首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Are ethical explanations explanatory? Meta-ethical beliefs shape judgments about explanations for social change 伦理解释是解释性的吗?元伦理信念决定了对社会变革解释的判断。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105860
Casey Lewry , George Tsai , Tania Lombrozo

Why were women given the right to vote? “Because it is morally wrong to deny women the right to vote.” This explanation does not seem to fit the typical pattern for explaining an event: rather than citing a cause, it appeals to an ethical claim. Do people judge ethical claims to be genuinely explanatory? And if so, why? In Studies 1 (N = 220) and 2 (N = 293), we find that many participants accept ethical explanations for social change and that this is predicted by their meta-ethical beliefs in moral progress and moral principles, suggesting that these participants treat morality as a directional feature of the world, somewhat akin to a causal force. In Studies 3 (N = 513) and 4 (N = 328), we find that participants recognize this relationship between ethical explanations and meta-ethical commitments, using the former to make inferences about individuals' beliefs in moral progress and moral principles. Together these studies demonstrate that our beliefs about the nature of morality shape our judgments of explanations and that explanations shape our inferences about others' moral commitments.

为什么赋予妇女选举权?"因为剥夺妇女的选举权在道德上是错误的。这一解释似乎并不符合解释事件的典型模式:它不是援引原因,而是诉诸道德主张。人们会判断道德主张是真正的解释吗?如果是,为什么?在研究 1(N = 220)和研究 2(N = 293)中,我们发现许多参与者接受对社会变革的伦理解释,而这是由他们对道德进步和道德原则的元伦理信念所预测的,这表明这些参与者将道德视为世界的一个方向性特征,有点类似于一种因果力量。在研究 3(N = 513)和研究 4(N = 328)中,我们发现参与者认识到了道德解释和元道德承诺之间的这种关系,并利用前者来推断个人对道德进步和道德原则的信念。这些研究共同表明,我们对道德本质的信念影响着我们对解释的判断,而解释又影响着我们对他人道德承诺的推断。
{"title":"Are ethical explanations explanatory? Meta-ethical beliefs shape judgments about explanations for social change","authors":"Casey Lewry ,&nbsp;George Tsai ,&nbsp;Tania Lombrozo","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105860","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105860","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Why were women given the right to vote? “Because it is morally wrong to deny women the right to vote.” This explanation does not seem to fit the typical pattern for explaining an event: rather than citing a cause, it appeals to an ethical claim. Do people judge ethical claims to be genuinely explanatory? And if so, why? In Studies 1 (<em>N</em> = 220) and 2 (<em>N</em> = 293), we find that many participants accept ethical explanations for social change and that this is predicted by their meta-ethical beliefs in moral progress and moral principles, suggesting that these participants treat morality as a directional feature of the world, somewhat akin to a causal force. In Studies 3 (<em>N</em> = 513) and 4 (<em>N</em> = 328), we find that participants recognize this relationship between ethical explanations and meta-ethical commitments, using the former to make inferences about individuals' beliefs in moral progress and moral principles. Together these studies demonstrate that our beliefs about the nature of morality shape our judgments of explanations and that explanations shape our inferences about others' moral commitments.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471642","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On second thoughts: Testing the underlying mechanisms of spontaneous future thought 二次思考:测试自发未来思维的内在机制。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105863
J. Helgi Clayton McClure, Charlotte Elwell, Theo Jones, Jelena Mirković, Scott N. Cole

The human capacity to imagine possible future events unintentionally, with minimal cognitive effort, is termed spontaneous future thought (SFT). This paper addresses an important theoretical question for cognitive science: What are the possible cognitive mechanisms underlying such SFT experiences? We contrasted three hypotheses present in the literature: the online construction hypothesis, the recasting hypothesis, and the memories of future thoughts hypothesis. Study 1 (N = 41) used novel subjective ratings which challenged the recasting mechanism: SFTs were mostly rated as dissimilar to autobiographical memories, suggesting they are not simply past experiences ‘recast’ as future events. Study 2 (N = 90) used a novel experimental paradigm, comparing effects of voluntary episodic future constructions and non-personal narratives upon subsequent spontaneous thought sampling. Results suggested that voluntary future constructions remain accessible to spontaneous retrieval, supporting the memories of future thoughts hypothesis. This finding, and other data presented across the two studies, still indicates a role for online construction processes in SFT, but further empirical work is needed to clarify how and when constructive processes are engaged in SFT. Taken together, these two studies represent initial efforts to elucidate the mechanisms underlying SFT, providing the first proof-of-principle that deliberately envisioned future events can reappear, without intention, in consciousness at some later time, and further supporting the dual process account of future thinking. These methods and findings provide a firm basis for subsequent experimental and longitudinal research on SFT.

