首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
The influence of pre- and intratask emotional experiences on affective working memory maintenance. 任务前和任务内情绪体验对情感性工作记忆维持的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001464
Nour Kardosh, Christian E Waugh, Joseph A Mikels, Nilly Mor

In two studies conducted in 2022, we examined the effect of images that elicit incidental emotions and the timing of exposure to these images, on the maintenance of positive and negative emotions in affective working memory (AWM). In Study 1, participants viewed a negative, positive, or neutral image while maintaining the emotional intensity of positive or negative emotions in AWM (intratrial). The results showed that experiencing a negative or positive incidental emotion (but not neutral states) improved the maintenance of negative (but not positive) emotions induced by another stimulus. In Study 2, participants were randomly assigned to experimental conditions. In the first condition, they viewed an emotion-eliciting image while maintaining an emotion elicited by a different image (replicating Study 1). In the second condition, they viewed the emotion-eliciting image before maintaining an emotion elicited by a different image. The results replicated those of Study 1 and showed that the timing of experiencing the incidental emotion (before or during the task) did not affect AWM. They also suggest that maintenance of negative emotions increases irrespective of the emotional context surrounding them. These findings offer valuable theoretical insights into the role of emotional contexts in intensifying negative emotions, potentially guiding future research on interventions designed to modulate negative emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

在2022年进行的两项研究中,我们考察了引发附带情绪的图像和接触这些图像的时间对情感工作记忆(AWM)中积极和消极情绪维持的影响。在研究1中,参与者在AWM(心房)中观看消极、积极或中性的图像,同时保持积极或消极情绪的情绪强度。结果表明,经历消极或积极的附带情绪(而不是中性状态)改善了由另一刺激引起的消极(而不是积极)情绪的维持。在研究2中,参与者被随机分配到实验条件中。在第一种情况下,他们观看了一张引发情绪的图片,同时保持了由另一张图片引起的情绪(重复研究1)。在第二种情况下,他们在观看了引发情绪的图片后,又保持了由另一张图片引起的情绪。结果重复了研究1的结果,并表明经历附带情绪的时间(任务前或任务中)不影响AWM。他们还表明,无论周围的情绪环境如何,消极情绪的维持都会增加。这些发现为情绪环境在强化负面情绪中的作用提供了有价值的理论见解,可能指导未来研究旨在调节负面情绪的干预措施。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The influence of pre- and intratask emotional experiences on affective working memory maintenance.","authors":"Nour Kardosh, Christian E Waugh, Joseph A Mikels, Nilly Mor","doi":"10.1037/emo0001464","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001464","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In two studies conducted in 2022, we examined the effect of images that elicit incidental emotions and the timing of exposure to these images, on the maintenance of positive and negative emotions in affective working memory (AWM). In Study 1, participants viewed a negative, positive, or neutral image while maintaining the emotional intensity of positive or negative emotions in AWM (intratrial). The results showed that experiencing a negative or positive incidental emotion (but not neutral states) improved the maintenance of negative (but not positive) emotions induced by another stimulus. In Study 2, participants were randomly assigned to experimental conditions. In the first condition, they viewed an emotion-eliciting image while maintaining an emotion elicited by a different image (replicating Study 1). In the second condition, they viewed the emotion-eliciting image before maintaining an emotion elicited by a different image. The results replicated those of Study 1 and showed that the timing of experiencing the incidental emotion (before or during the task) did not affect AWM. They also suggest that maintenance of negative emotions increases irrespective of the emotional context surrounding them. These findings offer valuable theoretical insights into the role of emotional contexts in intensifying negative emotions, potentially guiding future research on interventions designed to modulate negative emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Men and women's waking patterns to infant crying: Preparenthood differences are insufficient to explain uneven sharing of nighttime care. 男性和女性对婴儿啼哭的清醒模式:为人父母的差异不足以解释夜间照顾的不均匀分担。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001478
Arnault-Quentin Vermillet, Joshua Charles Skewes, Christine E Parsons

Crying in infancy is an important emotional signal that elicits care from adults, and women are often assumed to be more sensitive and reactive to infant crying than men. In a series of studies, we tested whether preparenthood gender differences in sensitivity to infant cries are a potential driver of the unequal share of early parenting. In Study 1, we tested for differences in men and women's awakening to infant crying and alarms among nonparents in an overnight experiment (N = 142). We found that at the lowest sound volumes only, estimated at a sound pressure level of between 33 and 44 decibels, women were 14% more likely to wake than men to both infant crying and alarm sounds. There were no differences between women and men at louder sound volumes. In Study 2, we examined the nighttime caregiving patterns of first-time parents over a week using experience sampling to obtain reports from both fathers and mothers (N = 117). We found that mothers were, on average, three times more likely to check on or respond to their infants at night than fathers. In 23% of couples, there was some evidence for equal sharing. Finally, in a simulation study, we reconstructed the distribution of care that could emerge from the awakening differences observed in Study 1. We then compared these simulations to the empirical nighttime caregiving patterns reported by first-time parents in Study 2. Our simulation showed that the large difference between parents' nighttime caregiving was unlikely to emerge from the small preparenthood differences in awakening likelihood. We conclude that the greater maternal share of nighttime caregiving cannot plausibly be explained by inherent preparenthood differences in auditory reactivity or nocturnal waking behavior in men or women. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

