首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Revisiting the moral forecasting error – A preregistered replication and extension of “Are we more moral than we think?” 重新审视道德预测错误--"我们比我们想象的更有道德吗?"的预登记复制和扩展
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104662
Simen Bø, Hallgeir Sjåstad

Predictions are often inaccurate. Still, the direction of prediction errors may vary. Contrary to research on the intention-behavior gap, where people fail to live up to their ambitions, a study on “moral forecasting” found that people behaved more honestly than they predicted. In this registered report, we present two close replication attempts and one conceptual replication attempt of this moral forecasting error across two experiments. In Experiment 1 (N = 1839), we recruited a general population sample from the same country as the original study (Canada) to an online experiment. We successfully replicated the moral forecasting error using a math-based cheating task from the original study: Predicted cheating was much higher in a moral forecasting condition than actual cheating in a moral action condition (d = 0.69). In Experiment 2 (N = 1381) we replicated the forecasting error again, using the same task in a general population sample from the U.S. (d = 0.72). However, we were unable to conceptually replicate the effect using a different dishonesty measure, the “mind game”, in Experiment 1 (φ = 0.03). We also could not reduce the forecasting error through a debiasing intervention in Experiment 2 (d = 0.01). Across both experiments, participants predicted that others would cheat much more than they would themselves. In this registered report, we conclude that the moral forecasting error is robust for the original cheating task. We also show that it can generalize contextually (from a lab to an online setting), but not to a different task. Future research may show exactly when predictions about one's own honesty are pessimistic rather than optimistic.

预测往往是不准确的。不过,预测错误的方向可能会有所不同。与人们未能实现其抱负的 "意图-行为差距 "研究相反,一项关于 "道德预测 "的研究发现,人们的行为比他们预测的更诚实。在本注册报告中,我们介绍了在两个实验中对这一道德预测误差进行的两次近似复制尝试和一次概念复制尝试。在实验 1(N = 1839)中,我们从与原始研究相同的国家(加拿大)招募了一个普通人群样本进行在线实验。我们使用原始研究中基于数学的作弊任务,成功地复制了道德预测误差:道德预测条件下的预测作弊率远远高于道德行动条件下的实际作弊率(d = 0.69)。在实验 2(N = 1381)中,我们在美国的普通人群样本中使用相同的任务再次重复了预测误差(d = 0.72)。然而,在实验 1 中,我们使用不同的不诚实测量方法 "智力游戏"(φ = 0.03),却无法在概念上复制这种效应。在实验 2 中,我们也无法通过去伪存真干预来减少预测误差(d = 0.01)。在这两个实验中,参与者对他人作弊的预测都远远高于对自己作弊的预测。在这份注册报告中,我们得出结论,道德预测误差对于最初的作弊任务来说是稳健的。我们还表明,它可以在情境中(从实验室到网络环境)泛化,但不能泛化到不同的任务中。未来的研究可能会准确地表明,什么时候对自己诚实的预测是悲观的,而不是乐观的。
{"title":"Revisiting the moral forecasting error – A preregistered replication and extension of “Are we more moral than we think?”","authors":"Simen Bø,&nbsp;Hallgeir Sjåstad","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104662","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104662","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Predictions are often inaccurate. Still, the direction of prediction errors may vary. Contrary to research on the intention-behavior gap, where people fail to live up to their ambitions, a study on “moral forecasting” found that people behaved <em>more</em> honestly than they predicted. In this registered report, we present two close replication attempts and one conceptual replication attempt of this moral forecasting error across two experiments. In Experiment 1 (<em>N</em> = 1839), we recruited a general population sample from the same country as the original study (Canada) to an online experiment. We successfully replicated the moral forecasting error using a math-based cheating task from the original study: Predicted cheating was much higher in a moral forecasting condition than actual cheating in a moral action condition (<em>d</em> = 0.69). In Experiment 2 (<em>N</em> = 1381) we replicated the forecasting error again, using the same task in a general population sample from the U.S. (<em>d</em> = 0.72). However, we were unable to conceptually replicate the effect using a different dishonesty measure, the “mind game”, in Experiment 1 (φ = 0.03). We also could not reduce the forecasting error through a debiasing intervention in Experiment 2 (<em>d</em> = 0.01). Across both experiments, participants predicted that others would cheat much more than they would themselves. In this registered report, we conclude that the moral forecasting error is robust for the original cheating task. We also show that it can generalize contextually (from a lab to an online setting), but not to a different task. Future research may show exactly when predictions about one's own honesty are pessimistic rather than optimistic.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104662"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000751/pdfft?md5=2e07dae9b41a7e365c6dd932efdd8151&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000751-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142122917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fairness revisionism: Reducing discrimination for the future reduces perceived unfairness in the past 公平修正主义:减少对未来的歧视,减少过去的不公平感
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104671
Tito L.H. Grillo , Shuhan Yang , Adrian F. Ward

