal activity were recorded throughout. Results showed that muscle activation through fNMES, even when controlling for fNMES-induced discomfort, modulated participants' emotional state as expected, with more positive emotions reported after stronger stimulation of the zygomaticus major than the depressor anguli oris muscle. The addition of expression-congruent emotional images increased the effect. Moreover, fNMES intensity predicted intensity ratings, reduced HR, and skin conductance response. The finding that changes in felt emotion can be induced through brief and controlled activation of specific facial muscles is in line with the facial feedback hypothesis and offers exciting opportunities for translational intervention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据面部反馈假说,面部肌肉的反馈可以启动和调节人的情绪状态。然而,这一假设还存在争议,现有的研究可以说缺乏对哪些面部肌肉被激活、何时激活、激活程度以及激活时间长短的控制。为了克服这些局限性,我们进行了一项预先登记的实验,共有 58 人参加。面部神经肌肉电刺激(fNMES)作用于双侧颧骨大肌和口角下压肌,持续 5 秒钟,刺激强度分别为参与者个人运动阈值的 100%和 50%。每次试验后,参与者都要报告自己的情绪价值和强度以及体验到的不适程度。面部肌肉激活情况通过自动视频编码进行验证;心率和皮电活动被全程记录。结果表明,即使控制了 fNMES 引起的不适感,通过 fNMES 进行的肌肉激活也能调节参与者的情绪状态,正如预期的那样,刺激颧大肌比刺激口角下压肌后,参与者会报告更多的积极情绪。添加表情一致的情绪图像会增强这种效果。此外,fNMES 强度可预测强度评级、降低的心率和皮肤电导反应。这一发现与面部反馈假说一致,并为转化干预提供了令人兴奋的机会。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Smiling and frowning induced by facial neuromuscular electrical stimulation (fNMES) modulate felt emotion and physiology.","authors":"Themis Nikolas Efthimiou, Joshua Baker, Arthur Elsenaar, Marc Mehu, Sebastian Korb","doi":"10.1037/emo0001408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to the facial feedback hypothesis, feedback from facial muscles can initiate and modulate a person's emotional state. This assumption is debated, however, and existing research has arguably suffered from a lack of control over which facial muscles are activated, when, to what degree, and for how long. To overcome these limitations, we carried out a preregistered experiment including 58 participants. Facial neuromuscular electrical stimulation (fNMES) was applied to the bilateral zygomaticus major and depressor anguli oris muscles for 5 s at 100% and 50% of the participants' individual motor threshold. After each trial, participants reported their emotional valence and intensity and levels of experienced discomfort. Facial muscle activations were verified with automatic video coding; heart rate and electrodermal activity were recorded throughout. Results showed that muscle activation through fNMES, even when controlling for fNMES-induced discomfort, modulated participants' emotional state as expected, with more positive emotions reported after stronger stimulation of the zygomaticus major than the depressor anguli oris muscle. The addition of expression-congruent emotional images increased the effect. Moreover, fNMES intensity predicted intensity ratings, reduced HR, and skin conductance response. The finding that changes in felt emotion can be induced through brief and controlled activation of specific facial muscles is in line with the facial feedback hypothesis and offers exciting opportunities for translational intervention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Negative (but not positive) affective episodic future thinking enhances proactive behavior in 5-year-old children. 消极(而非积极)情感性偶发未来思维会增强 5 岁儿童的主动行为。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001345
Felix Schreiber, Silvia Schneider, Albert Newen, Babett Voigt

Envisioning the future and how you may feel (affective episodic future thinking [EFT]) helps adults to act in favor for their future self, according to manifold experiments. The current study tested whether and how affective EFT also helps children to behave more proactively, that is, to self-initially prepare for an upcoming event. Five-year-old (N = 90) children (data collected from 2021 to 2022) were instructed to mentally imagine how they would feel after successfully managing an upcoming test (positive affective EFT), how they would feel after failing to do so (negative affective EFT), or they were reminded of an upcoming test without a prompt to imagine (control condition, random assignment). Proactive behavior