首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Differentiating anticipated and anticipatory emotions and their sensitivity to depressive symptoms. 区分预期情绪和期待情绪及其对抑郁症状的敏感性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001371
J Helgi Clayton McClure, Kevin J Riggs, Stephen A Dewhurst, Rachel J Anderson

Anticipated emotions are the feelings one expects if a hypothetical future event were to occur, whereas anticipatory emotions are those one experiences right now while imagining the event. There has been little direct comparison of these two forms of future-oriented emotion, and authors have typically focused on positive emotions (e.g., pleasure). Besides, their sensitivity to depressive symptoms-which may help to explain motivational problems in depression-has only recently been investigated (e.g., Anderson et al., 2023; Gamble et al., 2021). The present study (conducted September-November 2022) used innovative picture-and-text vignettes depicting everyday positive and negative future events, to which participants rated their anticipated and anticipatory responses on separate dimensions of valence (i.e., how positive or negative) and arousal (i.e., emotional intensity). Based on prior literature, anticipatory emotions were expected to be correlated with, yet weaker than, anticipated emotions, reflecting a conceptualization of anticipatory emotions as a "foretaste" of the affective response one expects in the future. We also predicted that high depressive symptoms would coincide with diminished emotion ratings overall and specifically for anticipatory emotions (tightly coupled with event expectations; Carrera et al., 2012). Results largely supported these preregistered predictions, yet anticipatory emotions (positive and negative) were only weaker in more highly depressed participants. Depressive symptoms may therefore affect how one currently feels about future possibilities without altering one's expectations of how such events would actually feel. Implications and future research objectives arising from this are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

预期情绪是一个人对假设的未来事件发生时的预期感受,而期待情绪则是一个人在想象事件发生时的当下体验。这两种面向未来的情绪形式很少有直接的比较,作者们通常关注的是积极情绪(如愉悦)。此外,它们对抑郁症状的敏感性--这可能有助于解释抑郁症中的动机问题--也只是最近才被研究(例如,Anderson 等人,2023 年;Gamble 等人,2021 年)。本研究(2022 年 9 月至 11 月进行)使用了创新性的图文并茂的小故事,描述了日常的积极和消极未来事件,参与者分别从情绪(即积极或消极程度)和唤醒(即情绪强度)两个维度对其预期和预期反应进行评分。根据先前的文献,预期情绪与预期情绪相关,但弱于预期情绪,这反映了预期情绪的概念化,即人们对未来预期情绪反应的 "预尝"。我们还预测,高抑郁症状会与整体情绪评分降低同时出现,特别是预期情绪评分降低(与事件预期紧密相关;Carrera 等人,2012 年)。结果在很大程度上支持了这些预先登记的预测,但预期情绪(积极和消极情绪)仅在抑郁程度较高的参与者中表现较弱。因此,抑郁症状可能会影响一个人目前对未来可能性的感受,而不会改变他对此类事件实际感受的预期。本文讨论了由此产生的影响和未来的研究目标。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Differentiating anticipated and anticipatory emotions and their sensitivity to depressive symptoms.","authors":"J Helgi Clayton McClure, Kevin J Riggs, Stephen A Dewhurst, Rachel J Anderson","doi":"10.1037/emo0001371","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001371","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anticipated emotions are the feelings one <i>expects</i> if a hypothetical future event were to occur, whereas anticipatory emotions are those one <i>experiences right now</i> while imagining the event. There has been little direct comparison of these two forms of future-oriented emotion, and authors have typically focused on positive emotions (e.g., pleasure). Besides, their sensitivity to depressive symptoms-which may help to explain motivational problems in depression-has only recently been investigated (e.g., Anderson et al., 2023; Gamble et al., 2021). The present study (conducted September-November 2022) used innovative picture-and-text vignettes depicting everyday positive and negative future events, to which participants rated their anticipated and anticipatory responses on separate dimensions of valence (i.e., how positive or negative) and arousal (i.e., emotional intensity). Based on prior literature, anticipatory emotions were expected to be correlated with, yet weaker than, anticipated emotions, reflecting a conceptualization of anticipatory emotions as a \"foretaste\" of the affective response one expects in the future. We also predicted that high depressive symptoms would coincide with diminished emotion ratings overall and specifically for anticipatory emotions (tightly coupled with event expectations; Carrera et al., 2012). Results largely supported these preregistered predictions, yet anticipatory emotions (positive and negative) were only weaker in more highly depressed participants. Depressive symptoms may therefore affect how one currently feels about future possibilities without altering one's expectations of how such events would actually feel. Implications and future research objectives arising from this are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1642-1651"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between changes in peer victimization and emotion dysregulation across adolescence. 青少年时期同伴伤害与情绪失调之间的纵向联系。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001369
Toria Herd, Celina Meyer, Brooks Casas, Jungmeen Kim-Spoon