婴儿的哭泣是一种重要的情感信号,它会引起成年人的照顾,而女性通常被认为对婴儿的哭泣比男性更敏感,反应更强烈。在一系列的研究中,我们测试了产前性别对婴儿哭声敏感性的差异是否是早期育儿份额不平等的潜在驱动因素。在研究1中,我们在非父母的夜间实验中测试了男性和女性对婴儿哭声和警报的觉醒差异(N = 142)。我们发现,仅在最低音量下,估计在33到44分贝的声压水平下,女性被婴儿哭声和警报声吵醒的可能性比男性高14%。在更大的音量下,女性和男性之间没有差异。在研究2中,我们采用经验抽样的方法,从父亲和母亲那里获得了超过一周的夜间照顾模式(N = 117)。我们发现,平均而言,母亲在夜间查看或回应婴儿的可能性是父亲的三倍。在23%的夫妇中,有一些证据表明他们是平等分享的。最后,在一项模拟研究中,我们重建了从研究1中观察到的觉醒差异中可能出现的关怀分布。然后,我们将这些模拟与研究2中首次父母报告的经验夜间照顾模式进行了比较。我们的模拟表明,父母夜间照顾的巨大差异不太可能来自于父母在觉醒可能性方面的微小差异。我们的结论是,更大的母亲夜间照顾的份额不能合理地解释为先天先天的听觉反应性差异或夜间清醒行为的男性或女性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Men and women's waking patterns to infant crying: Preparenthood differences are insufficient to explain uneven sharing of nighttime care.","authors":"Arnault-Quentin Vermillet, Joshua Charles Skewes, Christine E Parsons","doi":"10.1037/emo0001478","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001478","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Crying in infancy is an important emotional signal that elicits care from adults, and women are often assumed to be more sensitive and reactive to infant crying than men. In a series of studies, we tested whether preparenthood gender differences in sensitivity to infant cries are a potential driver of the unequal share of early parenting. In Study 1, we tested for differences in men and women's awakening to infant crying and alarms among nonparents in an overnight experiment (<i>N</i> = 142). We found that at the lowest sound volumes only, estimated at a sound pressure level of between 33 and 44 decibels, women were 14% more likely to wake than men to both infant crying and alarm sounds. There were no differences between women and men at louder sound volumes. In Study 2, we examined the nighttime caregiving patterns of first-time parents over a week using experience sampling to obtain reports from both fathers and mothers (<i>N</i> = 117). We found that mothers were, on average, three times more likely to check on or respond to their infants at night than fathers. In 23% of couples, there was some evidence for equal sharing. Finally, in a simulation study, we reconstructed the distribution of care that could emerge from the awakening differences observed in Study 1. We then compared these simulations to the empirical nighttime caregiving patterns reported by first-time parents in Study 2. Our simulation showed that the large difference between parents' nighttime caregiving was unlikely to emerge from the small preparenthood differences in awakening likelihood. We conclude that the greater maternal share of nighttime caregiving cannot plausibly be explained by inherent preparenthood differences in auditory reactivity or nocturnal waking behavior in men or women. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143013634","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion word production tasks grant insight into the development of emotion word organization and accessibility. 情绪词生成任务有助于深入了解情绪词组织和可及性的发展。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001491
Chantal A Valdivia-Moreno, Stephanie F Sasse, Hilary K Lambert, Katie A McLaughlin, Leah H Somerville, Erik C Nook

Children are often instructed to "use their words" to communicate their emotions, which requires them to quickly access words that best describe their feelings. Adults vary in their ability to bring both nonemotion and emotion words to mind (two capacities called verbal fluency and emotion fluency). However, no studies have examined how emotion fluency emerges across development, despite the fact that mastering emotion language is an important developmental task. A cross-sectional sample of participants aged 4-25 years (N = 194) generated as many fruit words as possible in 60 s (to measure verbal fluency) and as many emotion words as possible in 60 s (to measure emotion fluency). Emotion fluency was highly correlated with verbal fluency, and both showed similar increases across age, plateauing in late adolescence. Participants produced more negative emotion words than positive or neutral words, and these proportions were invariant across age. Network analyses shed light on the emergence of semantic networks underlying emotion organization across age. Finally, age of acquisition, valence, dominance, concreteness, and word length were significantly associated with the order in which emotion words came to participants' minds, suggesting that these dimensions are associated with the accessibility of emotion concepts. Interestingly, the influence of these dimensions on the order of emotion word production was invariant across age. Results from this study illustrate the developmental emergence of emotion fluency and provide new insight into the key dimensions that are associated with which emotion words rapidly come to mind. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