Marginalized groups may face systemic discrimination for generations until concrete advancements in society finally ensure fairer treatment for their members. Although fairness advancements may benefit these groups in the present and future, they do not change the past; they cannot undo the discrimination already experienced by previous generations. However, five studies (N = 1672) suggest that fairness advancements that benefit a marginalized group may change how its members perceive their own prior experiences with discrimination, leading them to see these experiences as having been fairer compared to when there are no such advancements. We find evidence of this revisionism of unfair past experiences in different historically marginalized groups (women and immigrants) and cultural contexts (U.S., U.K., and China). Critically, fairness revisionism arises even when fairness advancements have no objective impact on individuals themselves, as long as there are benefits for current and future members of their social group. Fairness revisionism does not arise, however, in response to gains for marginalized groups to which one does not belong, nor when individuals assess fairness in other groups' past experiences from an outsider's perspective. Overall, this phenomenon may be a double-edged sword: it may provide peace of mind for those treated unfairly by assuaging the memory of adverse experiences, but may also make discrimination issues in society seem less pressing based on the perspective of victims themselves.

边缘化群体可能会世世代代面临系统性歧视,直到社会的具体进步最终确保其成员得到更公平的待遇。尽管公平性的进步可能会使这些群体在现在和将来受益,但它们并不能改变过去;它们无法消除前几代人已经经历过的歧视。然而,五项研究(N = 1672)表明,使边缘化群体受益的公平进步可能会改变其成员对自己以前所受歧视经历的看法,从而使他们认为这些经历与没有公平进步时相比更加公平。我们在不同的历史边缘化群体(妇女和移民)和文化背景(美国、英国和中国)中发现了这种对过去不公平经历进行修正的证据。重要的是,即使公平的进步对个人本身没有客观影响,只要对其社会群体当前和未来的成员有利,就会出现公平修正主义。然而,公平修正主义并不是针对不属于该群体的边缘化群体的利益而产生的,当个人从局外人的角度评估其他群体过去经历的公平性时,也不会产生公平修正主义。总体而言,这种现象可能是一把双刃剑:它可以通过消除对不利经历的记忆,让那些受到不公平待遇的人感到安心,但也可能使社会中的歧视问题从受害者自身的角度来看显得不那么紧迫。
{"title":"Fairness revisionism: Reducing discrimination for the future reduces perceived unfairness in the past","authors":"Tito L.H. Grillo ,&nbsp;Shuhan Yang ,&nbsp;Adrian F. Ward","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104671","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104671","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Marginalized groups may face systemic discrimination for generations until concrete advancements in society finally ensure fairer treatment for their members. Although fairness advancements may benefit these groups in the present and future, they do not change the past; they cannot undo the discrimination already experienced by previous generations. However, five studies (<em>N</em> = 1672) suggest that fairness advancements that benefit a marginalized group may change how its members perceive their own prior experiences with discrimination, leading them to see these experiences as having been fairer compared to when there are no such advancements. We find evidence of this revisionism of unfair past experiences in different historically marginalized groups (women and immigrants) and cultural contexts (U.S., U.K., and China). Critically, fairness revisionism arises even when fairness advancements have no objective impact on individuals themselves, as long as there are benefits for current and future members of their social group. Fairness revisionism does not arise, however, in response to gains for marginalized groups to which one does not belong, nor when individuals assess fairness in other groups' past experiences from an outsider's perspective. Overall, this phenomenon may be a double-edged sword: it may provide peace of mind for those treated unfairly by assuaging the memory of adverse experiences, but may also make discrimination issues in society seem less pressing based on the perspective of victims themselves.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104671"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000842/pdfft?md5=ce114b4e5b6fac58c6e7e545293abd21&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000842-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142096565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Race in the eye of the beholder: Decomposing perceiver- and target-level variation in perceived racial prototypicality 观察者眼中的种族:分解感知种族原型性中感知者和目标层面的差异
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104667
Jasmine B. Norman , Daphne Castro Lingl , Eric Hehman , Jacqueline M. Chen

Perceivers' ability to use multiple sources of information when forming impressions—including top-down, perceiver-level features, and bottom-up, target-level features—is a hallmark of social cognition. We investigate this primary foundation by examining the role of perceiver-level and target-level variation in perceived racial prototypicality in the U.S. In Study 1 (200 unique faces; 2608 raters), we quantified contributions of perceiver- and target-level effects to perceived racial prototypicality. Perceiver- and target-level contributions varied across racial category (Asian, Black, Latine, Middle Eastern, and Multiracial), with Multiracial and Middle Eastern prototypicality being more perceiver-driven. Although several appearance features (e.g., perceived ambiguity, skin tone) related to perceived prototypicality, there were distinctions in how perceivers used them (e.g., some people strongly used skin tone to infer Black prototypicality, while others used this less or not at all). A second study (N = 511) experimentally manipulated race essentialist beliefs. While there was no impact on perceived racial prototypicality, regardless of the category (Asian, Black, Latine, Middle Eastern, Multiracial, and Native American), Middle Eastern, Multiracial, and Native American prototypicality were generally more perceiver-driven than other categories, converging with Study 1. Further, perceivers' social dominance orientation, but not several other individual differences, were associated with less use of each of these categories. Taken together, findings suggest perceived racial prototypicality may originate less from stable individual differences like attitudes and instead reflects both i) differences in perceptions of target features and ii) differences in how people use particular target features in making racial prototypicality judgments.