Emotion dysregulation emerges from an interaction between individual factors and environmental factors. Changes in biological, cognitive, and social systems that characterize adolescence create a complex array of environmental factors contributing to emotion dysregulation during this developmental period. In particular, peer victimization (PV) has long-term consequences for emotion dysregulation. Yet, previous research has also indicated that emotion dysregulation can be both an antecedent to and outcome of PV. The present study evaluated reciprocal associations between longitudinal changes within repeated measures of PV and emotion dysregulation across adolescence and into young adulthood. The sample included 167 adolescents (53% male, Mage = 14.07 years at Time 1) who participated in a longitudinal study across five time points, with approximately 1 year between each assessment. Latent change score modeling was used to examine reciprocal associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Results emphasize bidirectional associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Consistent with social information processing theory, greater emotion dysregulation predicted greater relational and overt victimization over time. Moreover, higher overt victimization predicted increases in emotion dysregulation. Our results offer insights toward developmentally informed longitudinal, transactional models linking negative social environments, and emotion dysregulation development across adolescence and into young adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪失调是个体因素和环境因素相互作用的结果。作为青春期特征的生物、认知和社会系统的变化产生了一系列复杂的环境因素,导致了这一发育阶段的情绪失调。其中,同伴伤害(PV)会对情绪失调产生长期影响。然而,以往的研究也表明,情绪失调既可能是同伴伤害的先兆,也可能是同伴伤害的结果。本研究评估了在整个青春期和进入青年期后,重复测量 PV 的纵向变化与情绪失调之间的相互关系。样本包括 167 名青少年(53% 为男性,年龄=14.07 岁(时间 1)),他们在五个时间点参与了纵向研究,每次评估之间相隔约一年。研究采用了潜伏变化评分模型来检验 PV 与情绪失调之间的相互关系。结果表明,PV 与情绪失调之间存在双向关联。与社会信息处理理论相一致的是,情绪失调程度越高,随着时间的推移,关系受害和公开受害的程度也越高。此外,较高的公开受害程度也预示着情绪失调程度的增加。我们的研究结果为将消极的社会环境和情绪失调的发展联系到整个青春期和青年期的交易型纵向发展模型提供了启示。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between changes in peer victimization and emotion dysregulation across adolescence.","authors":"Toria Herd, Celina Meyer, Brooks Casas, Jungmeen Kim-Spoon","doi":"10.1037/emo0001369","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001369","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion dysregulation emerges from an interaction between individual factors and environmental factors. Changes in biological, cognitive, and social systems that characterize adolescence create a complex array of environmental factors contributing to emotion dysregulation during this developmental period. In particular, peer victimization (PV) has long-term consequences for emotion dysregulation. Yet, previous research has also indicated that emotion dysregulation can be both an antecedent to and outcome of PV. The present study evaluated reciprocal associations between longitudinal changes within repeated measures of PV and emotion dysregulation across adolescence and into young adulthood. The sample included 167 adolescents (53% male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 14.07 years at Time 1) who participated in a longitudinal study across five time points, with approximately 1 year between each assessment. Latent change score modeling was used to examine reciprocal associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Results emphasize bidirectional associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Consistent with social information processing theory, greater emotion dysregulation predicted greater relational and overt victimization over time. Moreover, higher overt victimization predicted increases in emotion dysregulation. Our results offer insights toward developmentally informed longitudinal, transactional models linking negative social environments, and emotion dysregulation development across adolescence and into young adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1612-1624"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of executive function in cognitive reappraisal: A meta-analytic review. 执行功能在认知再评价中的作用:元分析综述。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001373
Wei Xing Toh, Jun Sheng Keh, James J Gross, Laura L Carstensen

Cognitive reappraisal refers to the reinterpretation of a situation to alter its emotional meaning. Theoretically, executive functions (EFs), such as inhibition, updating, and shifting, are core elements of reappraisal processes. However, empirical studies have yielded inconsistent evidence as to whether and to what extent EFs are associated with reappraisal. To address this issue, we conducted a meta-analysis of the literature in which 179 effect sizes from 59 independent samples (N = 4,703) were included. Using random-effects metaregression with robust-variance estimates and small-sample corrections, we also examined whether variation in effect sizes could be accounted for by potential moderators, such as the way reappraisal was assessed (i.e., questionnaires vs. task-based measures) and the type of stimuli used in EF tasks (i.e., affective vs. nonaffective). Overall, results indicate relatively small to typical associations between reappraisal and all three EFs (rs = .13-.19). While the way reappraisal was measured did not moderate any of the relations between EF and reappraisal, we found stronger relations between inhibition and reappraisal when EF was assessed using tasks that involved affective, relative to nonaffective, stimuli. Our meta-analytic findings offer modest support for the idea that EFs are cognitive constituents of reappraisal processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