孩子们经常被教导“用他们的语言”来表达他们的情绪,这需要他们迅速找到最能描述他们感受的词语。成年人记住非情感词汇和情感词汇的能力各不相同(这两种能力被称为语言流畅性和情感流畅性)。然而,尽管掌握情绪语言是一项重要的发展任务,但还没有研究调查情绪流畅性是如何在发展过程中出现的。一个4-25岁参与者的横断面样本(N = 194)在60岁时尽可能多地生成水果词(以测量语言流畅性),在60岁时尽可能多地生成情感词(以测量情感流畅性)。情感流畅度与语言流畅度高度相关,两者在不同年龄阶段都表现出相似的增长,在青春期后期趋于稳定。参与者说的消极情绪词汇多于积极或中性词汇,而且这些比例在各个年龄段都是不变的。网络分析揭示了跨年龄情感组织背后的语义网络的出现。最后,习得年龄、效价、优势度、具体性和单词长度与情绪词汇出现的顺序显著相关,表明这些维度与情绪概念的可及性有关。有趣的是,这些维度对情绪词产生顺序的影响在不同年龄是不变的。本研究的结果说明了情绪流畅性的发展过程,并为与情绪词汇快速出现相关的关键维度提供了新的见解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Emotion word production tasks grant insight into the development of emotion word organization and accessibility.","authors":"Chantal A Valdivia-Moreno, Stephanie F Sasse, Hilary K Lambert, Katie A McLaughlin, Leah H Somerville, Erik C Nook","doi":"10.1037/emo0001491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001491","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children are often instructed to \"use their words\" to communicate their emotions, which requires them to quickly access words that best describe their feelings. Adults vary in their ability to bring both nonemotion and emotion words to mind (two capacities called <i>verbal fluency</i> and <i>emotion fluency</i>). However, no studies have examined how emotion fluency emerges across development, despite the fact that mastering emotion language is an important developmental task. A cross-sectional sample of participants aged 4-25 years (N = 194) generated as many fruit words as possible in 60 s (to measure verbal fluency) and as many emotion words as possible in 60 s (to measure emotion fluency). Emotion fluency was highly correlated with verbal fluency, and both showed similar increases across age, plateauing in late adolescence. Participants produced more negative emotion words than positive or neutral words, and these proportions were invariant across age. Network analyses shed light on the emergence of semantic networks underlying emotion organization across age. Finally, age of acquisition, valence, dominance, concreteness, and word length were significantly associated with the order in which emotion words came to participants' minds, suggesting that these dimensions are associated with the accessibility of emotion concepts. Interestingly, the influence of these dimensions on the order of emotion word production was invariant across age. Results from this study illustrate the developmental emergence of emotion fluency and provide new insight into the key dimensions that are associated with which emotion words rapidly come to mind. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014373","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Managing emotions in everyday life: Why a toolbox of strategies matters. 在日常生活中管理情绪:为什么策略工具箱很重要。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001492
Chayce R Baldwin, Kathryn E Schertz, Ariana Orvell, Cory Costello, Sakura Takahashi, Jason S Moser, Ozlem Ayduk, Ethan Kross

Emerging research indicates that people use multiple strategies to manage their emotions in everyday life. Yet, we know little about what these strategy combinations look like, how they function, or how individual differences influence these phenomena. We addressed these issues in two, 2-week daily diary studies performed during the height of the COVID-19 pandemic (N = 422; data collected April and September 2020). Each evening, participants rated their level of COVID-19 anxiety and indicated which of 18 emotion regulation strategies they used to manage it. There was tremendous diversity in the strategy combinations people used: 74% of the combinations were unique across participants and included strategies seldom studied together (e.g., exercise, journaling, social interaction, and cognitive reframing). On average, using a given strategy predicted same-day use of another strategy with only 1% accuracy. Despite this variability, a set of features consistently predicted effective regulation: Using large and healthy repertoires of strategies in diverse ways predicted reductions in anxiety over time. Psychologically distressed individuals experienced more daily anxiety and drew on a wider but more unhealthy set of strategies. However, when they used adaptive strategy combinations, they benefited just as much as less distressed individuals. These results illuminate the anatomy of people's emotion regulatory lives, underscoring the need to develop frameworks that capture the diverse ways people manage their emotions. They also identify specific mechanisms that interventions can target to improve how people manage their emotions under ecologically valid conditions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