感知者在形成印象时使用多种信息源的能力--包括自上而下的、感知者层面的特征和自下而上的、目标层面的特征--是社会认知的一个标志。在研究 1(200 个独特的面孔;2608 个评分者)中,我们量化了感知者和目标水平对感知种族原型的影响。感知者和目标水平对不同种族类别(亚洲人、黑人、拉丁人、中东人和多种族人)的贡献各不相同,其中多种族人和中东人的原型性更多地由感知者驱动。虽然一些外观特征(如感知模糊性、肤色)与感知原型性有关,但感知者在如何使用这些特征方面存在差异(如一些人强烈使用肤色来推断黑人原型性,而其他人则较少使用或根本不使用)。第二项研究(N = 511)通过实验操纵了种族本质主义信念。虽然对感知到的种族原型性没有影响,但不管是什么类别(亚洲人、黑人、拉丁人、中东人、多种族人和美国本土人),中东人、多种族人和美国本土人的原型性一般比其他类别更受感知者的驱动,这与研究 1 一致。此外,感知者的社会优势取向(而非其他一些个体差异)与较少使用这些类别有关。综上所述,研究结果表明,感知到的种族原型性可能较少来源于稳定的个体差异(如态度),而是反映了 i) 对目标特征的感知差异和 ii) 人们在进行种族原型性判断时如何使用特定目标特征的差异。
{"title":"Race in the eye of the beholder: Decomposing perceiver- and target-level variation in perceived racial prototypicality","authors":"Jasmine B. Norman ,&nbsp;Daphne Castro Lingl ,&nbsp;Eric Hehman ,&nbsp;Jacqueline M. Chen","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104667","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104667","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Perceivers' ability to use multiple sources of information when forming impressions—including top-down, perceiver-level features, and bottom-up, target-level features—is a hallmark of social cognition. We investigate this primary foundation by examining the role of perceiver-level and target-level variation in perceived racial prototypicality in the U.S. In Study 1 (200 unique faces; 2608 raters), we quantified contributions of perceiver- and target-level effects to perceived racial prototypicality. Perceiver- and target-level contributions varied across racial category (Asian, Black, Latine, Middle Eastern, and Multiracial), with Multiracial and Middle Eastern prototypicality being more perceiver-driven. Although several appearance features (e.g., perceived ambiguity, skin tone) related to perceived prototypicality, there were distinctions in how perceivers used them (e.g., some people strongly used skin tone to infer Black prototypicality, while others used this less or not at all). A second study (<em>N</em> = 511) experimentally manipulated race essentialist beliefs. While there was no impact on perceived racial prototypicality, regardless of the category (Asian, Black, Latine, Middle Eastern, Multiracial, and Native American), Middle Eastern, Multiracial, and Native American prototypicality were generally more perceiver-driven than other categories, converging with Study 1. Further, perceivers' social dominance orientation, but not several other individual differences, were associated with less use of each of these categories. Taken together, findings suggest perceived racial prototypicality may originate less from stable individual differences like attitudes and instead reflects both i) differences in perceptions of target features and ii) differences in how people use particular target features in making racial prototypicality judgments.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104667"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142089336","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exemplar-based ingroup projection: The superordinate national category is associated more strongly with ingroup than outgroup political leaders 基于榜样的内群投射:上位国家类别与内群政治领导人的联系比与外群政治领导人的联系更紧密
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104669
Adi Amit , Ido Liviatan , Sari Mentser , Eitan Venzhik , Yuval Karmel , Tal Moran

We studied mental representations of social categories in the context of political groups nested within national identities. Extending previous works derived from the Ingroup Projection Model, which had investigated category representations based on prototypical attributes, we examined category representations based on prototypical exemplars, focusing on group leaders. We hypothesized that the mental representation of the superordinate, national, category is more strongly associated with the mental representation of ingroup political leaders than outgroup political leaders. We tested our hypothesis in three preregistered experiments, looking at two different national-political contexts and using diverse methods. In Studies 1 (N = 145) and 2 (N = 103), both conducted in Israel, we found that participants explicitly associated the national category with leaders of their own political camp more than with leaders of the rival political camp. In Study 2, we further found that participants were more likely to falsely remember that ingroup relative to outgroup political leaders were paired with the national category (versus their political wing). In Study 3 (N = 381), conducted in the USA, we found using an implicit measure of association (ST-IAT) that Democrats and Republicans sorted stimuli, representing political leaders, faster when the national category (represented by American symbols) was paired with the ingroup rather than the outgroup category. Implications for theoretical accounts of ingroup projection, as well as for the understanding of political polarization and intergroup leadership, are discussed.