认知再评价指的是对情境进行重新解释,以改变其情感含义。从理论上讲,抑制、更新和转移等执行功能(EFs)是再评价过程的核心要素。然而,关于执行功能是否以及在多大程度上与再评价相关,实证研究得出的证据并不一致。为了解决这个问题,我们对文献进行了荟萃分析,纳入了来自 59 个独立样本(N = 4703)的 179 个效应大小。通过使用随机效应元回归、稳健方差估计和小样本校正,我们还研究了效应大小的变化是否可以通过潜在的调节因素来解释,例如重新评价的评估方式(即问卷调查与基于任务的测量)以及在 EF 任务中使用的刺激类型(即情感性与非情感性)。总体而言,研究结果表明,再评价与所有三种 EF 之间的关联相对较小,但也很典型(rs = .13-.19)。虽然重评的测量方式并没有调节 EF 与重评之间的任何关系,但我们发现,当使用涉及情感性刺激的任务评估 EF 时,抑制与重评之间的关系比使用非情感性刺激时更强。我们的荟萃分析结果为 "EF 是再评价过程的认知成分 "这一观点提供了适度的支持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of executive function in cognitive reappraisal: A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Wei Xing Toh, Jun Sheng Keh, James J Gross, Laura L Carstensen","doi":"10.1037/emo0001373","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001373","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive reappraisal refers to the reinterpretation of a situation to alter its emotional meaning. Theoretically, executive functions (EFs), such as inhibition, updating, and shifting, are core elements of reappraisal processes. However, empirical studies have yielded inconsistent evidence as to whether and to what extent EFs are associated with reappraisal. To address this issue, we conducted a meta-analysis of the literature in which 179 effect sizes from 59 independent samples (<i>N</i> = 4,703) were included. Using random-effects metaregression with robust-variance estimates and small-sample corrections, we also examined whether variation in effect sizes could be accounted for by potential moderators, such as the way reappraisal was assessed (i.e., questionnaires vs. task-based measures) and the type of stimuli used in EF tasks (i.e., affective vs. nonaffective). Overall, results indicate relatively small to typical associations between reappraisal and all three EFs (<i>r</i>s = .13-.19). While the way reappraisal was measured did not moderate any of the relations between EF and reappraisal, we found stronger relations between inhibition and reappraisal when EF was assessed using tasks that involved affective, relative to nonaffective, stimuli. Our meta-analytic findings offer modest support for the idea that EFs are cognitive constituents of reappraisal processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1563-1581"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141311980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining demand effects on direct and indirect affect measures in affect induction procedures. 研究需求对情感诱导程序中直接和间接情感测量的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001368
Mario Wenzel, Matthias Winkler, Jonathan Lasi, Zarah Rowland

Affect induction procedures are effectively implemented in psychological research. However, because participants are typically asked to self-report their affect immediately after viewing emotional stimuli, the goal of eliciting affect is relatively easy for participants to infer, making their responses susceptible to demand effects. To examine this demand effect, research has used an unrelated-studies paradigm, in which participants are led to believe that they are participating in two different, unrelated studies. While this paradigm has been used in some studies using affect induction procedures, none have examined the extent of demand effects in affect induction procedures. To do so, we conducted six online experiments (N = 170, N = 254, N = 664, N = 260, N = 239, N = 249) by contrasting an unrelated- with a related-studies design. The participants in the related-studies condition were to believe that the affect measurement after the induction belonged to the same pretest as the affect induction, whereas the participants in the unrelated-studies condition were to believe that this measurement was part of a second, unrelated pretest. We found that a related- versus unrelated-studies design produced a significant demand effect for both positive and negative affect, as indicated by greater increases in positive and negative affect in the related-studies compared with the unrelated-studies condition. Demand effects were also found on some indirect measures of affect, as reflected by a significantly smaller self-reported momentary thought-action repertoire, but not by worse memory performance or more distrust in the related-studies condition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情感诱导程序在心理学研究中得到了有效实施。然而,由于参与者通常会被要求在观看情绪刺激后立即自我报告他们的情绪,因此参与者相对容易推断出诱发情绪的目标,从而使他们的反应容易受到需求效应的影响。为了研究这种需求效应,研究人员使用了无关研究范式,即让参与者相信他们正在参与两项不同的、无关的研究。虽然这种范式已在一些使用情感诱导程序的研究中使用过,但还没有一项研究考察了情感诱导程序中需求效应的程度。为此,我们进行了六次在线实验(N = 170、N = 254、N = 664、N = 260、N = 239、N = 249),对比了无关研究和相关研究设计。在相关研究条件下,参与者要相信诱导后的情感测量属于与情感诱导相同的前测,而在非相关研究条件下,参与者要相信该测量属于第二个非相关前测的一部分。我们发现,相关学习与非相关学习的设计对积极和消极情绪都产生了显著的需求效应,这表现在相关学习条件下的积极和消极情绪比非相关学习条件下的积极和消极情绪增加得更多。在一些间接的情感测量中也发现了需求效应,这体现在自我报告的瞬间思维-行动剧目明显较少,但在相关研究条件下,记忆表现较差或更不自信。