最新研究表明,人们在日常生活中使用多种策略来管理自己的情绪。然而,我们对这些策略组合是什么样子的,它们是如何起作用的,或者个体差异是如何影响这些现象的,知之甚少。我们在COVID-19大流行高峰期进行的两项为期两周的每日日记研究中解决了这些问题(N = 422;数据收集于2020年4月和9月)。每天晚上,参与者对他们的COVID-19焦虑程度进行评分,并指出他们使用了18种情绪调节策略中的哪一种来管理这种焦虑。人们使用的策略组合存在巨大的多样性:74%的组合在参与者中是独特的,包括很少一起研究的策略(例如,锻炼,日记,社会互动和认知重构)。平均而言,使用给定策略预测当天使用另一策略的准确率只有1%。尽管存在这种可变性,但一系列特征一致地预测了有效的调节:以不同的方式使用大量健康的策略可以预测随着时间的推移焦虑会减少。心理痛苦的人会经历更多的日常焦虑,并采取更广泛但更不健康的策略。然而,当他们使用适应性策略组合时,他们和不那么痛苦的人一样受益。这些结果阐明了人们情绪调节生活的解剖结构,强调了开发捕捉人们管理情绪的不同方式的框架的必要性。他们还确定了干预措施可以针对的具体机制,以改善人们在生态有效条件下管理情绪的方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Managing emotions in everyday life: Why a toolbox of strategies matters.","authors":"Chayce R Baldwin, Kathryn E Schertz, Ariana Orvell, Cory Costello, Sakura Takahashi, Jason S Moser, Ozlem Ayduk, Ethan Kross","doi":"10.1037/emo0001492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001492","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emerging research indicates that people use multiple strategies to manage their emotions in everyday life. Yet, we know little about what these strategy combinations look like, how they function, or how individual differences influence these phenomena. We addressed these issues in two, 2-week daily diary studies performed during the height of the COVID-19 pandemic (<i>N</i> = 422; data collected April and September 2020). Each evening, participants rated their level of COVID-19 anxiety and indicated which of 18 emotion regulation strategies they used to manage it. There was tremendous diversity in the strategy combinations people used: 74% of the combinations were unique across participants and included strategies seldom studied together (e.g., exercise, journaling, social interaction, and cognitive reframing). On average, using a given strategy predicted same-day use of another strategy with only 1% accuracy. Despite this variability, a set of features consistently predicted effective regulation: Using large and healthy repertoires of strategies in diverse ways predicted reductions in anxiety over time. Psychologically distressed individuals experienced more daily anxiety and drew on a wider but more unhealthy set of strategies. However, when they used adaptive strategy combinations, they benefited just as much as less distressed individuals. These results illuminate the anatomy of people's emotion regulatory lives, underscoring the need to develop frameworks that capture the diverse ways people manage their emotions. They also identify specific mechanisms that interventions can target to improve how people manage their emotions under ecologically valid conditions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evolutionary continuities and discontinuities in affective voice signaling. 情感语音信号的进化连续性与非连续性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001484
Sascha Frühholz, Joris Dietziker, Matthias Staib, Marine Bobin, Florence Steiner

Affective voice signaling has significant biological and social relevance across various species, and different affective signaling types have emerged through the evolution of voice communication. These types range from basic affective voice bursts and nonverbal affective up to affective intonations superimposed on speech utterances in humans in the form of paraverbal prosodic patterns. These different types of affective signaling should have evolved to be acoustically and perceptually distinctive, allowing accurate and nuanced affective communication. It might be assumed that affect signaling is most effective and distinctive in affective prosody as the presumably most recently evolved form of acoustic voice signaling. We investigated and compared two signaling types in human voice communication with different evolutionary backgrounds, referred to as nonverbal affect signals (shared across many species) and affective prosody (being exclusive in humans). We found, first, that various basic affect categories seem to be distinctively encoded in both signal types, but there seems minimal continuity in the acoustic code from nonverbal affect signals to affective prosody and vice versa. Second, we found that decoding affective meaning seems considerably impaired from affective prosody. Many positive affect signals and especially vocal disgust showed extreme decoding impairments from affective prosody, with speech acoustics probably constraining affect encoding in prosody to a considerable degree. Only the recognizability of voice signals of threat seems to be largely preserved in affective prosody. In conclusion, it points to considerable discontinuities between nonverbal and paraverbal affect signals, which questions the evolutionary precursors of human affect signaling in voice communication. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