人类能够以最小的认知努力,无意中想象未来可能发生的事件,这种能力被称为自发未来思维(SFT)。本文探讨了认知科学的一个重要理论问题:这种自发未来思维体验背后可能存在哪些认知机制?我们对比了文献中的三种假说:在线构建假说、重铸假说和未来想法记忆假说。研究 1(N = 41)使用了新颖的主观评分,对重铸机制提出了质疑:大部分 SFTs 被评为与自传体记忆不相似,这表明它们并不是简单地将过去的经历 "重铸 "为未来事件。研究 2(N = 90)采用了一种新颖的实验范式,比较了自愿的偶发未来建构和非个人叙述对后续自发思维取样的影响。结果表明,自发的未来结构仍然可以被自发检索到,这支持了未来想法记忆的假设。这一发现以及这两项研究中的其他数据仍然表明在线建构过程在自发思维取样中的作用,但还需要进一步的实证工作来澄清建构过程在自发思维取样中是如何以及何时参与的。综合来看,这两项研究代表了人们为阐明 SFT 的内在机制所做的初步努力,首次证明了刻意设想的未来事件可以在无意中于稍后的某个时间重新出现在意识中,并进一步支持了未来思维的双重过程论。这些方法和发现为后续关于未来思维的实验和纵向研究奠定了坚实的基础。
{"title":"On second thoughts: Testing the underlying mechanisms of spontaneous future thought","authors":"J. Helgi Clayton McClure,&nbsp;Charlotte Elwell,&nbsp;Theo Jones,&nbsp;Jelena Mirković,&nbsp;Scott N. Cole","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105863","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105863","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The human capacity to imagine possible future events unintentionally, with minimal cognitive effort, is termed spontaneous future thought (SFT). This paper addresses an important theoretical question for cognitive science: What are the possible cognitive mechanisms underlying such SFT experiences? We contrasted three hypotheses present in the literature: the <em>online construction hypothesis</em>, the <em>recasting hypothesis</em>, and the <em>memories of future thoughts hypothesis</em>. Study 1 (<em>N</em> = 41) used novel subjective ratings which challenged the recasting mechanism: SFTs were mostly rated as dissimilar to autobiographical memories, suggesting they are not simply past experiences ‘recast’ as future events. Study 2 (<em>N</em> = 90) used a novel experimental paradigm, comparing effects of voluntary episodic future constructions and non-personal narratives upon subsequent spontaneous thought sampling. Results suggested that voluntary future constructions remain accessible to spontaneous retrieval, supporting the memories of future thoughts hypothesis. This finding, and other data presented across the two studies, still indicates a role for online construction processes in SFT, but further empirical work is needed to clarify how and when constructive processes are engaged in SFT. Taken together, these two studies represent initial efforts to elucidate the mechanisms underlying SFT, providing the first proof-of-principle that deliberately envisioned future events can reappear, without intention, in consciousness at some later time, and further supporting the dual process account of future thinking. These methods and findings provide a firm basis for subsequent experimental and longitudinal research on SFT.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001495/pdfft?md5=31f5383d4088d618cdc2bb6c70944fa8&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001495-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141459997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face recognition's practical relevance: Social bonds, not social butterflies 人脸识别的实用性:社会纽带,而非社会蝴蝶
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105816
Laura M. Engfors , Jeremy Wilmer , Romina Palermo , Gilles E. Gignac , Laura T. Germine , Linda Jeffery

Research on individual differences in face recognition has provided important foundational insights: their broad range, cognitive specificity, strong heritability, and resilience to change. Elusive, however, has been the key issue of practical relevance: do these individual differences correlate with aspects of life that go beyond the recognition of faces, per se? Though often assumed, especially in social realms, such correlates remain largely theoretical, without empirical support. Here, we investigate an array of potential social correlates of face recognition. We establish social relationship quality as a reproducible correlate. This link generalises across face recognition tasks and across independent samples. In contrast, we detect no robust association with the sheer quantity of social connections, whether measured directly via number of social contacts or indirectly via extraversion-related personality indices. These findings document the existence of a key social correlate of face recognition and provide some of the first evidence to support its practical relevance. At the same time, they challenge the naive assumption that face recognition relates equally to all social outcomes. In contrast, they suggest a focused link of face recognition to the quality, not quantity, of one's social connections.