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Examining demand effects on direct and indirect affect measures in affect induction procedures.","authors":"Mario Wenzel, Matthias Winkler, Jonathan Lasi, Zarah Rowland","doi":"10.1037/emo0001368","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001368","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affect induction procedures are effectively implemented in psychological research. However, because participants are typically asked to self-report their affect immediately after viewing emotional stimuli, the goal of eliciting affect is relatively easy for participants to infer, making their responses susceptible to demand effects. To examine this demand effect, research has used an unrelated-studies paradigm, in which participants are led to believe that they are participating in two different, unrelated studies. While this paradigm has been used in some studies using affect induction procedures, none have examined the extent of demand effects in affect induction procedures. To do so, we conducted six online experiments (<i>N</i> = 170, <i>N</i> = 254, <i>N</i> = 664, <i>N</i> = 260, <i>N</i> = 239, <i>N</i> = 249) by contrasting an unrelated- with a related-studies design. The participants in the related-studies condition were to believe that the affect measurement after the induction belonged to the same pretest as the affect induction, whereas the participants in the unrelated-studies condition were to believe that this measurement was part of a second, unrelated pretest. We found that a related- versus unrelated-studies design produced a significant demand effect for both positive and negative affect, as indicated by greater increases in positive and negative affect in the related-studies compared with the unrelated-studies condition. Demand effects were also found on some indirect measures of affect, as reflected by a significantly smaller self-reported momentary thought-action repertoire, but not by worse memory performance or more distrust in the related-studies condition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1753-1770"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in emotion prediction and implications for social success. 情绪预测的个体差异及其对社交成功的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001386
Elyssa M Barrick, Mark A Thornton, Zidong Zhao, Diana I Tamir

The social world requires people to predict others' thoughts, feelings, and actions. People who successfully predict others' emotions experience significant social advantages. What makes a person good at predicting emotions? To predict others' future emotional states, a person must know how one emotion transitions to the next. People learn how emotions transition from at least two sources: (a) internal information, or one's own emotion experiences, and (b) external information, such as the social cues detected in a person's face. Across five studies collected between 2018 and 2020, we find evidence that both sources of information are related to accurate emotion prediction: individuals with atypical personal emotion transitions, difficulty understanding their own emotional experiences, and impaired emotion perception displayed impaired emotion prediction. This ability to predict others' emotions has real-world social implications. Individuals who make accurate emotion predictions have better relationships with their friends and communities and experience less loneliness. In contrast, disruptions in both internal and external information sources explain prediction inaccuracy in individuals with social difficulties, specifically with social communication difficulties common in autism spectrum disorder. These findings provide evidence that successful emotion prediction, which relies on the perception of accurate internal and external data about how emotions transition, may be key to social success. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会世界要求人们预测他人的思想、情感和行为。成功预测他人情绪的人在社交中会获得巨大优势。是什么让一个人善于预测情绪呢?要预测他人未来的情绪状态,一个人必须知道一种情绪是如何过渡到下一种情绪的。人们至少可以从以下两个方面了解情绪是如何过渡的:(a) 内部信息,即一个人自身的情绪体验;(b) 外部信息,例如从一个人的脸上察觉到的社交线索。在2018年至2020年期间收集的五项研究中,我们发现有证据表明,这两种信息来源都与准确的情绪预测有关:个人情绪转换不典型、难以理解自身情绪体验以及情绪感知受损的个体显示出情绪预测能力受损。这种预测他人情绪的能力具有现实的社会意义。情绪预测准确的人与朋友和社区的关系更好,孤独感更少。与此相反,内部和外部信息源的干扰解释了社交障碍患者预测不准确的原因,特别是自闭症谱系障碍中常见的社交沟通障碍。这些发现提供了证据,证明成功的情绪预测可能是社交成功的关键,而成功的情绪预测依赖于对情绪转变的准确内部和外部数据的感知。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Individual differences in emotion prediction and implications for social success.","authors":"Elyssa M Barrick, Mark A Thornton, Zidong Zhao, Diana I Tamir","doi":"10.1037/emo0001386","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001386","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The social world requires people to predict others' thoughts, feelings, and actions. People who successfully predict others' emotions experience significant social advantages. What makes a person good at predicting emotions? To predict others' future emotional states, a person must know how one emotion transitions to the next. People learn how emotions transition from at least two sources: (a) <i>internal information,</i> or one's own emotion experiences, and (b) <i>external information,</i> such as the social cues detected in a person's face. Across five studies collected between 2018 and 2020, we find evidence that both sources of information are related to accurate emotion prediction: individuals with atypical personal emotion transitions, difficulty understanding their own emotional experiences, and impaired emotion perception displayed impaired emotion prediction. This ability to predict others' emotions has real-world social implications. Individuals who make accurate emotion predictions have better relationships with their friends and communities and experience less loneliness. In contrast, disruptions in both internal and external information sources explain prediction inaccuracy in individuals with social difficulties, specifically with social communication difficulties common in autism spectrum disorder. These findings provide evidence that successful emotion prediction, which relies on the perception of accurate internal and external data about how emotions transition, may be key to social success. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1697-1708"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427972","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A brief reappraisal intervention leads to durable affective benefits. 简短的重新评估干预会带来持久的情感益处。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001391