情感语音信号在不同物种之间具有重要的生物学和社会相关性,并且在语音通信的进化过程中出现了不同的情感信号类型。这些类型包括从基本的情感声音爆发和非语言情感到以语旁韵律模式的形式叠加在人类话语上的情感语调。这些不同类型的情感信号应该已经进化到在声学和感知上不同,允许准确和微妙的情感交流。可以认为,情感信号在情感韵律中是最有效和独特的,可能是最近进化的声音信号形式。我们研究并比较了不同进化背景下人类语音交流中的两种信号类型,即非语言情感信号(在许多物种中共享)和情感韵律(仅在人类中存在)。我们发现,首先,各种基本的情感类别似乎在两种信号类型中都有明显的编码,但从非语言情感信号到情感韵律的声学编码似乎没有什么连续性,反之亦然。其次,我们发现情感韵律对情感意义的解码似乎严重受损。许多积极的情感信号,尤其是声音厌恶,在情感韵律中表现出极端的解码障碍,言语声学可能在相当程度上限制了情感在韵律中的编码。只有对威胁的声音信号的可识别性似乎在很大程度上保留在情感韵律中。总之,它指出了非语言和准语言情感信号之间相当大的不连续性,这对人类语音交流中情感信号的进化前兆提出了质疑。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Evolutionary continuities and discontinuities in affective voice signaling.","authors":"Sascha Frühholz, Joris Dietziker, Matthias Staib, Marine Bobin, Florence Steiner","doi":"10.1037/emo0001484","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001484","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affective voice signaling has significant biological and social relevance across various species, and different affective signaling types have emerged through the evolution of voice communication. These types range from basic affective voice bursts and nonverbal affective up to affective intonations superimposed on speech utterances in humans in the form of paraverbal prosodic patterns. These different types of affective signaling should have evolved to be acoustically and perceptually distinctive, allowing accurate and nuanced affective communication. It might be assumed that affect signaling is most effective and distinctive in affective prosody as the presumably most recently evolved form of acoustic voice signaling. We investigated and compared two signaling types in human voice communication with different evolutionary backgrounds, referred to as nonverbal affect signals (shared across many species) and affective prosody (being exclusive in humans). We found, first, that various basic affect categories seem to be distinctively encoded in both signal types, but there seems minimal continuity in the acoustic code from nonverbal affect signals to affective prosody and vice versa. Second, we found that decoding affective meaning seems considerably impaired from affective prosody. Many positive affect signals and especially vocal disgust showed extreme decoding impairments from affective prosody, with speech acoustics probably constraining affect encoding in prosody to a considerable degree. Only the recognizability of voice signals of threat seems to be largely preserved in affective prosody. In conclusion, it points to considerable discontinuities between nonverbal and paraverbal affect signals, which questions the evolutionary precursors of human affect signaling in voice communication. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972780","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social rejection amplifies the value of choice. 社会排斥放大了选择的价值。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001456
Jordan M Dejoie, Melanie Ruiz, Emily G Brudner, Dominic S Fareri

Social rejection has been routinely associated with negative physical and mental health outcomes. However, less is known about how social rejection impacts cognitive processes, including our decision-making abilities. This is critical to understand, given how ubiquitous experiences of rejection have become in the current era of social media. In this preregistered study, we hypothesized that social rejection would amplify the value of choice. Participants made choices about whether to participate in a lottery themselves or defer the choice to a computer across a series of interactions with purported anonymous peers who provided varying degrees of positive (e.g., likes) and negative (e.g., dislikes) feedback to simulate experiences of rejection and acceptance. Subjective experiences of affect and the likelihood of future social engagement with peers were measured. Following experiences of rejection, results revealed that participants were more likely to want to choose for themselves rather than defer the choice to the computer. However, negative affect modulated this pattern, such that when participants reported feeling worse during the task after rejection, they were more likely to defer choice to the computer. Further, negative affect significantly predicted participant's willingness to engage in future social behavior with their partners and individual differences in social symptoms (e.g., social anxiety and the need to belong) were significantly related to choice behavior. Taken together, our findings suggest that experience of social rejection can negatively impact our affective states, perceptions of others, and the degree to which we value choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会排斥通常与负面的身心健康结果有关。然而,对于社会排斥如何影响认知过程,包括我们的决策能力,我们知之甚少。考虑到在当前的社交媒体时代,被拒绝的经历无处不在,理解这一点至关重要。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们假设社会排斥会放大选择的价值。参与者在与匿名同伴的一系列互动中选择是自己参加彩票还是将选择推迟给计算机,这些匿名同伴提供不同程度的积极(例如,喜欢)和消极(例如,不喜欢)反馈,以模拟被拒绝和接受的经历。主观情感体验和未来与同伴交往的可能性被测量。在经历了拒绝之后,结果显示,参与者更愿意自己做出选择,而不是把选择交给电脑。然而,负面影响调节了这种模式,当参与者在被拒绝后报告在任务中感觉更糟时,他们更有可能把选择推迟给电脑。此外,负面情绪显著预测了参与者未来与伴侣进行社交行为的意愿,社交症状(如社交焦虑和归属需要)的个体差异与选择行为显著相关。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,被社会排斥的经历会对我们的情感状态、对他人的看法以及我们重视选择的程度产生负面影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Social rejection amplifies the value of choice.","authors":"Jordan M Dejoie, Melanie Ruiz, Emily G Brudner, Dominic S Fareri","doi":"10.1037/emo0001456","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001456","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social rejection has been routinely associated with negative physical and mental health outcomes. However, less is known about how social rejection impacts cognitive processes, including our decision-making abilities. This is critical to understand, given how ubiquitous experiences of rejection have become in the current era of social media. In this preregistered study, we hypothesized that social rejection would amplify the value of choice. Participants made choices about whether to participate in a lottery themselves or defer the choice to a computer across a series of interactions with purported anonymous peers who provided varying degrees of positive (e.g., likes) and negative (e.g., dislikes) feedback to simulate experiences of rejection and acceptance. Subjective experiences of affect and the likelihood of future social engagement with peers were measured. Following experiences of rejection, results revealed that participants were more likely to want to choose for themselves rather than defer the choice to the computer. However, negative affect modulated this pattern, such that when participants reported feeling worse during the task after rejection, they were more likely to defer choice to the computer. Further, negative affect significantly predicted participant's willingness to engage in future social behavior with their partners and individual differences in social symptoms (e.g., social anxiety and the need to belong) were significantly related to choice behavior. Taken together, our findings suggest that experience of social rejection can negatively impact our affective states, perceptions of others, and the degree to which we value choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142856006","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Angry or neutral, it does not matter to me: Implicit processing of facial emotions is not related to peer victimization experiences. 愤怒还是中立,对我来说都无所谓:面部情绪的内隐加工与同伴受害经历无关。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001405
Sanne Kellij, Gerine M A Lodder, René Veenstra, Berna Güroğlu