我们研究了嵌套在民族身份中的政治团体对社会类别的心理表征。之前的研究是基于原型属性的类别表征,我们在此基础上研究了基于原型范例的类别表征,重点是群体领袖。我们假设,与外群体政治领袖相比,上位者(国家)类别的心理表征与内群体政治领袖的心理表征有更强的关联。我们在三个预先登记的实验中测试了我们的假设,研究了两种不同的国家政治背景,并使用了不同的方法。研究 1(145 人)和研究 2(103 人)都是在以色列进行的,在这两项研究中,我们发现参与者更明确地将国家类别与自己政治阵营的领导人联系在一起,而不是与敌对政治阵营的领导人联系在一起。在研究 2 中,我们进一步发现,相对于外群体的政治领袖,参与者更容易错误地记住内群体的政治领袖与国家类别(相对于其政治派别)配对。在美国进行的研究 3(N = 381)中,我们使用内隐联想测量法(ST-IAT)发现,当国家类别(以美国符号为代表)与内群类别而非外群类别配对时,民主党人和共和党人会更快地对代表政治领袖的刺激进行分类。本文讨论了内群体投射理论的意义,以及对政治极化和群体间领导力的理解。
{"title":"Exemplar-based ingroup projection: The superordinate national category is associated more strongly with ingroup than outgroup political leaders","authors":"Adi Amit ,&nbsp;Ido Liviatan ,&nbsp;Sari Mentser ,&nbsp;Eitan Venzhik ,&nbsp;Yuval Karmel ,&nbsp;Tal Moran","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104669","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104669","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We studied mental representations of social categories in the context of political groups nested within national identities. Extending previous works derived from the Ingroup Projection Model, which had investigated category representations based on prototypical <em>attributes</em>, we examined category representations based on prototypical <em>exemplars</em>, focusing on group leaders. We hypothesized that the mental representation of the superordinate, national, category is more strongly associated with the mental representation of ingroup political leaders than outgroup political leaders. We tested our hypothesis in three preregistered experiments, looking at two different national-political contexts and using diverse methods. In Studies 1 (<em>N</em> = 145) and 2 (<em>N</em> = 103), both conducted in Israel, we found that participants explicitly associated the national category with leaders of their own political camp more than with leaders of the rival political camp. In Study 2, we further found that participants were more likely to falsely remember that ingroup relative to outgroup political leaders were paired with the national category (versus their political wing). In Study 3 (<em>N</em> = 381), conducted in the USA, we found using an implicit measure of association (ST-IAT) that Democrats and Republicans sorted stimuli, representing political leaders, faster when the national category (represented by American symbols) was paired with the ingroup rather than the outgroup category. Implications for theoretical accounts of ingroup projection, as well as for the understanding of political polarization and intergroup leadership, are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104669"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142075787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intergroup bias in perceived trustworthiness among few or many minimal groups 少数或多数最小群体在感知可信度方面的群体间偏差
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104668
Johanna Woitzel, Moritz Ingendahl, Hans Alves

In diversifying societies, people are inevitably exposed to an increasing number of outgroups. As impressions of outgroups are more negative than those of ingroups, this may overall lead to more negative social attitudes and behaviors. In six preregistered experiments (Ntotal = 1832) using a minimal group paradigm, we investigated whether the mere number of groups influences the perceived trustworthiness of ingroups, outgroups, and the total population. Our results consistently show that higher diversity does not decrease overall population trustworthiness, despite a larger number of outgroups. This is because of a stronger intergroup bias such that ingroups receive an additional boost in trustworthiness judgments when there are more outgroups. Our experiments show that these effects are not due to objective or perceived group sizes or greater attitude differences toward the group-defining attributes. Instead, people seem to perceive members of their ingroup as more similar to themselves if there is a higher number of outgroups and high similarity is related to high perceived trustworthiness.