Julia W Y Kam, Lauren Wan-Sai-Cheong, Alexandra A Ouellette Zuk, Ashish Mehta, Matthew L Dixon, James J Gross

People who report frequently using cognitive reappraisal to decrease the impact of potentially upsetting situations report better affective functioning than people who report using cognitive reappraisal less frequently. However, most work linking everyday reappraisal use to affective outcomes has been correlational, making causal inference difficult. In this study, we examined whether 2 weeks of daily practice of reappraising negatively valenced personally relevant events would improve affective functioning compared with a wait-list control. Data were collected between 2021 and 2022 from a sample mainly comprised of females (82%) and who identified as Asian (35%) or White/Caucasian (40%). Our planned analyses indicated that reappraisal decreased depressive symptoms and perceived stress as well as increased life satisfaction both immediately and 4 weeks postintervention. Reductions in depressive symptoms and perceived stress were mediated by increases in reappraisal self-efficacy. These findings support the causal efficacy of brief reappraisal training. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

经常使用认知再评价来减少潜在令人不安情况影响的人,其情感功能要好于较少使用认知再评价的人。然而,大多数将日常重评的使用与情感结果联系起来的研究都是相关性的,因此很难进行因果推论。在本研究中,我们考察了与等待对照组相比,每天练习两周重新评估负向个人相关事件是否会改善情感功能。数据收集时间为 2021 年至 2022 年,样本主要由女性(82%)和亚裔(35%)或白人/高加索人(40%)组成。我们计划进行的分析表明,干预后 4 周内和干预后 4 周内,重新评估都能减少抑郁症状和感知压力,并提高生活满意度。抑郁症状和感知压力的减轻是由再评价自我效能感的增强所促成的。这些研究结果支持了简短再评价训练的因果功效。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A brief reappraisal intervention leads to durable affective benefits.","authors":"Julia W Y Kam, Lauren Wan-Sai-Cheong, Alexandra A Ouellette Zuk, Ashish Mehta, Matthew L Dixon, James J Gross","doi":"10.1037/emo0001391","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001391","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People who report frequently using cognitive reappraisal to decrease the impact of potentially upsetting situations report better affective functioning than people who report using cognitive reappraisal less frequently. However, most work linking everyday reappraisal use to affective outcomes has been correlational, making causal inference difficult. In this study, we examined whether 2 weeks of daily practice of reappraising negatively valenced personally relevant events would improve affective functioning compared with a wait-list control. Data were collected between 2021 and 2022 from a sample mainly comprised of females (82%) and who identified as Asian (35%) or White/Caucasian (40%). Our planned analyses indicated that reappraisal decreased depressive symptoms and perceived stress as well as increased life satisfaction both immediately and 4 weeks postintervention. Reductions in depressive symptoms and perceived stress were mediated by increases in reappraisal self-efficacy. These findings support the causal efficacy of brief reappraisal training. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1676-1688"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional clarity in daily life is associated with reduced indecisiveness and greater goal pursuit. 日常生活中的情绪清晰度与优柔寡断的减少和对目标的更大追求有关。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001384
Nathaniel S Eckland, Rebecca L Feldman, Haijing Wu Hallenbeck, Renee J Thompson

Affect-as-information theory posits that understanding of one's emotions (i.e., emotional clarity) can be leveraged to make decisions and attain goals. Furthermore, recent work has emphasized the dynamic nature of emotional clarity and its fluctuations in daily life. Therefore, we sought to test how momentary emotional clarity, experienced in everyday life, would be associated with levels of indecisiveness and goal pursuit. Following affect-as-information, we hypothesized that emotional clarity would be associated with lower indecisiveness but greater goal pursuit. We also hypothesized that indecisiveness would be associated with less goal pursuit with momentary emotional clarity being a potential moderator of this association. Adults (N = 215, Mage = 44.3) experiencing a range of depression, a disorder characterized by indecisiveness, completed a self-report measure of indecisiveness and 2 weeks of experience sampling assessing momentary emotional clarity, goal pursuit, and negative affect. Momentary emotional clarity showed robust links to lower indecisiveness and greater goal pursuit that were not accounted for by negative affect. We did not observe a link between indecisiveness and goal pursuit. Emotional clarity appears to play a role in motivational and cognitive processes that unfold in daily life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情感即信息理论认为,了解一个人的情感(即情感清晰度)可以用来做出决策和实现目标。