The aim of this study was to examine whether prolonged victimization relates to differential processing of emotions. Based on the social information processing theory, it was hypothesized that prolonged victimization would modulate emotion processing, such that victimization relates to a heightened attentional focus toward negative facial expressions and increased amygdala activation in response to negative facial expressions. We targeted a unique sample of 83 children (Mage = 10.6, 49.4% girls) whose victimization history in the past 2 years was available. An Emotional Dot-Probe Task and an Emotion Processing fMRI Task were administered to the participants. Findings included that victimization did not relate significantly to a heightened attentional focus on happy, angry, or fearful expressions. Viewing facial expressions resulted in the activation of the posterior medial frontal cortex, bilateral insula, bilateral fusiform face area, and the right amygdala and hippocampus, which was not related to victimization, nor was victimization related to activation in the amygdala or the social brain regions (medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, precuneus, posterior superior temporal sulcus) when viewing specific emotional (happy, angry, afraid, sad) expressions. Together, these results do not provide evidence that implicit emotion processing without social context relates to victimization. Future research should replicate these results and further examine emotion processing in relation to severe victimization experiences and support systems, such as friendships or parenting, on emotion processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究的目的是检验长期受害是否与情绪的差异处理有关。基于社会信息加工理论,我们假设长时间的受害行为会调节情绪加工,例如受害行为与对消极面部表情的高度注意集中和对消极面部表情的杏仁核激活增加有关。我们的目标是一个独特的样本,83名儿童(法师= 10.6,49.4%的女孩),他们在过去2年的受害历史是可用的。对参与者进行了情绪点探测任务和情绪处理功能磁共振成像任务。研究结果包括,受害与对快乐、愤怒或恐惧表情的高度关注没有显著关系。观察面部表情导致后内侧额叶皮层、双侧脑岛、双侧梭状回面部区、右侧杏仁核和海马的激活,与受害无关,也与特定情绪表情(快乐、愤怒、恐惧、悲伤)时杏仁核或大脑社交区(内侧前额叶皮层、颞顶连接、楔前叶、颞后上沟)的激活无关。总之,这些结果并没有提供证据表明,没有社会背景的内隐情绪加工与受害有关。未来的研究应该重复这些结果,并进一步研究与严重受害经历和支持系统(如友谊或父母)有关的情绪处理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Angry or neutral, it does not matter to me: Implicit processing of facial emotions is not related to peer victimization experiences.","authors":"Sanne Kellij, Gerine M A Lodder, René Veenstra, Berna Güroğlu","doi":"10.1037/emo0001405","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001405","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim of this study was to examine whether prolonged victimization relates to differential processing of emotions. Based on the social information processing theory, it was hypothesized that prolonged victimization would modulate emotion processing, such that victimization relates to a heightened attentional focus toward negative facial expressions and increased amygdala activation in response to negative facial expressions. We targeted a unique sample of 83 children (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 10.6, 49.4% girls) whose victimization history in the past 2 years was available. An Emotional Dot-Probe Task and an Emotion Processing fMRI Task were administered to the participants. Findings included that victimization did not relate significantly to a heightened attentional focus on happy, angry, or fearful expressions. Viewing facial expressions resulted in the activation of the posterior medial frontal cortex, bilateral insula, bilateral fusiform face area, and the right amygdala and hippocampus, which was not related to victimization, nor was victimization related to activation in the amygdala or the social brain regions (medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, precuneus, posterior superior temporal sulcus) when viewing specific emotional (happy, angry, afraid, sad) expressions. Together, these results do not provide evidence that implicit emotion processing without social context relates to victimization. Future research should replicate these results and further examine emotion processing in relation to severe victimization experiences and support systems, such as friendships or parenting, on emotion processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142855848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The need for a unified language framework in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation research. 外在人际情绪调节研究需要统一的语言框架。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001463
Sarah A Walker, Belén López-Pérez, Jens F Beckmann, Hannah Kunst, Shayne Polias