was indicated by children's choice to play one of three games before the actual test (one of the games was announced to be the test game). Mechanisms (e.g., motivation to win, psychological distance, current affect) and moderators (ability of episodically thinking about the future in everyday life, behavioral inhibition, and behavioral approach) for the possible effects of affective EFT were explored. Children in the negative affective EFT condition chose the target game significantly above chance level and more often than children in the control group, whereas children in the positive affective EFT condition did not. This effect was independent of the assumed mediators and moderators. Findings are discussed in the context of the theoretical and empirical literature on affective EFT in adults and suggestions for future studies are given. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

多方面的实验表明,设想未来和自己可能的感受(情感性发作性未来思维 [EFT])有助于成年人采取有利于未来自己的行动。本研究测试了情感外显未来思维是否以及如何帮助儿童更加积极主动地行动,即为即将发生的事件做好自我准备。研究人员指导五岁儿童(N = 90)(数据收集于 2021 年至 2022 年)在头脑中想象他们在成功应对即将到来的测试后的感受(积极情感性 EFT)、失败后的感受(消极情感性 EFT),或者在不提示想象的情况下提醒他们即将到来的测试(对照条件,随机分配)。孩子们在实际测试前选择玩三个游戏中的一个(其中一个游戏被宣布为测试游戏),这表明他们的行为是积极主动的。研究人员探讨了情绪化 EFT 可能产生影响的机制(如获胜动机、心理距离、当前情绪)和调节因素(在日常生活中偶发思考未来的能力、行为抑制和行为方法)。消极情绪 EFT 条件下的儿童选择目标游戏的次数明显高于对照组儿童的机会水平,而积极情绪 EFT 条件下的儿童则没有。这种效应与假定的中介和调节因素无关。本研究结合有关成人情感性 EFT 的理论和实证文献对研究结果进行了讨论,并对今后的研究提出了建议。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Negative (but not positive) affective episodic future thinking enhances proactive behavior in 5-year-old children.","authors":"Felix Schreiber, Silvia Schneider, Albert Newen, Babett Voigt","doi":"10.1037/emo0001345","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001345","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Envisioning the future and how you may feel (affective episodic future thinking [EFT]) helps adults to act in favor for their future self, according to manifold experiments. The current study tested whether and how affective EFT also helps children to behave more proactively, that is, to self-initially prepare for an upcoming event. Five-year-old (<i>N</i> = 90) children (data collected from 2021 to 2022) were instructed to mentally imagine how they would feel after successfully managing an upcoming test (positive affective EFT), how they would feel after failing to do so (negative affective EFT), or they were reminded of an upcoming test without a prompt to imagine (control condition, random assignment). Proactive behavior was indicated by children's choice to play one of three games before the actual test (one of the games was announced to be the test game). Mechanisms (e.g., motivation to win, psychological distance, current affect) and moderators (ability of episodically thinking about the future in everyday life, behavioral inhibition, and behavioral approach) for the possible effects of affective EFT were explored. Children in the negative affective EFT condition chose the target game significantly above chance level and more often than children in the control group, whereas children in the positive affective EFT condition did not. This effect was independent of the assumed mediators and moderators. Findings are discussed in the context of the theoretical and empirical literature on affective EFT in adults and suggestions for future studies are given. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1481-1493"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional content reduces the cognitive effort invested in processing the credibility of social (mis)information. 情感内容可减少处理社会(错误)信息可信度时投入的认知努力。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001355
Julia Baum, Romy Frömer, Rasha Abdel Rahman