关于人脸识别个体差异的研究已经提供了重要的基础性见解:这些差异范围广泛、具有认知特异性、遗传性强以及对变化的适应能力强。然而,具有实际意义的关键问题却一直悬而未决:这些个体差异是否与生活中超出人脸识别本身的其他方面相关联?尽管人们经常假设,特别是在社会领域,但这种相关性在很大程度上仍然是理论性的,没有经验支持。在这里,我们研究了人脸识别的一系列潜在社会相关因素。我们确定社会关系质量是一种可重复的相关因素。这种关联在不同的人脸识别任务和不同的独立样本中都具有普遍性。与此相反,我们并没有发现纯粹的社会关系数量与人脸识别的相关性,无论是直接通过社会联系人的数量还是间接通过与外向性相关的人格指数来衡量。这些发现证明了人脸识别存在一个关键的社会相关因素,并为其实际意义提供了一些初步证据。与此同时,这些研究还对人脸识别与所有社会结果具有同等关系的天真假设提出了质疑。相反,他们认为人脸识别与一个人的社会关系的质量而非数量有着密切的联系。
{"title":"Face recognition's practical relevance: Social bonds, not social butterflies","authors":"Laura M. Engfors ,&nbsp;Jeremy Wilmer ,&nbsp;Romina Palermo ,&nbsp;Gilles E. Gignac ,&nbsp;Laura T. Germine ,&nbsp;Linda Jeffery","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105816","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105816","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research on individual differences in face recognition has provided important foundational insights: their broad range, cognitive specificity, strong heritability, and resilience to change. Elusive, however, has been the key issue of practical relevance: do these individual differences correlate with aspects of life that go beyond the recognition of faces, per se? Though often assumed, especially in social realms, such correlates remain largely theoretical, without empirical support. Here, we investigate an array of potential social correlates of face recognition. We establish social relationship quality as a reproducible correlate. This link generalises across face recognition tasks and across independent samples. In contrast, we detect no robust association with the sheer quantity of social connections, whether measured directly via number of social contacts or indirectly via extraversion-related personality indices. These findings document the existence of a key social correlate of face recognition and provide some of the first evidence to support its practical relevance. At the same time, they challenge the naive assumption that face recognition relates equally to all social outcomes. In contrast, they suggest a focused link of face recognition to the quality, not quantity, of one's social connections.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001021/pdfft?md5=a204162951019096b794fb3aa9c80c21&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001021-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141438096","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When rules are over-ruled: Virtual bargaining as a contractualist method of moral judgment 当规则被推翻虚拟谈判作为一种契约主义道德判断方法
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105790
Sydney Levine , Max Kleiman-Weiner , Nick Chater , Fiery Cushman , Joshua B. Tenenbaum

Rules help guide our behavior—particularly in complex social contexts. But rules sometimes give us the “wrong” answer. How do we know when it is okay to break the rules? In this paper, we argue that we sometimes use contractualist (agreement-based) mechanisms to determine when a rule can be broken. Our model draws on a theory of social interactions – “virtual bargaining” – that assumes that actors engage in a simulated bargaining process when navigating the social world. We present experimental data which suggests that rule-breaking decisions are sometimes driven by virtual bargaining and show that these data cannot be explained by more traditional rule-based or outcome-based approaches.

规则有助于指导我们的行为,尤其是在复杂的社会环境中。但规则有时会给我们 "错误 "的答案。我们如何知道何时可以打破规则?在本文中,我们认为我们有时会使用契约主义(基于协议)机制来决定何时可以打破规则。我们的模型借鉴了一种社会互动理论--"虚拟讨价还价",该理论假定行为者在社会世界中游刃有余时,会参与一个模拟的讨价还价过程。我们提出的实验数据表明,打破规则的决定有时是由虚拟讨价还价驱动的,并表明这些数据无法用更传统的基于规则或基于结果的方法来解释。
{"title":"When rules are over-ruled: Virtual bargaining as a contractualist method of moral judgment","authors":"Sydney Levine ,&nbsp;Max Kleiman-Weiner ,&nbsp;Nick Chater ,&nbsp;Fiery Cushman ,&nbsp;Joshua B. Tenenbaum","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105790","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105790","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Rules help guide our behavior—particularly in complex social contexts. But rules sometimes give us the “wrong” answer. How do we know when it is okay to break the rules? In this paper, we argue that we sometimes use <em>contractualist</em> (agreement-based) mechanisms to determine when a rule can be broken. Our model draws on a theory of social interactions – “virtual bargaining” – that assumes that actors engage in a simulated bargaining process when navigating the social world. We present experimental data which suggests that rule-breaking decisions are sometimes driven by virtual bargaining and show that these data cannot be explained by more traditional rule-based or outcome-based approaches.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141438097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Clarifying the effects of sequential item presentation in the police lineup task 澄清警察列队任务中顺序项目呈现的影响
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105840
Matthew Kaesler , John C. Dunn , Carolyn Semmler