此外,最近的研究强调了情绪清晰度的动态性质及其在日常生活中的波动。因此,我们试图测试在日常生活中体验到的瞬间情绪清晰度与优柔寡断和目标追求水平之间的关系。根据 "情感即信息 "的理论,我们假设情感清晰度会与较低的优柔寡断程度和较高的目标追求程度相关。我们还假设,优柔寡断与较少的目标追求有关,而瞬间的情绪清晰度则是这种关联的潜在调节因素。患有一系列以优柔寡断为特征的抑郁症的成年人(人数 = 215,年龄 = 44.3)完成了一项关于优柔寡断的自我报告测量,并对瞬间情绪清晰度、目标追求和负面情绪进行了为期两周的经验取样评估。瞬时情绪清晰度与较低的优柔寡断程度和较高的目标追求之间存在密切联系,而负面情绪则无法解释这种联系。我们没有观察到优柔寡断和目标追求之间的联系。情绪清晰度似乎在日常生活中展开的动机和认知过程中发挥了作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Emotional clarity in daily life is associated with reduced indecisiveness and greater goal pursuit.","authors":"Nathaniel S Eckland, Rebecca L Feldman, Haijing Wu Hallenbeck, Renee J Thompson","doi":"10.1037/emo0001384","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001384","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affect-as-information theory posits that understanding of one's emotions (i.e., emotional clarity) can be leveraged to make decisions and attain goals. Furthermore, recent work has emphasized the dynamic nature of emotional clarity and its fluctuations in daily life. Therefore, we sought to test how momentary emotional clarity, experienced in everyday life, would be associated with levels of indecisiveness and goal pursuit. Following affect-as-information, we hypothesized that emotional clarity would be associated with lower indecisiveness but greater goal pursuit. We also hypothesized that indecisiveness would be associated with less goal pursuit with momentary emotional clarity being a potential moderator of this association. Adults (<i>N</i> = 215, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 44.3) experiencing a range of depression, a disorder characterized by indecisiveness, completed a self-report measure of indecisiveness and 2 weeks of experience sampling assessing momentary emotional clarity, goal pursuit, and negative affect. Momentary emotional clarity showed robust links to lower indecisiveness and greater goal pursuit that were not accounted for by negative affect. We did not observe a link between indecisiveness and goal pursuit. Emotional clarity appears to play a role in motivational and cognitive processes that unfold in daily life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1771-1775"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Holiday or hell? Emotion regulation and memory of depressive symptoms during lockdown. 假期还是地狱?封锁期间的情绪调节和抑郁症状记忆。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001367
Valerie T Chang, Nickola C Overall

The ongoing repercussions of the COVID-19 pandemic provide an unparalleled context to examine how distressing events are remembered. Prior theory and research suggest that (a) distress during lockdowns may fade and be remembered as less distressing, or remain salient and be remembered as more distressing, than initially experienced and (b) emotional suppression and cognitive reappraisal may predict these memory biases. We test these possibilities by assessing depressive symptoms and emotion regulation during two lockdowns: at the start of the pandemic (Lockdown 2020) and 17 months later (Lockdown 2021) in a sample of parents with young children (N = 272). We assessed tracking accuracy, directional bias, and projection bias in memory of depressive symptoms in Lockdown 2020, and the moderating role of emotion regulation at the time of encoding (Lockdown 2020) and recall (Lockdown 2021). People experiencing more depressive symptoms later in the pandemic (2021) remembered the start of the pandemic (2020) to involve more depressive symptoms than initially experienced (projection bias). People engaging in greater emotional suppression at recall (2021) were less likely to project their current depressive symptoms onto memory of the prior lockdown (lower projection bias) and remembered feeling lower depressive symptoms than initially experienced (underestimation directional bias). By contrast, people engaging in cognitive reappraisal at recall (2021) remembered feeling greater depressive symptoms than they initially experienced (overestimation directional bias). These unexpected results indicate that emotion regulation may shape memory of emotion during real-life challenges differently than patterns observed in lab-based research and may reflect helpful coping with life events. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

COVID-19 大流行病的持续影响为研究如何记忆痛苦事件提供了无与伦比的背景。先前的理论和研究表明:(a) 与最初的经历相比,封锁期间的痛苦可能会消退并被记忆为不那么痛苦,或者仍然突出并被记忆为更痛苦;(b) 情绪抑制和认知再评价可能会预测这些记忆偏差。我们通过在两次封锁期间评估抑郁症状和情绪调节来验证这些可能性:在大流行开始时(2020 年封锁)和 17 个月后(2021 年封锁),以有年幼子女的父母为样本(N = 272)。我们评估了 2020 年 "禁闭 "期间抑郁症状记忆的追踪准确性、方向偏差和投射偏差,以及编码(2020 年 "禁闭")和回忆(2021 年 "禁闭")时情绪调节的调节作用。在大流行后期(2021 年)出现更多抑郁症状的人回忆起大流行开始时(2020 年)出现的抑郁症状比最初出现的要多(投射偏差)。在回忆时(2021 年)情绪压抑程度较高的人不太可能将目前的抑郁症状投射到之前的封锁记忆中(投射偏差较低),他们记忆中的抑郁症状比最初经历的要轻(低估方向偏差)。相比之下,在回忆时进行认知再评价(2021 年)的人所记得的抑郁症状比他们最初经历过的要严重(高估方向偏差)。这些出乎意料的结果表明,在现实生活中,情绪调节对情绪记忆的影响可能不同于实验室研究中观察到的模式,并可能反映出对生活事件的有益应对。