Emotionality likely is a key factor affecting our susceptibility to misinformation. However, the mechanisms underlying this observation are not well understood. Specifically, when people derive social information from person-related news, they rely predominantly on emotional content, apparently unperturbed by the credibility of the source. To help explain this bias, we here contrast two hypotheses of information processing reflected in changes in pupil size during news-based judgments: Emotion and cognitive effort. Thirty participants were first exposed to websites of well-known trusted or distrusted news media sources exhibiting headlines about unfamiliar persons, followed by social judgments. As expected, emotional relative to neutral headline contents lead to faster and more strongly valenced judgments. In line with the cognitive effort hypothesis, credibility modulated pupil size with larger pupils for headlines from distrusted sources, however only in response to neutral headline contents. Source credibility did not modulate pupil size in response to emotional headline contents. Instead, pupil size was smaller for emotional compared to neutral headlines for both trusted and distrusted sources. This pattern of findings suggests that emotional contents yield fluent social judgments that are made with relatively little mental effort-even if based on untrustworthy news. Cognitive resources to evaluate the credibility of news may primarily be allocated when emotional contents providing (false) fluency are not available. This insight into the biases underlying the processing of potential misinformation may be used as a protection against biased opinions and judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪可能是影响我们对错误信息易感性的一个关键因素。然而,人们对这一现象的内在机制还不甚了解。具体来说,当人们从与人相关的新闻中获取社会信息时,他们主要依赖于情感内容,显然不受信息来源可信度的影响。为了帮助解释这种偏差,我们在此对基于新闻判断时瞳孔大小变化所反映的两种信息处理假设进行了对比:情绪和认知努力。我们首先让 30 名参与者浏览知名的、可信的或不可信的新闻媒体来源的网站,网站上展示的是关于陌生人物的标题,然后让他们进行社会判断。不出所料,相对于中性的标题内容,情绪化的内容会导致更快、更强烈的判断。与认知努力假说一致的是,可信度会调节瞳孔大小,不信任来源的标题会使瞳孔变大,但仅限于中性标题内容。来源可信度并不会调节情绪化标题内容的瞳孔大小。相反,与中性标题相比,对于可信和不可信来源的情感标题,瞳孔都较小。这种发现模式表明,情绪化的内容能产生流畅的社会判断,即使是基于不可信的新闻,也只需花费相对较少的脑力就能做出判断。当情感内容无法提供(虚假的)流畅性时,评估新闻可信度的认知资源可能会主要被分配。这种对潜在错误信息的处理过程中存在的偏见的洞察力,可以用来防止出现有偏见的观点和判断。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Emotional content reduces the cognitive effort invested in processing the credibility of social (mis)information.","authors":"Julia Baum, Romy Frömer, Rasha Abdel Rahman","doi":"10.1037/emo0001355","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001355","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotionality likely is a key factor affecting our susceptibility to misinformation. However, the mechanisms underlying this observation are not well understood. Specifically, when people derive social information from person-related news, they rely predominantly on emotional content, apparently unperturbed by the credibility of the source. To help explain this bias, we here contrast two hypotheses of information processing reflected in changes in pupil size during news-based judgments: Emotion and cognitive effort. Thirty participants were first exposed to websites of well-known trusted or distrusted news media sources exhibiting headlines about unfamiliar persons, followed by social judgments. As expected, emotional relative to neutral headline contents lead to faster and more strongly valenced judgments. In line with the cognitive effort hypothesis, credibility modulated pupil size with larger pupils for headlines from distrusted sources, however only in response to neutral headline contents. Source credibility did not modulate pupil size in response to emotional headline contents. Instead, pupil size was smaller for emotional compared to neutral headlines for both trusted and distrusted sources. This pattern of findings suggests that emotional contents yield fluent social judgments that are made with relatively little mental effort-even if based on untrustworthy news. Cognitive resources to evaluate the credibility of news may primarily be allocated when emotional contents providing (false) fluency are not available. This insight into the biases underlying the processing of potential misinformation may be used as a protection against biased opinions and judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1468-1480"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is it better to be happy or right? Examining the relative role of the pragmatic and epistemic imperatives in momentary affective evaluations. 快乐好还是正确好?研究实用主义和认识论在瞬间情感评价中的相对作用。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001349