在多元化社会中,人们不可避免地会接触到越来越多的外群体。由于人们对外部群体的印象比对内部群体的印象更负面,这可能会在整体上导致更消极的社会态度和行为。在六个预先登记的实验中(总人数 = 1832),我们采用了最小群体范式,研究了群体数量是否会影响内群体、外群体和总人口的可信度。我们的研究结果一致表明,尽管外群体数量较多,但较高的多样性并不会降低整体人口的可信度。这是因为群体间的偏差更强,当外群体数量更多的时候,内群体的可信度判断会得到额外的提升。我们的实验表明,这些效应并不是由于客观的或感知的群体规模,也不是由于对群体定义属性的态度差异。相反,如果有更多的外群体,人们似乎会认为内群体成员与自己更相似,而高相似度与高感知可信度相关。
{"title":"Intergroup bias in perceived trustworthiness among few or many minimal groups","authors":"Johanna Woitzel,&nbsp;Moritz Ingendahl,&nbsp;Hans Alves","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104668","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104668","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In diversifying societies, people are inevitably exposed to an increasing number of outgroups. As impressions of outgroups are more negative than those of ingroups, this may overall lead to more negative social attitudes and behaviors. In six preregistered experiments (<em>N</em><sub><em>total</em></sub> = 1832) using a minimal group paradigm, we investigated whether the mere number of groups influences the perceived trustworthiness of ingroups, outgroups, and the total population. Our results consistently show that higher diversity does not decrease overall population trustworthiness, despite a larger number of outgroups. This is because of a stronger intergroup bias such that ingroups receive an additional boost in trustworthiness judgments when there are more outgroups. Our experiments show that these effects are not due to objective or perceived group sizes or greater attitude differences toward the group-defining attributes. Instead, people seem to perceive members of their ingroup as more similar to themselves if there is a higher number of outgroups and high similarity is related to high perceived trustworthiness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104668"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000817/pdfft?md5=956868db8eb3972e99d4d4f2eab40564&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000817-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142083066","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of fear appeals on reactance in climate change communication 恐惧诉求对气候变化传播中反应的影响
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104666
Laura Bilfinger, Benjamin Brummernhenrich, Regina Jucks

Addressing the existential threat posed by climate change requires urgent actions, both on an individual level and on a policy level. In the present research, we applied an emotion-based persuasion appeal model to climate change mitigation to test the effect of climate mitigation appeals formulated with different levels of threat (high vs. low) and appealing to different types of climate change solutions (individual vs. policy) in eliciting psychological reactance, motivating support for climate change mitigation, and influencing willingness to discuss the issue with others. Through an online between-subjects experiment, we found that appealing to individuals (as opposed to policy) increased individuals' perceived threat to freedom, and psychological reactance. Our threat manipulation increased levels of fear, but the interaction between level of threat and type of appeal was not statistically significant on any of our dependent variables. Results are discussed in light of the active debate regarding the effectiveness of fear appeals in the climate change communication context.

应对气候变化带来的生存威胁需要个人和政策层面的紧急行动。在本研究中,我们将基于情感的说服诉求模型应用于气候变化减缓,以检验不同威胁程度(高与低)和不同气候变化解决方案类型(个人与政策)的气候减缓诉求在激发心理反应、激励支持气候变化减缓以及影响与他人讨论该问题的意愿方面的效果。通过在线主体间实验,我们发现,对个人(相对于政策)的呼吁增加了个人对自由的感知威胁和心理反应。我们的威胁操纵增加了恐惧水平,但威胁水平和呼吁类型之间的交互作用对我们的任何因变量都没有统计学意义。本文结合气候变化传播背景下有关恐惧呼吁有效性的激烈辩论对结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"The effects of fear appeals on reactance in climate change communication","authors":"Laura Bilfinger,&nbsp;Benjamin Brummernhenrich,&nbsp;Regina Jucks","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104666","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104666","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Addressing the existential threat posed by climate change requires urgent actions, both on an individual level and on a policy level. In the present research, we applied an emotion-based persuasion appeal model to climate change mitigation to test the effect of climate mitigation appeals formulated with different levels of threat (high vs. low) and appealing to different types of climate change solutions (individual vs. policy) in eliciting psychological reactance, motivating support for climate change mitigation, and influencing willingness to discuss the issue with others. Through an online between-subjects experiment, we found that appealing to individuals (as opposed to policy) increased individuals' perceived threat to freedom, and psychological reactance. Our threat manipulation increased levels of fear, but the interaction between level of threat and type of appeal was not statistically significant on any of our dependent variables. Results are discussed in light of the active debate regarding the effectiveness of fear appeals in the climate change communication context.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104666"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000799/pdfft?md5=377758af974dc22ff982d11825b7fcd5&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000799-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142040405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dissociations between animalistic and mechanistic dehumanization in the context of labor exploitation 劳动剥削背景下动物性非人化与机械性非人化之间的分离
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104665
Matthew L. Stanley , Aaron C. Kay