Previous research has reported diverging patterns of results with respect to discriminability and response bias when comparing the simultaneous lineup to two different lineup procedures in which items are presented sequentially, the sequential stopping rule lineup and the UK lineup. In a single large sample experiment, we compared discriminability and response bias in six-item photographic lineups presented either simultaneously, sequentially with a stopping rule, or sequentially requiring two full laps through the items before making an identification and including the ability to revisit items, analogous to the UK lineup procedure. Discriminability was greater for the simultaneous lineup compared to the sequential stopping rule lineup, despite a non-significant difference in empirical discriminability between the procedures. There was no significant difference in discriminability when comparing the simultaneous lineup to the sequential two lineup and the sequential two lap lineup to the sequential stopping rule lineup. Responding was most lenient for the sequential two lap lineup, followed by the simultaneous lineup, followed by the sequential lineup. These results imply that sequential item presentation may not exert a large effect in isolation on discriminability and response bias. Rather, discriminability and response bias in the sequential stopping rule lineup and UK lineup result from the interaction of sequential item presentation with other aspects of these procedures.

以往的研究报告显示,在比较同时列队和两种不同的列队程序(即顺序停止规则列队和英国式列队)时,在辨别力和反应偏差方面出现了不同的结果模式。在一次大样本实验中,我们比较了六项照相排序的可辨别性和反应偏差,这两种排序方式分别是:同时排序、顺序排序(有停止规则)或顺序排序(在进行辨别之前需要将项目看两遍),其中顺序排序还包括重新查看项目的能力,类似于英国的排序程序。与顺序停止规则排队相比,同时排队的辨别能力更强,尽管两种程序之间的经验辨别能力差异不大。同时列队与按顺序两两列队相比,以及按顺序两圈列队与按顺序停止规则列队相比,辨别能力没有明显差异。顺序两圈排列的反应最宽松,其次是同时排列,再次是顺序排列。这些结果表明,顺序项目的呈现可能不会单独对辨别力和反应偏差产生很大的影响。相反,顺序停止规则排队和英国式排队中的可辨别性和反应偏差是顺序项目呈现与这些程序的其他方面相互作用的结果。
{"title":"Clarifying the effects of sequential item presentation in the police lineup task","authors":"Matthew Kaesler ,&nbsp;John C. Dunn ,&nbsp;Carolyn Semmler","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105840","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105840","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has reported diverging patterns of results with respect to discriminability and response bias when comparing the simultaneous lineup to two different lineup procedures in which items are presented sequentially, the sequential stopping rule lineup and the UK lineup. In a single large sample experiment, we compared discriminability and response bias in six-item photographic lineups presented either simultaneously, sequentially with a stopping rule, or sequentially requiring two full laps through the items before making an identification and including the ability to revisit items, analogous to the UK lineup procedure. Discriminability was greater for the simultaneous lineup compared to the sequential stopping rule lineup, despite a non-significant difference in empirical discriminability between the procedures. There was no significant difference in discriminability when comparing the simultaneous lineup to the sequential two lineup and the sequential two lap lineup to the sequential stopping rule lineup. Responding was most lenient for the sequential two lap lineup, followed by the simultaneous lineup, followed by the sequential lineup. These results imply that sequential item presentation may not exert a large effect in isolation on discriminability and response bias. Rather, discriminability and response bias in the sequential stopping rule lineup and UK lineup result from the interaction of sequential item presentation with other aspects of these procedures.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141439141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A dissociation between the effects of expectations and attention in selective visual processing 在选择性视觉加工过程中,预期和注意力的影响相互分离
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105864
Alon Zivony , Martin Eimer