Inon Raz, Niv Reggev, Michael Gilead

According to research highlighting the importance of predictions, the confirmation of expectations may be a positively-laden experience. A strong test of this principle is the case of the "doomsayer's delight": the possibility that belief confirmation can be rewarding even when negative expectations are realized. In order to investigate this idea, we conducted two high-powered experiments examining people's immediate affective reactions following exposure to expected or unexpected positive and negative stimuli. The results show that people feel significantly worse when their pessimistic expectations are confirmed than when their optimistic expectations are violated. This finding was not moderated by several theoretically relevant individual difference measures or temporal dynamics. Findings from this study contribute to our understanding of the interplay between epistemic and pragmatic motivations in guiding emotional responses. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据强调预测重要性的研究,预期的确认可能是一种积极的体验。末日喜悦 "就是对这一原理的有力验证:即使消极的预期实现了,信念的确认也可能带来回报。为了研究这一观点,我们进行了两项高能实验,考察人们在受到预期或意外的正面和负面刺激后的即时情感反应。结果表明,当人们的悲观预期得到证实时,他们的情绪会明显比乐观预期落空时更糟糕。这一发现并没有受到几种理论上相关的个体差异测量或时间动态的调节。这项研究的结果有助于我们理解认识论动机和实用主义动机在引导情绪反应方面的相互作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is it better to be happy or right? Examining the relative role of the pragmatic and epistemic imperatives in momentary affective evaluations.","authors":"Inon Raz, Niv Reggev, Michael Gilead","doi":"10.1037/emo0001349","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001349","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to research highlighting the importance of predictions, the confirmation of expectations may be a positively-laden experience. A strong test of this principle is the case of the \"doomsayer's delight\": the possibility that belief confirmation can be rewarding even when negative expectations are realized. In order to investigate this idea, we conducted two high-powered experiments examining people's immediate affective reactions following exposure to expected or unexpected positive and negative stimuli. The results show that people feel significantly worse when their pessimistic expectations are confirmed than when their optimistic expectations are violated. This finding was not moderated by several theoretically relevant individual difference measures or temporal dynamics. Findings from this study contribute to our understanding of the interplay between epistemic and pragmatic motivations in guiding emotional responses. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1343-1357"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An experimental paradigm for triggering a depressive syndrome. 引发抑郁综合征的实验范例。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001338
Maxwell Altman, Lily W Martin, Candice Chiu, Stefanie B Northover, Siqi Huang, Sarah Goegan, Marta M Maslej, Steven D Hollon, Benoit H Mulsant, Paul W Andrews

Research investigating whether depression is an adaptation or a disorder has been hindered by the lack of an experimental paradigm that can test causal relationships. Moreover, studies attempting to induce the syndrome often fail to capture the suite of feelings, thoughts, and behaviors that characterize depression. An experimental paradigm for triggering depressive symptoms can improve our etiological understanding of the syndrome. The present study attempts to induce core symptoms of depression, particularly those related to rumination, in a healthy, nonclinical sample through a controlled social experiment. These symptoms are sad or depressed mood, anhedonia, feelings of worthlessness or guilt, and difficulty concentrating. One hundred and thirty-four undergraduate students were randomly assigned to either an exclusion (E) or control (C) group. Participants in the exclusion group were exposed to a modified Cyberball paradigm, designed to make them feel socially excluded, followed by a dual-interference task to assess whether their exclusion interfered with their working memory. Excluded participants: (a) self-reported a significant increase in sadness and decrease in happiness, but not anxiety or calmness; (b) scored significantly higher in four of five variables related to depressive rumination; and (c) performed significantly worse on a dual-interference task, suggesting an impaired ability to concentrate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