Across eight studies (and two additional supplemental studies), we investigate possible bidirectional causal links between dehumanization and exploitation (total N = 5923). Participants were less opposed to the exploitation of mechanistically dehumanized workers – i.e., workers perceived to lack traits central to human nature like emotionality and warmth – than other workers (Studies 1–5). The effects of mechanistic dehumanization on exploitation judgments were statistically explained by perceptions that mechanistically dehumanized workers are more capable of enduring experiences that typically elicit suffering and hardship (Studies 2–4). We also found evidence against several other possible explanations for the effects of mechanistic dehumanization on exploitation judgments (i.e., competence, dependability, likeability; Studies 2–4). In addition, we found consistent evidence for the reverse causal pathway: Workers who are exploited – relative to workers who are not exploited – are more likely to be attributed qualities indicative of mechanistic dehumanization (Studies 6–8; Supplemental Studies 1–2). The effects across studies were invariant to job type, the perceived race/ethnicity and gender of the target worker, and the specific case of exploitation. In addition, the relationships between dehumanization and exploitation judgments were specific to the mechanistic form of dehumanization and not the animalistic form – i.e., perceiving others as lacking traits that distinguish humans from animals like self-control, rationality, civility. These bidirectional causal relations between mechanistic dehumanization and exploitation have the potential to create a vicious cycle of suffering and unfair treatment for certain workers.

通过八项研究(以及另外两项补充研究),我们调查了非人化与剥削之间可能存在的双向因果关系(总计 = 5923)。与其他工人相比,受试者不太反对剥削机械非人化工人,即被认为缺乏情感和温暖等人性核心特征的工人(研究 1-5)。机械非人化对剥削判断的影响在统计学上可以用以下观点来解释:机械非人化的工人更有能力忍受通常会引起痛苦和困难的经历(研究 2-4)。我们还发现有证据表明,机械非人性化对剥削判断影响的其他几种可能解释(即能力、可依赖性、可亲近性;研究 2-4)是不成立的。此外,我们还发现了反向因果关系的一致证据:与未受剥削的工人相比,受剥削的工人更有可能被认为具有机械非人化的特质(研究6-8;补充研究1-2)。各研究的效果与工作类型、目标工人的种族/民族和性别以及具体的剥削情况无关。此外,非人化与剥削判断之间的关系只针对机械形式的非人化,而非动物形式的非人化,即认为他人缺乏自我控制、理性、文明等区别于动物的特征。机械非人化与剥削之间的这种双向因果关系,有可能造成某些工人遭受痛苦和不公平待遇的恶性循环。
{"title":"Dissociations between animalistic and mechanistic dehumanization in the context of labor exploitation","authors":"Matthew L. Stanley ,&nbsp;Aaron C. Kay","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104665","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104665","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Across eight studies (and two additional supplemental studies), we investigate possible bidirectional causal links between dehumanization and exploitation (total <em>N</em> = 5923). Participants were less opposed to the exploitation of mechanistically dehumanized workers – i.e., workers perceived to lack traits central to human nature like emotionality and warmth – than other workers (Studies 1–5). The effects of mechanistic dehumanization on exploitation judgments were statistically explained by perceptions that mechanistically dehumanized workers are more capable of enduring experiences that typically elicit suffering and hardship (Studies 2–4). We also found evidence against several other possible explanations for the effects of mechanistic dehumanization on exploitation judgments (i.e., competence, dependability, likeability; Studies 2–4). In addition, we found consistent evidence for the reverse causal pathway: Workers who are exploited – relative to workers who are not exploited – are more likely to be attributed qualities indicative of mechanistic dehumanization (Studies 6–8; Supplemental Studies 1–2). The effects across studies were invariant to job type, the perceived race/ethnicity and gender of the target worker, and the specific case of exploitation. In addition, the relationships between dehumanization and exploitation judgments were specific to the mechanistic form of dehumanization and not the animalistic form – i.e., perceiving others as lacking traits that distinguish humans from animals like self-control, rationality, civility. These bidirectional causal relations between mechanistic dehumanization and exploitation have the potential to create a vicious cycle of suffering and unfair treatment for certain workers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104665"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141915314","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral decay in investment 投资中的道德败坏
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104664
Paweł Niszczota , Paul Conway , Michał Białek

How strongly do higher investment premiums tempt people to invest in unethical assets, such as harmful ‘sin stocks’? We present two experimental studies (Ntotal = 1260) examining baseline willingness to invest in ‘sin stocks’ (without a premium), changes in investments as premiums increase, and how individual differences in deontological and utilitarian inclinations and dark personality traits impact baseline and changes to investments. We compare results to hypothetical models of sensitivity to higher returns: a) full resilience (to moral decay), where people increase investment in regular but not sin stocks with increasing premiums, b) partial resilience, where increasing premiums increases investment more slowly for sin than regular stocks, c) sin deduction: a flat baseline penalty for sin versus regular stocks resulting in similar sensitivity to increasing premiums, and d) decay, where investment differences in sin versus regular stocks reduce as premiums increase. On average, responses aligned best with the partial resilience model. Individual differences in morally-relevant traits moderated effects: most notably, people with higher deontological inclinations and lower dark traits showed greater resilience. However, 21–33% of participants exhibited full resilience, refusing to invest more in sin stocks even as premiums increased, which was more common in people with higher deontological inclinations and lower dark traits. These findings suggest that decisions to invest in sin stocks reflect the sensitivity to the sinfulness of the stock, which remains strong even after unethical investments are made more attractive. We conclude that increasing the economic reward of unethical investments does not crowd out moral concerns.