It is often claimed that probabilistic expectations affect visual perception directly, without mediation by selective attention. However, these claims have been disputed, as effects of expectation and attention are notoriously hard to dissociate experimentally. In this study, we used a new approach to separate expectations from attention. In four experiments (N = 60), participants searched for a target in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream and had to identify a digit or a letter defined by a low-level cue (colour or shape). Expectations about the target's alphanumeric category were probabilistically manipulated. Since category membership is a high-level feature and since the target was embedded among many distractors that shared its category, targets from the expected category should not attract attention more than targets from the unexpected category. In the first experiment, these targets were more likely to be identified relative to targets from the unexpected category. Importantly, in the following experiments, we also included behavioural and electrophysiological indices of attentional guidance and engagement. This allowed us to examine whether expectations also modulated these or earlier attentional processes. Results showed that category-based expectations had no modulatory effects on attention, and only affected processing at later encoding-related stages. Alternative interpretation of expectation effects in terms of repetition priming or response bias were also ruled out. These observations provide new evidence for direct attention-independent expectation effects on perception. We suggest that expectations can adjust the threshold required for encoding expectations-congruent information, thereby affecting the speed with which target objects are encoded in working memory.

人们经常声称,概率预期会直接影响视觉感知,而不会受到选择性注意的干扰。然而,这些说法一直存在争议,因为在实验中很难将期望和注意的影响区分开来。在本研究中,我们采用了一种新方法来分离期望和注意。在四个实验中(N = 60),参与者在快速序列视觉呈现(RSVP)流中寻找目标,并必须识别由低级线索(颜色或形状)定义的数字或字母。对目标的字母数字类别的预期会受到概率操纵。由于类别成员资格是一个高级特征,而且目标被嵌入到许多共享其类别的干扰物中,因此预期类别中的目标不应该比意外类别中的目标更能吸引注意力。在第一个实验中,这些目标相对于意外类别的目标更容易被识别。重要的是,在接下来的实验中,我们还加入了注意力引导和参与的行为和电生理指标。这样,我们就可以研究期望是否也会调节这些或更早的注意过程。结果表明,基于类别的期望对注意力没有调节作用,只影响编码相关的后期处理。我们还排除了用重复引物或反应偏差来解释期望效应的其他方法。这些观察结果为与注意无关的期望对知觉的直接影响提供了新的证据。我们认为,期望可以调整编码期望一致信息所需的阈值,从而影响目标对象在工作记忆中的编码速度。
{"title":"A dissociation between the effects of expectations and attention in selective visual processing","authors":"Alon Zivony ,&nbsp;Martin Eimer","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105864","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105864","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It is often claimed that probabilistic expectations affect visual perception directly, without mediation by selective attention. However, these claims have been disputed, as effects of expectation and attention are notoriously hard to dissociate experimentally. In this study, we used a new approach to separate expectations from attention. In four experiments (<em>N</em> = 60), participants searched for a target in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream and had to identify a digit or a letter defined by a low-level cue (colour or shape). Expectations about the target's alphanumeric category were probabilistically manipulated. Since category membership is a high-level feature and since the target was embedded among many distractors that shared its category, targets from the expected category should not attract attention more than targets from the unexpected category. In the first experiment, these targets were more likely to be identified relative to targets from the unexpected category. Importantly, in the following experiments, we also included behavioural and electrophysiological indices of attentional guidance and engagement. This allowed us to examine whether expectations also modulated these or earlier attentional processes. Results showed that category-based expectations had no modulatory effects on attention, and only affected processing at later encoding-related stages. Alternative interpretation of expectation effects in terms of repetition priming or response bias were also ruled out. These observations provide new evidence for direct attention-independent expectation effects on perception. We suggest that expectations can adjust the threshold required for encoding expectations-congruent information, thereby affecting the speed with which target objects are encoded in working memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001501/pdfft?md5=f8517f4c0fe24edb97ecd98c63d1ca5d&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001501-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141434081","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The statistics of cognitive variability: Explaining common patterns in individuals, groups and financial markets 认知变异统计:解释个人、群体和金融市场的共同模式
IF 3.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105858
Jian-Qiao Zhu , Jake Spicer , Adam Sanborn , Nick Chater

Psychological variability (i.e., “noise”) displays interesting structure which is hidden by the common practice of averaging over trials. Interesting noise structure, termed ‘stylized facts’, is observed in financial markets (i.e., behaviors from many thousands of traders). Here we investigate the parallels between psychological and financial time series. In a series of three experiments (total N = 202), we successively simplified a market-based price prediction task by first removing external information, and then removing any interaction between participants. Finally, we removed any resemblance to an asset market by asking individual participants to simply reproduce temporal intervals. All three experiments reproduced the main stylized facts found in financial markets, and the robustness of the results suggests that a common cognitive-level mechanism can produce them. We identify one potential model based on mental sampling algorithms, showing how this general-purpose model might account for behavior across these very different tasks.