由于缺乏能够检验因果关系的实验范式,抑郁症究竟是一种适应还是一种疾病的研究一直受到阻碍。此外,试图诱发抑郁症综合征的研究往往无法捕捉到抑郁症所特有的一系列感受、想法和行为。诱发抑郁症状的实验范式可以提高我们对该综合征的病因学认识。本研究试图通过受控的社会实验,在健康的非临床样本中诱发抑郁症的核心症状,尤其是与反刍有关的症状。这些症状包括悲伤或抑郁情绪、厌世情绪、无价值感或内疚感以及注意力难以集中。134 名本科生被随机分配到排除组(E)或对照组(C)。排挤组的参与者被置于一个改良的网络球范例中,旨在让他们感到自己被社会排挤,然后再接受双重干扰任务,以评估他们的排挤是否会干扰他们的工作记忆。被排斥的参与者:(a) 自我报告的悲伤情绪显著增加,快乐情绪显著减少,但焦虑或平静情绪没有显著增加;(b) 在与抑郁反刍有关的五个变量中,有四个变量的得分显著增加;(c) 在双重干扰任务中的表现显著变差,表明集中注意力的能力受损。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"An experimental paradigm for triggering a depressive syndrome.","authors":"Maxwell Altman, Lily W Martin, Candice Chiu, Stefanie B Northover, Siqi Huang, Sarah Goegan, Marta M Maslej, Steven D Hollon, Benoit H Mulsant, Paul W Andrews","doi":"10.1037/emo0001338","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001338","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research investigating whether depression is an adaptation or a disorder has been hindered by the lack of an experimental paradigm that can test causal relationships. Moreover, studies attempting to induce the syndrome often fail to capture the suite of feelings, thoughts, and behaviors that characterize depression. An experimental paradigm for triggering depressive symptoms can improve our etiological understanding of the syndrome. The present study attempts to induce core symptoms of depression, particularly those related to rumination, in a healthy, nonclinical sample through a controlled social experiment. These symptoms are sad or depressed mood, anhedonia, feelings of worthlessness or guilt, and difficulty concentrating. One hundred and thirty-four undergraduate students were randomly assigned to either an exclusion (E) or control (C) group. Participants in the exclusion group were exposed to a modified Cyberball paradigm, designed to make them feel socially excluded, followed by a dual-interference task to assess whether their exclusion interfered with their working memory. Excluded participants: (a) self-reported a significant increase in sadness and decrease in happiness, but not anxiety or calmness; (b) scored significantly higher in four of five variables related to depressive rumination; and (c) performed significantly worse on a dual-interference task, suggesting an impaired ability to concentrate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1442-1455"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Regulating emotions about secrets. 调节对秘密的情绪。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001357
Valentina Bianchi, Katharine H Greenaway, Michael L Slepian, Elise K Kalokerinos

Secrecy is common and psychologically costly. Research shows that secrets have high emotional stakes, but no research has directly tested how people regulate their emotions about secrets. To fill this gap, we conducted an experimental study (Study 1), then moved to studying secrecy "in the wild" to capture regulatory processes as they unfold in everyday life (Studies 2 and 3). In Study 1 (N = 498), people reported using different strategies to regulate emotions about secrets compared to matched nonsecrets. In two daily diary studies (NStudy 2 = 174, 1,059 surveys; NStudy 3 = 240, 2,764 surveys), participants reported engaging in acceptance, distraction, and expressive suppression most-and social sharing least-to manage emotions about secrets. Moreover, in testing which kinds of secrets required most regulation, Study 3 suggested that significant, negative, controllable, and socially harmful secrets were associated with greater use of rumination, distraction, and suppression; perceived immorality of keeping secrets was associated with greater use of reappraisal; and secret discoverability did not differentially predict regulation strategies. Our findings indicate that when regulating emotions about their secrets, people appear to prioritize their intention to keep secret information hidden, despite potential well-being costs that may come with enacting this intention. Understanding the regulatory processes involved in secrecy is a foundation on which future research can build to identify ways of alleviating the burden of secrecy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