较高的投资溢价对人们投资有害的 "罪恶股票 "等不道德资产的诱惑力有多大?我们介绍了两项实验研究(= 1260),分别考察了投资 "罪恶股票"(无溢价)的基准意愿、随着溢价增加投资的变化,以及个体在去道德主义和功利主义倾向上的差异和黑暗人格特质如何影响投资的基准和变化。我们将结果与对高回报敏感度的假设模型进行了比较:a) 完全弹性(对道德败坏),即随着保费的增加,人们会增加对正常股票的投资,但不会增加对罪恶股票的投资;b) 部分弹性,即保费的增加对罪恶股票投资的增加比对正常股票投资的增加更慢;c) 罪恶扣减:对罪恶股票与正常股票的基线惩罚相同,导致对保费增加的敏感度相似;d) 衰减,即随着保费的增加,罪恶股票与正常股票的投资差异减小。平均而言,对部分复原模型的反应最为一致。道德相关特质的个体差异调节了效果:最明显的是,道德倾向较高和黑暗特质较低的人表现出更强的复原力。然而,21%-33% 的参与者表现出了完全的抗逆性,即使在溢价增加的情况下也拒绝投资更多的罪恶股票,这种情况在道德倾向较高和阴暗特质较低的人中更为常见。这些发现表明,投资罪恶股票的决定反映了对股票罪恶性的敏感度,即使在不道德的投资变得更具吸引力之后,这种敏感度仍然很强。我们的结论是,增加不道德投资的经济回报并不会挤掉道德关注。
{"title":"Moral decay in investment","authors":"Paweł Niszczota ,&nbsp;Paul Conway ,&nbsp;Michał Białek","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104664","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104664","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How strongly do higher investment premiums tempt people to invest in unethical assets, such as harmful ‘sin stocks’? We present two experimental studies (<em>N</em><sub>total</sub> = 1260) examining baseline willingness to invest in ‘sin stocks’ (without a premium), changes in investments as premiums increase, and how individual differences in deontological and utilitarian inclinations and dark personality traits impact baseline and changes to investments. We compare results to hypothetical models of sensitivity to higher returns: a) full resilience (to moral decay), where people increase investment in regular but not sin stocks with increasing premiums, b) partial resilience, where increasing premiums increases investment more slowly for sin than regular stocks, c) sin deduction: a flat baseline penalty for sin versus regular stocks resulting in similar sensitivity to increasing premiums, and d) decay, where investment differences in sin versus regular stocks reduce as premiums increase. On average, responses aligned best with the partial resilience model. Individual differences in morally-relevant traits moderated effects: most notably, people with higher deontological inclinations and lower dark traits showed greater resilience. However, 21–33% of participants exhibited full resilience, refusing to invest more in sin stocks even as premiums increased, which was more common in people with higher deontological inclinations and lower dark traits. These findings suggest that decisions to invest in sin stocks reflect the sensitivity to the sinfulness of the stock, which remains strong even after unethical investments are made more attractive. We conclude that increasing the economic reward of unethical investments does not crowd out moral concerns.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104664"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141915315","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Scientific identity and STEMM-relevant outcomes: Elaboration moderates use of identity-certainty 科学认同与 STEMM 相关成果:阐述对身份确定性使用的调节作用
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104663
Lorena Moreno , Pablo Briñol , Borja Paredes , Richard E. Petty

This research investigates the link between scientific identity and STEMM (Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics, and Medicine)-related outcomes as a function of identity certainty. Across a pilot study and three additional studies, participants' scientific identity was first measured using different procedures. Then, the certainty with which that identity was held was either measured (pilot study and Studies 1 & 2) or manipulated (Study 3). Both subjective outcomes (e.g., interest and career decisions in the pilot study and Study 2) and objective consequences (e.g., performance in Studies 1 and 3) served as dependent variables. As expected, results showed that participants' scientific identity was more strongly associated with STEMM-relevant outcomes as certainty in that identity increased. Beyond predicting when and for whom scientific identity is more likely to guide career decisions and performance, this research showed that reliance on identity certainty (a metacognitive assessment) is more likely to occur as the extent of thinking is increased. By inducing elaboration after certainty was already manipulated (Study 3), this research distinguishes between forming a metacognitive judgment of certainty and subsequently using that already existing certainty.