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Holiday or hell? Emotion regulation and memory of depressive symptoms during lockdown.","authors":"Valerie T Chang, Nickola C Overall","doi":"10.1037/emo0001367","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001367","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ongoing repercussions of the COVID-19 pandemic provide an unparalleled context to examine how distressing events are remembered. Prior theory and research suggest that (a) distress during lockdowns may fade and be remembered as less distressing, or remain salient and be remembered as more distressing, than initially experienced and (b) emotional suppression and cognitive reappraisal may predict these memory biases. We test these possibilities by assessing depressive symptoms and emotion regulation during two lockdowns: at the start of the pandemic (Lockdown 2020) and 17 months later (Lockdown 2021) in a sample of parents with young children (<i>N</i> = 272). We assessed tracking accuracy, directional bias, and projection bias in memory of depressive symptoms in Lockdown 2020, and the moderating role of emotion regulation at the time of encoding (Lockdown 2020) and recall (Lockdown 2021). People experiencing more depressive symptoms later in the pandemic (2021) remembered the start of the pandemic (2020) to involve more depressive symptoms than initially experienced (projection bias). People engaging in greater emotional suppression at recall (2021) were less likely to project their current depressive symptoms onto memory of the prior lockdown (lower projection bias) and remembered feeling lower depressive symptoms than initially experienced (underestimation directional bias). By contrast, people engaging in cognitive reappraisal at recall (2021) remembered feeling greater depressive symptoms than they initially experienced (overestimation directional bias). These unexpected results indicate that emotion regulation may shape memory of emotion during real-life challenges differently than patterns observed in lab-based research and may reflect helpful coping with life events. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1625-1641"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Go with your gut! The beneficial mood effects of intuitive decisions. 跟着感觉走直觉决定对情绪的有益影响
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001385
Carina Remmers, Sascha Topolinski, Christine Knaevelsrud, Thea Zander-Schellenberg, Sebastian Unger, Albert Anoschin, Johannes Zimmermann

People make countless decisions every day. We explored the self-regulatory function of decisions and assumed that the very act of making a decision in everyday life enhances people's mood. We expected that this decision-related mood change would be more pronounced for intuitive decisions than for analytical ones. The ease of making a decision and the feeling of rightness were expected to mediate the effect of intuitive (vs. analytical) decisions on participants' mood. In a preregistered experimental experience sampling study, participants from the general population were asked to report when they were about to make an everyday decision over the course of 14 days (N = 256 participants, 6,779 decisions). For each decision, participants were randomly instructed to decide either based on their intuition or based on careful analysis. We assessed several variables before and immediately after the decision. Participants also reported retrospectively on their choices in voluntary follow-up assessments. Making a decision per se immediately enhanced participants' mood. This mood enhancement was stronger for intuitive compared to analytic decisions and persisted until follow-up. Ease of decision, but not feeling of rightness, mediated this effect. Intuitive decisions compared to analytic decisions were more likely to be implemented and led to greater satisfaction and pleasantness of the chosen option. Having more options for a particular decision led to generally higher mood improvement and satisfaction. This is the first empirical demonstration showing that using one's gut has beneficial effects in everyday life. Study limitations and implications for theory and practice are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们每天都要做出无数的决定。我们探讨了决策的自我调节功能,并假定在日常生活中做出决策这一行为本身会提升人们的情绪。我们预计,这种与决策相关的情绪变化在直觉决策中比在分析决策中更为明显。做出决定的难易程度和正确感预计会调节直觉型(与分析型)决定对参与者情绪的影响。在一项预先登记的实验经验取样研究中,来自普通人群的参与者被要求报告他们在 14 天内即将做出日常决定时的情况(N = 256 名参与者,6,779 个决定)。每次决策时,参与者都会被随机指示根据直觉或仔细分析做出决定。我们在决策前和决策后立即对几个变量进行了评估。参与者还在自愿的后续评估中回顾了他们的选择。做出决定本身会立即提升参与者的情绪。与分析型决策相比,直觉型决策对情绪的改善更明显,而且这种改善一直持续到后续评估。做出决定的难易程度(而非正确感)对这一效果起到了中介作用。与分析型决策相比,直觉型决策更有可能被执行,并能使所选方案带来更大的满足感和愉悦感。对某一特定决策有更多的选择,通常会带来更高的情绪改善和满意度。这是首次通过实证研究证明,在日常生活中运用自己的直觉会产生有益的影响。本文讨论了研究的局限性以及对理论和实践的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Go with your gut! The beneficial mood effects of intuitive decisions.","authors":"Carina Remmers, Sascha Topolinski, Christine Knaevelsrud, Thea Zander-Schellenberg, Sebastian Unger, Albert Anoschin, Johannes Zimmermann","doi":"10.1037/emo0001385","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001385","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People make countless decisions every day. We explored the self-regulatory function of decisions and assumed that the very act of making a decision in everyday life enhances people's mood. We expected that this decision-related mood change would be more pronounced for intuitive decisions than for analytical ones. The ease of making a decision and the feeling of rightness were expected to mediate the effect of intuitive (vs. analytical) decisions on participants' mood. In a