心理变异(即 "噪音")显示出有趣的结构,而这种结构却被对试验进行平均的常见做法所掩盖。在金融市场(即成千上万交易者的行为)中也能观察到有趣的噪音结构,即 "风格化事实"。在此,我们研究了心理和金融时间序列之间的相似之处。在三个系列的实验中(总人数 = 202),我们先后简化了基于市场的价格预测任务,首先去除外部信息,然后去除参与者之间的任何互动。最后,我们消除了与资产市场的任何相似之处,要求单个参与者简单地再现时间间隔。所有这三个实验都再现了金融市场中发现的主要典型事实,而结果的稳健性表明,一个共同的认知层面机制可以产生这些事实。我们确定了一种基于心理取样算法的潜在模型,并展示了这种通用模型如何解释这些截然不同的任务中的行为。
{"title":"The statistics of cognitive variability: Explaining common patterns in individuals, groups and financial markets","authors":"Jian-Qiao Zhu ,&nbsp;Jake Spicer ,&nbsp;Adam Sanborn ,&nbsp;Nick Chater","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105858","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105858","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Psychological variability (i.e., “noise”) displays interesting structure which is hidden by the common practice of averaging over trials. Interesting noise structure, termed ‘stylized facts’, is observed in financial markets (i.e., behaviors from many thousands of traders). Here we investigate the parallels between psychological and financial time series. In a series of three experiments (total <em>N</em> <em>=</em> <em>202</em>), we successively simplified a market-based price prediction task by first removing external information, and then removing any interaction between participants. Finally, we removed any resemblance to an asset market by asking individual participants to simply reproduce temporal intervals. All three experiments reproduced the main stylized facts found in financial markets, and the robustness of the results suggests that a common cognitive-level mechanism can produce them. We identify one potential model based on mental sampling algorithms, showing how this general-purpose model might account for behavior across these very different tasks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001446/pdfft?md5=cf090b35b7ad29f44437b6f38055aed4&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001446-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141429814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Behavioural interference at event boundaries reduces long-term memory performance in the virtual water maze task without affecting working memory performance 事件边界的行为干扰会降低虚拟水迷宫任务中的长期记忆能力,但不会影响工作记忆能力
IF 3.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105859
Marie Pahlenkemper , Hannah Bernhard , Joel Reithler , Mark J. Roberts

Narrative episodic memory of movie clips can be retroactively impaired by presenting unrelated stimuli coinciding with event boundaries. This effect has been linked with rapid hippocampal processes triggered by the offset of the event, that are alternatively related either to memory consolidation or with working memory processes. Here we tested whether this effect extended to spatial memory, the temporal specificity and extent of the interference, and its effect on working- vs long-term memory. In three computerized adaptations of the Morris Water Maze, participants learned the location of an invisible target over three trials each. A second spatial navigation task was presented either immediately after finding the target, after a 10-s delay, or no second task was presented (control condition). A recall session, in which participants indicated the learned target location with 10 ‘pin-drop’ trials for each condition, was performed after a 1-h or a 24-h break. Spatial memory was measured by the mean distance between pins and the true location. Results indicated that the immediate presentation of the second task led to worse memory performance, for both break durations, compared to the delayed condition. There was no difference in performance between the delayed presentation and the control condition. Despite this long-term memory effect, we found no difference in the rate of performance improvement during the learning session, indicating no effect of the second task on working memory. Our findings are in line with a rapid process, linked to the offset of an event, that is involved in the early stages of memory consolidation.