本研究调查了科学认同与 STEMM(科学、技术、工程、数学和医学)相关结果之间的联系,并以此作为认同确定性的函数。在一项试点研究和另外三项研究中,首先使用不同的程序测量参与者的科学认同。然后,测量(试验研究、研究 1 和研究 2)或操纵(研究 3)参与者对科学身份的确定性。主观结果(如试验研究和研究 2 中的兴趣和职业决定)和客观后果(如研究 1 和研究 3 中的成绩)都是因变量。不出所料,研究结果表明,随着参与者科学认同感的增强,其科学认同与 STEMM 相关结果的关联度更高。除了预测科学身份更有可能在何时、对谁而言更有可能指导职业决策和表现之外,这项研究还表明,随着思考程度的提高,对身份确定性(一种元认知评估)的依赖更有可能发生。通过在确定性已被操纵后诱导阐述(研究 3),本研究区分了对确定性形成元认知判断和随后使用已存在的确定性。
{"title":"Scientific identity and STEMM-relevant outcomes: Elaboration moderates use of identity-certainty","authors":"Lorena Moreno ,&nbsp;Pablo Briñol ,&nbsp;Borja Paredes ,&nbsp;Richard E. Petty","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104663","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104663","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This research investigates the link between scientific identity and STEMM (Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics, and Medicine)-related outcomes as a function of identity certainty. Across a pilot study and three additional studies, participants' scientific identity was first measured using different procedures. Then, the certainty with which that identity was held was either measured (pilot study and Studies 1 &amp; 2) or manipulated (Study 3). Both subjective outcomes (e.g., interest and career decisions in the pilot study and Study 2) and objective consequences (e.g., performance in Studies 1 and 3) served as dependent variables. As expected, results showed that participants' scientific identity was more strongly associated with STEMM-relevant outcomes as certainty in that identity increased. Beyond predicting when and for whom scientific identity is more likely to guide career decisions and performance, this research showed that reliance on identity certainty (a metacognitive assessment) is more likely to occur as the extent of thinking is increased. By inducing elaboration after certainty was already manipulated (Study 3), this research distinguishes between forming a metacognitive judgment of certainty and subsequently using that already existing certainty.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104663"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000763/pdfft?md5=4ab3f786383215b8f38a008214131823&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000763-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141892112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing not to see: Visual inattention as a method of information avoidance 选择不看:视觉注意力不集中是一种回避信息的方法
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104661
Caroline Kjær Børsting , Aleksandr Batuev , Shaul Shalvi , Jacob Lund Orquin

People rely on a number of methods to avoid information that would compel them to change their beliefs or behaviors. However, it remains unclear whether people use visual inattention as a method of information avoidance. In three eye-tracking experiments, we test the hypothesis that people avoid visual information by strategically suppressing and facilitating visual attention depending on where desired and avoided information is likely to appear. Introducing a novel search task, we independently manipulate the probability of where desired and avoided information appear on the screen. Study 1 show that participants learn statistical regularities in information location and utilize this to gradually suppress attention to undesired information. Study 2 and 3 show that participants can simultaneously reduce and increase visual attention to areas where avoided and desired information are most likely to appear. The findings point to suppression of attention as a mechanism behind information avoidance through visual inattention and that reducing the predictability of where information appears could be a fruitful avenue for reducing it.

人们会通过多种方法来回避那些会迫使他们改变信念或行为的信息。然而,人们是否将视觉注意力不集中作为一种回避信息的方法,目前仍不清楚。在三项眼动追踪实验中,我们验证了这样一个假设:人们会根据所需和所回避信息可能出现的位置,有策略地抑制和促进视觉注意力,从而回避视觉信息。我们引入了一个新颖的搜索任务,独立地操纵屏幕上出现所需信息和回避信息的概率。研究 1 表明,参与者可以学习信息位置的统计规律性,并利用这种规律性逐渐抑制对不想要的信息的注意。研究 2 和研究 3 表明,参与者可以同时减少和增加对最有可能出现回避信息和期望信息的区域的视觉注意力。研究结果表明,注意力抑制是通过视觉不集中来回避信息的一种机制,而降低信息出现位置的可预测性可能是减少注意力抑制的一个有效途径。
{"title":"Choosing not to see: Visual inattention as a method of information avoidance","authors":"Caroline Kjær Børsting ,&nbsp;Aleksandr Batuev ,&nbsp;Shaul Shalvi ,&nbsp;Jacob Lund Orquin","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104661","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104661","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People rely on a number of methods to avoid information that would compel them to change their beliefs or behaviors. However, it remains unclear whether people use visual inattention as a method of information avoidance. In three eye-tracking experiments, we test the hypothesis that people avoid visual information by strategically suppressing and facilitating visual attention depending on where desired and avoided information is likely to appear. Introducing a novel search task, we independently manipulate the probability of where desired and avoided information appear on the screen. Study 1 show that participants learn statistical regularities in information location and utilize this to gradually suppress attention to undesired information. Study 2 and 3 show that participants can simultaneously reduce and increase visual attention to areas where avoided and desired information are most likely to appear. The findings point to suppression of attention as a mechanism behind information avoidance through visual inattention and that reducing the predictability of where information appears could be a fruitful avenue for reducing it.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"115 ","pages":"Article 104661"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141638916","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1