保守秘密很常见,但心理成本也很高。研究表明,秘密具有很高的情感利害关系,但还没有研究直接测试人们如何调节对秘密的情感。为了填补这一空白,我们进行了一项实验研究(研究 1),然后转而研究 "野生 "秘密,以捕捉日常生活中的调节过程(研究 2 和研究 3)。在研究 1(N = 498)中,人们报告说,与匹配的非秘密相比,他们使用了不同的策略来调节有关秘密的情绪。在两项每日日记研究中(研究 2 = 174 人,1,059 份调查;研究 3 = 240 人,2,764 份调查),参与者报告说,他们在管理与秘密有关的情绪时,使用最多的是接受、转移注意力和表达性抑制,使用最少的是社交分享。此外,在测试哪种秘密最需要调节时,研究 3 表明,重要的、消极的、可控的和对社会有害的秘密与更多地使用反刍、转移注意力和压抑有关;认为保守秘密是不道德的与更多地使用重新评价有关;秘密的可发现性对调节策略的预测没有差异。我们的研究结果表明,在调节有关秘密的情绪时,人们似乎会优先考虑隐藏秘密信息的意图,尽管实施这一意图可能会带来潜在的幸福代价。了解保密所涉及的调节过程是未来研究确定减轻保密负担的方法的基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Regulating emotions about secrets.","authors":"Valentina Bianchi, Katharine H Greenaway, Michael L Slepian, Elise K Kalokerinos","doi":"10.1037/emo0001357","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001357","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Secrecy is common and psychologically costly. Research shows that secrets have high emotional stakes, but no research has directly tested how people regulate their emotions about secrets. To fill this gap, we conducted an experimental study (Study 1), then moved to studying secrecy \"in the wild\" to capture regulatory processes as they unfold in everyday life (Studies 2 and 3). In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 498), people reported using different strategies to regulate emotions about secrets compared to matched nonsecrets. In two daily diary studies (<i>N</i><sub>Study 2</sub> = 174, 1,059 surveys; <i>N</i><sub>Study 3</sub> = 240, 2,764 surveys), participants reported engaging in acceptance, distraction, and expressive suppression most-and social sharing least-to manage emotions about secrets. Moreover, in testing which kinds of secrets required most regulation, Study 3 suggested that significant, negative, controllable, and socially harmful secrets were associated with greater use of rumination, distraction, and suppression; perceived immorality of keeping secrets was associated with greater use of reappraisal; and secret discoverability did not differentially predict regulation strategies. Our findings indicate that when regulating emotions about their secrets, people appear to prioritize their intention to keep secret information hidden, despite potential well-being costs that may come with enacting this intention. Understanding the regulatory processes involved in secrecy is a foundation on which future research can build to identify ways of alleviating the burden of secrecy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1386-1402"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relative difficulty of resolving motivational conflicts is affective context-dependent. 解决动机冲突的相对难度取决于情境。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001353
Maya Enisman, Tali Kleiman

According to Lewin's seminal motivational theory, conflicts between undesirable alternatives (avoidance-avoidance conflicts) are more difficult to resolve than conflicts between desirable alternatives (approach-approach conflicts). This difference in the difficulty of resolving approach-approach and avoidance-avoidance conflicts was suggested as a general law for human behavior, and subsequent research provided robust evidence to support it. Here we challenge this assertion. We argue that the difference in conflict resolution difficulty depends on the compatibility between the type of conflict (approach-approach vs. avoidance-avoidance) and the affective context (positive vs. negative) in which the conflict is being resolved. We report five studies. Data were collected from 2019 to 2021. In Studies 1-4, we presented participants with both conflict types, embedded in either a positive or a negative affective context. Across different designs and stimuli, and for both experienced