电影片段的叙事性历时记忆会因出现与事件边界重合的无关刺激而受到追溯性损害。这种效应与事件偏移引发的快速海马过程有关,也与记忆巩固或工作记忆过程有关。在这里,我们测试了这种效应是否扩展到空间记忆、干扰的时间特异性和程度,以及它对工作记忆和长期记忆的影响。在莫里斯水迷宫的三次计算机化改编中,受试者在三次试验中学会了隐形目标的位置。在找到目标后,或在延迟 10 秒后立即执行第二项空间导航任务,或不执行第二项任务(对照条件)。在休息 1 小时或 24 小时后进行回忆,在每种条件下进行 10 次 "针落 "试验,让参与者指出所学目标的位置。空间记忆以大头针与真实位置之间的平均距离来衡量。结果表明,与延迟条件相比,在两种休息时间内立即进行第二项任务的记忆效果都较差。而延迟呈现和对照条件下的表现没有差异。尽管存在这种长期记忆效应,但我们发现在学习过程中成绩提高的速度没有差异,这表明第二个任务对工作记忆没有影响。我们的研究结果与记忆巩固早期阶段的一个与事件偏移有关的快速过程相吻合。
{"title":"Behavioural interference at event boundaries reduces long-term memory performance in the virtual water maze task without affecting working memory performance","authors":"Marie Pahlenkemper ,&nbsp;Hannah Bernhard ,&nbsp;Joel Reithler ,&nbsp;Mark J. Roberts","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105859","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105859","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Narrative episodic memory of movie clips can be retroactively impaired by presenting unrelated stimuli coinciding with event boundaries. This effect has been linked with rapid hippocampal processes triggered by the offset of the event, that are alternatively related either to memory consolidation or with working memory processes. Here we tested whether this effect extended to spatial memory, the temporal specificity and extent of the interference, and its effect on working- vs long-term memory. In three computerized adaptations of the Morris Water Maze, participants learned the location of an invisible target over three trials each. A second spatial navigation task was presented either immediately after finding the target, after a 10-s delay, or no second task was presented (control condition). A recall session, in which participants indicated the learned target location with 10 ‘pin-drop’ trials for each condition, was performed after a 1-h or a 24-h break. Spatial memory was measured by the mean distance between pins and the true location. Results indicated that the immediate presentation of the second task led to worse memory performance, for both break durations, compared to the delayed condition. There was no difference in performance between the delayed presentation and the control condition. Despite this long-term memory effect, we found no difference in the rate of performance improvement during the learning session, indicating no effect of the second task on working memory. Our findings are in line with a rapid process, linked to the offset of an event, that is involved in the early stages of memory consolidation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001458/pdfft?md5=a883a5cbb8a492299b00bbe292b12210&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001458-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141423012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Objectively quantifying subjective phenomena: Measuring the flashed face distortion effect 客观量化主观现象:测量闪烁脸部失真效应
IF 3.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105861
Yi Gao , Minzhi Wang , Dobromir Rahnev

Objectively quantifying subjective phenomena like visual illusions is challenging. We address this issue in the context of the Flashed Face Distortion Effect (FFDE), where faces presented in succession appear distorted and grotesque. We first show that the traditional method of quantifying FFDE – via subjective ratings of the level of distortion – is subject to substantial biases. Motivated by this finding, we develop an objective method for quantifying FFDE by introducing two design innovations. First, we create artificially distorted faces and ask subjects to discriminate between undistorted and objectively distorted faces. Second, we employ both an illusion condition, which includes a succession of 15 face flashes, and a control condition, which includes a single face flash and does not induce an illusion. Using these innovations, we quantify the strength of the face distortion illusion by comparing the response bias for identifying distorted faces between the illusion and control conditions. We find that our method successfully quantifies the face distortion, with subjects exhibiting a more liberal response bias in the illusion condition. Finally, we apply our new method to evaluate how the face distortion illusion is modulated by face eccentricity, face inversion, the temporal frequency of the face flashes, and presence of temporal gaps between consecutive faces. Our results demonstrate the utility of our objective method in quantifying the subjective illusion of face distortion. Critically, the method is general and can be applied to other phenomena that are inherently subjective.