With increasing research interest in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, this article aims to address the critical need for a unified language framework to strengthen and support these research efforts. Despite increasing interest and research in this area, the lack of consistent terminology poses significant challenges to conceptual clarity and scientific progress. By examining the current landscape, the authors identify the proliferation of varied terms across disciplines, which threatens to hamper effective communication and collaboration and, thus, progress. This article first argues for the necessity of a unified terminology and then proposes a possible methodological approach to achieve this. A Delphi study that provides a frame for the collaborative effort of subject matter experts is outlined. Establishing such unified language framework is expected to enhance research quality, foster innovation, and facilitate knowledge accumulation in the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

随着对外在人际情绪调节的研究兴趣的增加,本文旨在解决对统一语言框架的迫切需求,以加强和支持这些研究工作。尽管对这一领域的兴趣和研究日益增加,但缺乏一致的术语对概念清晰度和科学进步构成了重大挑战。通过对当前形势的考察,作者确定了不同学科之间术语的激增,这可能会阻碍有效的沟通和合作,从而阻碍进步。本文首先论证了统一术语的必要性,然后提出了实现这一目标的可能的方法方法。一个德尔菲研究,提供了一个框架的合作努力的主题问题专家概述。建立这种统一的语言框架有望提高研究质量,促进创新,并促进该领域的知识积累。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The need for a unified language framework in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation research.","authors":"Sarah A Walker, Belén López-Pérez, Jens F Beckmann, Hannah Kunst, Shayne Polias","doi":"10.1037/emo0001463","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001463","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>With increasing research interest in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, this article aims to address the critical need for a unified language framework to strengthen and support these research efforts. Despite increasing interest and research in this area, the lack of consistent terminology poses significant challenges to conceptual clarity and scientific progress. By examining the current landscape, the authors identify the proliferation of varied terms across disciplines, which threatens to hamper effective communication and collaboration and, thus, progress. This article first argues for the necessity of a unified terminology and then proposes a possible methodological approach to achieve this. A Delphi study that provides a frame for the collaborative effort of subject matter experts is outlined. Establishing such unified language framework is expected to enhance research quality, foster innovation, and facilitate knowledge accumulation in the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142856013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impact of parity and salivary hormonal levels on motivation toward infant emotions. 胎次和唾液激素水平对婴儿情绪动机的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001467
Hanneli Sinisalo, Amanda C Hahn, Benedict C Jones, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Mikko J Peltola

Infant faces have been shown to be particularly motivating stimuli for women. No studies, however, have compared mothers and nonmothers in whether parity modulates approach motivation toward emotional infant faces. We studied 54 Finnish first-time mothers and 42 nonmothers in a pay-per-view key-press task where the participants were shown 20 infant faces with smiling and crying expressions. Participants were able to adjust the time each face was visible. In addition, salivary testosterone, estradiol, and cortisol levels were measured and their impact on motivation toward infants analyzed. When controlling for the hormonal levels, happy infant faces were viewed longer than crying faces and there was no difference in mean viewing times between mothers and nonmothers. An interaction between parity and emotion emerged: Mothers were more motivated to view happy faces and less motivated to view crying infant faces than nonmothers. Testosterone had a significant effect on viewing times: The higher the testosterone levels were, the shorter amount of time infant faces were viewed. This indicates that testosterone is inversely associated with approach motivation to emotional infant stimuli. This study is the first to compare mothers and nonmothers in a task measuring motivational responses to infant stimuli and indicates that the difference between the approach motivation caused by happy and distressed infant emotions might be more heightened in new mothers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

婴儿的脸对女性来说是特别有激励作用的刺激物。然而,还没有研究比较过母亲和非母亲在胎次是否会调节婴儿情绪化面孔的接近动机。我们研究了54名芬兰初为人母的母亲和42名未为人母的母亲,在按次付费的按键任务中,参与者看到了20张婴儿的脸,其中有微笑和哭泣的表情。参与者能够调整每张脸的可见时间。此外,还测量了唾液睾酮、雌二醇和皮质醇水平,并分析了它们对婴儿动机的影响。在控制激素水平的情况下,婴儿快乐的脸比哭泣的脸被观看的时间更长,母亲和非母亲的平均观看时间没有差异。平等和情感之间的相互作用出现了:与非母亲相比,母亲更有动力去看快乐的脸,而较少动力去看哭泣的婴儿脸。睾丸激素对观看时间有显著影响:睾丸激素水平越高,观看婴儿脸的时间越短。这表明睾酮与婴儿情绪刺激的接近动机呈负相关。这项研究首次比较了母亲和非母亲对婴儿刺激的动机反应,并表明由婴儿的快乐和痛苦情绪引起的接近动机之间的差异在新妈妈身上可能会更加明显。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Impact of parity and salivary hormonal levels on motivation toward infant emotions.","authors":"Hanneli Sinisalo, Amanda C Hahn, Benedict C Jones, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Mikko J Peltola","doi":"10.1037/emo0001467","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001467","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Infant faces have been shown to be particularly motivating stimuli for women. No studies, however, have compared mothers and nonmothers in whether parity modulates approach motivation toward emotional infant faces. We studied 54 Finnish first-time mothers and 42 nonmothers in a pay-per-view key-press task where the participants were shown 20 infant faces with smiling and crying expressions. Participants were able to adjust the time each face was visible. In addition, salivary testosterone, estradiol, and cortisol levels were measured and their impact on motivation toward infants analyzed. When controlling for the hormonal levels, happy infant faces were viewed longer than crying faces and there was no difference in mean viewing times between mothers and nonmothers. An interaction between parity and emotion emerged: Mothers were more motivated to view happy faces and less motivated to view crying infant faces than nonmothers. Testosterone had a significant effect on viewing times: The higher the testosterone levels were, the shorter amount of time infant faces were viewed. This indicates that testosterone is inversely associated with approach motivation to emotional infant stimuli. This study is the first to compare mothers and nonmothers in a task measuring motivational responses to infant stimuli and indicates that the difference between the approach motivation caused by happy and distressed infant emotions might be more heightened in new mothers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142855884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does the Brief Implicit Association Test measure semantic or affective valence representations? 简要内隐联想测验测量的是语义效价表征还是情感效价表征?
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-16 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001480
Yiftach Argaman, Orit Heimer, Yoav Bar-Anan, Assaf Kron