preregistered experimental experience sampling study, participants from the general population were asked to report when they were about to make an everyday decision over the course of 14 days (<i>N</i> = 256 participants, 6,779 decisions). For each decision, participants were randomly instructed to decide either based on their intuition or based on careful analysis. We assessed several variables before and immediately after the decision. Participants also reported retrospectively on their choices in voluntary follow-up assessments. Making a decision per se immediately enhanced participants' mood. This mood enhancement was stronger for intuitive compared to analytic decisions and persisted until follow-up. Ease of decision, but not feeling of rightness, mediated this effect. Intuitive decisions compared to analytic decisions were more likely to be implemented and led to greater satisfaction and pleasantness of the chosen option. Having more options for a particular decision led to generally higher mood improvement and satisfaction. This is the first empirical demonstration showing that using one's gut has beneficial effects in everyday life. Study limitations and implications for theory and practice are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1652-1662"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141311979","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How bad becomes good: A neurocomputational model of affect-informed choice. 坏是如何变成好的?情感选择的神经计算模型。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001347
Ian D Roberts, Azadeh HajiHosseini, Cendri A Hutcherson

People often draw on their current affective experience to inform their decisions, yet little is known about the underlying mechanisms of this process. Understanding them has important implications for many big questions in both the affective and decision sciences. Do the same neural circuits that generate affect generate value? What differentiates people who have greater contextual flexibility in their reliance on affect? Do affective choices invoke processes that are distinct from less affective choices? To investigate these questions, we developed a neurocomputational model of affect-informed choice, in which people convert subjective affect into context-sensitive decision value through a process of weighted evidence accumulation. We then tested model predictions by recording electroencephalography and facial electromyography during a novel affective choice paradigm in a sample of racially diverse undergraduate participants (data collected in 2018-2019). In addition to validating our model, we found that generation of affective responses occurs earlier than, and is neurally distinct from, valuation of that affect. Moreover, individual differences in contextual flexibility of affective weighting correlated only with later valuation processes, not earlier affect generation processes. Our results have important theoretical implications for emotion, emotion regulation, and decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们经常利用自己当前的情感体验来做出决策,但人们对这一过程的内在机制却知之甚少。了解这些机制对情感科学和决策科学中的许多重大问题都有重要影响。产生情感的神经回路也会产生价值吗?在依赖情感方面具有更大情境灵活性的人有何不同?情感化的选择是否会引发与情感化程度较低的选择不同的过程?为了探究这些问题,我们建立了一个情感知情选择的神经计算模型,在该模型中,人们通过加权证据积累过程将主观情感转化为对情境敏感的决策价值。然后,我们在一个新颖的情感选择范式中,通过记录不同种族的本科生参与者的脑电图和面部肌电图(数据收集于2018-2019年),对模型预测进行了测试。除了验证我们的模型,我们还发现,情感反应的产生早于对该情感的评价,并且在神经上与之不同。此外,情感权重的情境灵活性的个体差异只与较晚的估价过程相关,而与较早的情感生成过程无关。我们的研究结果对情绪、情绪调节和决策制定具有重要的理论意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How bad becomes good: A neurocomputational model of affect-informed choice.","authors":"Ian D Roberts, Azadeh HajiHosseini, Cendri A Hutcherson","doi":"10.1037/emo0001347","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001347","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often draw on their current affective experience to inform their decisions, yet little is known about the underlying mechanisms of this process. Understanding them has important implications for many big questions in both the affective and decision sciences. Do the same neural circuits that generate affect generate value? What differentiates people who have greater contextual flexibility in their reliance on affect? Do affective choices invoke processes that are distinct from less affective choices? To investigate these questions, we developed a neurocomputational model of affect-informed choice, in which people convert subjective affect into context-sensitive decision value through a process of weighted evidence accumulation. We then tested model predictions by recording electroencephalography and facial electromyography during a novel affective choice paradigm in a sample of racially diverse undergraduate participants (data collected in 2018-2019). In addition to validating our model, we found that generation of affective responses occurs earlier than, and is neurally distinct from, valuation of that affect. Moreover, individual differences in contextual flexibility of affective weighting correlated only with later valuation processes, not earlier affect generation processes. Our results have important theoretical implications for emotion, emotion regulation, and decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1737-1752"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427971","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1