difficulty and decision time, we found that in a positive affective context, avoidance-avoidance conflicts were more difficult to resolve than approach-approach conflicts; however, in a negative affective context, no difference between the conflict types was found. In Study 5, we added a neutral control condition to relate our findings to previous research, which did not manipulate the affective context. Taken together, our findings challenge a seminal motivational theory and show that choosing the lesser of two evils is not always more difficult than choosing the greater of two goods. Instead, the difference in conflict resolution difficulty depends on the affective context in which the choice is being made. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据卢因的开创性动机理论,与理想选择之间的冲突(接近-接近冲突)相比,不理想选择之间的冲突(回避-回避冲突)更难解决。这种 "接近-接近 "冲突和 "回避-回避 "冲突在解决难度上的差异被认为是人类行为的普遍规律,随后的研究也提供了有力的证据来支持这一观点。在此,我们对这一论断提出质疑。我们认为,冲突解决难度的差异取决于冲突类型(接近-接近与回避-回避)和解决冲突时的情感环境(积极与消极)之间的相容性。我们报告了五项研究。数据收集时间为 2019 年至 2021 年。在第 1-4 项研究中,我们向参与者展示了两种冲突类型,并将其嵌入积极或消极的情感背景中。通过不同的设计和刺激,以及经验难度和决策时间,我们发现在积极情绪情境中,回避-回避冲突比接近-接近冲突更难解决;然而,在消极情绪情境中,冲突类型之间没有差异。在研究 5 中,我们增加了一个中性对照条件,以便将我们的研究结果与之前的研究结果联系起来。综上所述,我们的研究结果对一个开创性的动机理论提出了质疑,并表明两害相权取其轻并不总是比两利相权取其重更困难。相反,冲突解决难度的差异取决于做出选择时的情感背景。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The relative difficulty of resolving motivational conflicts is affective context-dependent.","authors":"Maya Enisman, Tali Kleiman","doi":"10.1037/emo0001353","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001353","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to Lewin's seminal motivational theory, conflicts between undesirable alternatives (avoidance-avoidance conflicts) are more difficult to resolve than conflicts between desirable alternatives (approach-approach conflicts). This difference in the difficulty of resolving approach-approach and avoidance-avoidance conflicts was suggested as a general law for human behavior, and subsequent research provided robust evidence to support it. Here we challenge this assertion. We argue that the difference in conflict resolution difficulty depends on the compatibility between the type of conflict (approach-approach vs. avoidance-avoidance) and the affective context (positive vs. negative) in which the conflict is being resolved. We report five studies. Data were collected from 2019 to 2021. In Studies 1-4, we presented participants with both conflict types, embedded in either a positive or a negative affective context. Across different designs and stimuli, and for both experienced difficulty and decision time, we found that in a positive affective context, avoidance-avoidance conflicts were more difficult to resolve than approach-approach conflicts; however, in a negative affective context, no difference between the conflict types was found. In Study 5, we added a neutral control condition to relate our findings to previous research, which did not manipulate the affective context. Taken together, our findings challenge a seminal motivational theory and show that choosing the lesser of two evils is not always more difficult than choosing the greater of two goods. Instead, the difference in conflict resolution difficulty depends on the affective context in which the choice is being made. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1358-1375"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140144347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Daily stress encounters: Positive emotion upregulation and depressive symptoms. 日常压力遭遇:积极情绪上调与抑郁症状。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001362
Desirée Colombo, Rosa María Baños, Lorena Desdentado, Annet Kleiboer, Jean-Baptiste Pavani, Maja Wrzesien, Juana María Bretón López