Valence, the representation of a stimulus as positive or negative, is fundamental to conceptualizing attitudes and their empirical research. Valence has two potential representations: semantic and affective. The current line of studies investigates the degree to which the congruency effect of the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT), often used as an indirect evaluation measure, reflects affective or semantic aspects of valence. In three preregistered experiments (N = 1,056, with 352 participants each), we examined how the congruency effect of the BIAT reflects these aspects. In all three experiments, we used a repeated exposure manipulation, which typically causes a habituation effect on affective but not on semantic aspects of valence, to differentiate between the two types. In the first experiment, repeated exposure occurred before the BIAT, while in the second and third experiments, it was performed in the context of the BIAT task. We utilized three dependent variables: feelings-focused self-reports (measuring participants' reports about their feelings), knowledge-focused self-reports (measuring semantic evaluations), and the BIAT congruence effect. Supported by Bayesian analysis, we found consistent evidence that the repeated exposure manipulation influenced feelings-focused self-reports but did not affect knowledge-focused self-reports or the BIAT. The results suggest that the BIAT effect is sensitive to semantic (and not affective) representations of valence. Implications for attitude theory and measurement are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

效价,即刺激的积极或消极表征,是态度概念化及其实证研究的基础。效价有两种潜在的表征:语义和情感。摘要简要内隐联想测验(BIAT)作为一种间接评价手段,其一致性效应在多大程度上反映了效价的情感或语义方面,目前的研究主要集中在此方面。在三个预注册实验中(N = 1,056,每个实验有352名参与者),我们研究了BIAT的一致性效应如何反映这些方面。在所有三个实验中,我们使用了重复暴露操作,这通常会对效价的情感方面而不是语义方面产生习惯效应,以区分两种类型。在第一个实验中,重复暴露发生在BIAT前,而在第二个和第三个实验中,重复暴露发生在BIAT任务的背景下。我们使用了三个因变量:以感觉为中心的自我报告(测量参与者关于他们的感觉的报告),以知识为中心的自我报告(测量语义评价)和BIAT一致性效应。在贝叶斯分析的支持下,我们发现了一致的证据,即重复暴露操作影响了以感觉为中心的自我报告,但不影响以知识为中心的自我报告或BIAT。结果表明,BIAT效应对价态的语义表征敏感(而非情感表征)。讨论了态度理论和测量的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Does the Brief Implicit Association Test measure semantic or affective valence representations?","authors":"Yiftach Argaman, Orit Heimer, Yoav Bar-Anan, Assaf Kron","doi":"10.1037/emo0001480","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001480","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Valence, the representation of a stimulus as positive or negative, is fundamental to conceptualizing attitudes and their empirical research. Valence has two potential representations: semantic and affective. The current line of studies investigates the degree to which the congruency effect of the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT), often used as an indirect evaluation measure, reflects affective or semantic aspects of valence. In three preregistered experiments (<i>N</i> = 1,056, with 352 participants each), we examined how the congruency effect of the BIAT reflects these aspects. In all three experiments, we used a repeated exposure manipulation, which typically causes a habituation effect on affective but not on semantic aspects of valence, to differentiate between the two types. In the first experiment, repeated exposure occurred before the BIAT, while in the second and third experiments, it was performed in the context of the BIAT task. We utilized three dependent variables: feelings-focused self-reports (measuring participants' reports about their feelings), knowledge-focused self-reports (measuring semantic evaluations), and the BIAT congruence effect. Supported by Bayesian analysis, we found consistent evidence that the repeated exposure manipulation influenced feelings-focused self-reports but did not affect knowledge-focused self-reports or the BIAT. The results suggest that the BIAT effect is sensitive to semantic (and not affective) representations of valence. Implications for attitude theory and measurement are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142830271","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1