客观量化视错觉等主观现象具有挑战性。我们以 "闪烁人脸失真效应"(FFDE)为背景探讨了这一问题,在这种效应中,连续呈现的人脸会出现扭曲和怪异。我们首先证明,量化 FFDE 的传统方法--通过对失真程度的主观评价--存在很大的偏差。受这一发现的启发,我们通过引入两项设计创新,开发出一种量化 FFDE 的客观方法。首先,我们制作了人为扭曲的人脸,并要求受试者区分未扭曲和客观扭曲的人脸。其次,我们同时采用了幻觉条件和对照条件,前者包括连续 15 次人脸闪烁,后者包括单次人脸闪烁且不诱发幻觉。利用这些创新,我们通过比较幻觉条件和对照条件下识别扭曲人脸的反应偏差,来量化人脸扭曲幻觉的强度。我们发现,我们的方法成功地量化了人脸失真,受试者在幻觉条件下表现出更宽松的反应偏差。最后,我们运用新方法评估了人脸失真幻觉如何受到人脸偏心率、人脸反转、人脸闪烁的时间频率以及连续人脸之间是否存在时间间隙的影响。我们的结果表明,我们的客观方法可以量化人脸失真的主观错觉。重要的是,该方法具有通用性,可应用于其他固有的主观现象。
{"title":"Objectively quantifying subjective phenomena: Measuring the flashed face distortion effect","authors":"Yi Gao ,&nbsp;Minzhi Wang ,&nbsp;Dobromir Rahnev","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105861","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105861","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Objectively quantifying subjective phenomena like visual illusions is challenging. We address this issue in the context of the Flashed Face Distortion Effect (FFDE), where faces presented in succession appear distorted and grotesque. We first show that the traditional method of quantifying FFDE – via subjective ratings of the level of distortion – is subject to substantial biases. Motivated by this finding, we develop an objective method for quantifying FFDE by introducing two design innovations. First, we create artificially distorted faces and ask subjects to discriminate between undistorted and objectively distorted faces. Second, we employ both an illusion condition, which includes a succession of 15 face flashes, and a control condition, which includes a single face flash and does not induce an illusion. Using these innovations, we quantify the strength of the face distortion illusion by comparing the response bias for identifying distorted faces between the illusion and control conditions. We find that our method successfully quantifies the face distortion, with subjects exhibiting a more liberal response bias in the illusion condition. Finally, we apply our new method to evaluate how the face distortion illusion is modulated by face eccentricity, face inversion, the temporal frequency of the face flashes, and presence of temporal gaps between consecutive faces. Our results demonstrate the utility of our objective method in quantifying the subjective illusion of face distortion. Critically, the method is general and can be applied to other phenomena that are inherently subjective.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421482","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An examination of the motivation to manage distraction 研究管理分心的动机
IF 3.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105862
Brian A. Anderson

Individuals exhibit limited awareness of when their attention is captured by salient but irrelevant stimuli, and it has long been argued that involuntary attentional capture by such stimuli is minimally disruptive to information processing. Yet, robust mechanisms of distractor suppression are hypothesized to support the control of attention, which presumably serve in the interest of managing distraction. In the present study, I examine whether participants are aware of the cost of distraction with respect to task performance, and whether they are motivated to manage this cost even when it is effortful to do so. Across three experiments, participants were willing to exert physical effort in order to reduce the frequency with which they encountered physically salient distractors, and in a fourth experiment tended to prefer trials with fewer distractors when given a choice over distractor frequency. Importantly, the amount of physical effort exerted varied as a function of the degree to which task-irrelevant distractors impaired search performance, suggesting that people are sensitive to the cost of distraction.

人们对自己的注意力何时被突出但不相关的刺激物所吸引的意识很有限,长期以来,人们一直认为这种刺激物对注意力的非自主吸引对信息处理的干扰很小。然而,人们假定,强大的分心抑制机制可以支持对注意力的控制,这大概是为了管理分心。在本研究中,笔者考察了参与者是否意识到分心对任务完成所带来的代价,以及他们是否有动力去控制这种代价,即使是在费力的情况下。在三项实验中,参与者都愿意付出体力,以减少他们遇到物理上突出的分心物的频率,而在第四项实验中,当参与者可以选择分心物的频率时,他们倾向于选择分心物较少的试验。重要的是,所付出的体力随任务无关的干扰物对搜索表现的影响程度而变化,这表明人们对干扰的代价很敏感。
{"title":"An examination of the motivation to manage distraction","authors":"Brian A. Anderson","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105862","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105862","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Individuals exhibit limited awareness of when their attention is captured by salient but irrelevant stimuli, and it has long been argued that involuntary attentional capture by such stimuli is minimally disruptive to information processing. Yet, robust mechanisms of distractor suppression are hypothesized to support the control of attention, which presumably serve in the interest of managing distraction. In the present study, I examine whether participants are aware of the <em>cost</em> of distraction with respect to task performance, and whether they are motivated to manage this cost even when it is effortful to do so. Across three experiments, participants were willing to exert physical effort in order to reduce the frequency with which they encountered physically salient distractors, and in a fourth experiment tended to prefer trials with fewer distractors when given a choice over distractor frequency. Importantly, the amount of physical effort exerted varied as a function of the degree to which task-irrelevant distractors impaired search performance, suggesting that people are sensitive to the cost of distraction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141328472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1