When it comes to coping with stress, positive emotion upregulation is of utmost importance. Positive emotions have been suggested to be an important resource during stressful times since people try to create and upregulate pleasant emotional states when feeling stressed. Accordingly, individual differences in the ability to generate and savor positive emotional states could also affect one's skills in dealing with stress. In this regard, an important factor might be depression, which is associated with impaired positive emotion regulation. To disentangle the reciprocal influence between perceived stress and positive emotion upregulation, we conducted an Ecological Momentary Assessment study (n = 92) in which we assessed participants' stress levels and use of positive upregulating strategies (attentional deployment, cognitive change, and response modulation) three times a day over 2 weeks. Results from linear mixed-effects models showed that higher levels of perceived stress at one point predicted increased use of positive upregulating strategies from this point to the next which, in turn, resulted in subsequent diminished stress levels. Interaction analyses indicated that participants with higher depressive symptoms implemented upregulating strategies to a lower extent when experiencing intense stress. Furthermore, attentional deployment was less effective in decreasing stress in individuals higher in depression, whereas the other strategies showed comparable or even higher efficacy. Overall, positive emotion upregulation might be regarded as an adaptive tool that helps cope with stress. This mechanism might be altered in people higher in depression, who specifically struggle to implement positive upregulating strategies during times of stress. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

说到应对压力,积极情绪的上调至关重要。积极情绪被认为是压力时期的一种重要资源,因为人们在感到压力时会努力创造和上调愉快的情绪状态。因此,产生和品味积极情绪状态能力的个体差异也会影响一个人应对压力的技能。在这方面,抑郁症可能是一个重要因素,因为抑郁症与积极情绪调节能力受损有关。为了厘清感知压力和积极情绪上调之间的相互影响,我们进行了一项生态瞬间评估研究(n = 92),在这项研究中,我们对参与者的压力水平和积极上调策略(注意力调配、认知改变和反应调节)的使用情况进行了评估,每天三次,为期两周。线性混合效应模型的结果表明,在某一点上感知到的压力水平越高,预示着从这一点到下一点使用积极上调策略的次数越多,反过来,这又会导致随后压力水平的降低。交互分析表明,抑郁症状较重的参与者在经历巨大压力时,采用上调策略的程度较低。此外,在抑郁程度较高的人中,注意力调配对减轻压力的效果较差,而其他策略的效果则相当甚至更高。总的来说,积极情绪上调可被视为一种有助于应对压力的适应工具。这种机制可能会在抑郁程度较高的人群中发生改变,因为他们在压力时期特别难以实施积极的上调策略。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Daily stress encounters: Positive emotion upregulation and depressive symptoms.","authors":"Desirée Colombo, Rosa María Baños, Lorena Desdentado, Annet Kleiboer, Jean-Baptiste Pavani, Maja Wrzesien, Juana María Bretón López","doi":"10.1037/emo0001362","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When it comes to coping with stress, positive emotion upregulation is of utmost importance. Positive emotions have been suggested to be an important resource during stressful times since people try to create and upregulate pleasant emotional states when feeling stressed. Accordingly, individual differences in the ability to generate and savor positive emotional states could also affect one's skills in dealing with stress. In this regard, an important factor might be depression, which is associated with impaired positive emotion regulation. To disentangle the reciprocal influence between perceived stress and positive emotion upregulation, we conducted an Ecological Momentary Assessment study (<i>n</i> = 92) in which we assessed participants' stress levels and use of positive upregulating strategies (attentional deployment, cognitive change, and response modulation) three times a day over 2 weeks. Results from linear mixed-effects models showed that higher levels of perceived stress at one point predicted increased use of positive upregulating strategies from this point to the next which, in turn, resulted in subsequent diminished stress levels. Interaction analyses indicated that participants with higher depressive symptoms implemented upregulating strategies to a lower extent when experiencing intense stress. Furthermore, attentional deployment was less effective in decreasing stress in individuals higher in depression, whereas the other strategies showed comparable or even higher efficacy. Overall, positive emotion upregulation might be regarded as an adaptive tool that helps cope with stress. This mechanism might be altered in people higher in depression, who specifically struggle to implement positive upregulating strategies during times of stress. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1403-1416"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1