首页 > 最新文献

Behaviour Research and Therapy最新文献

英文 中文
Computerized cognitive control training to reduce rumination in major depression: A randomized controlled trial 计算机化认知控制训练可减少重度抑郁症患者的反刍:随机对照试验
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104521
Ulrike Zetsche , Pauline Neumann , Paul-Christian Bürkner , Babette Renneberg , Ernst H.W. Koster , Kristof Hoorelbeke

Objective

Rumination is a major risk factor for the onset and recurrence of depressive episodes and has been associated with deficits in updating working memory content. This randomized controlled trial examines whether training updating-specific cognitive control processes reduces daily ruminative thoughts in clinically depressed individuals.

Methods

Sixty-five individuals with a current major depressive episode were randomized to 10 sessions of either cognitive control training (N = 31) or placebo training (N = 34). The frequency and negativity of individuals’ daily ruminative thoughts were assessed for seven days before training, after training, and at a 3-month follow-up using experience sampling methodology. Secondary outcomes were depressive symptoms, depressed mood, and level of disability.

Results

Cognitive control training led to stronger improvements in the trained task than placebo training. However, cognitive control training did not lead to greater reductions in the frequency or negativity of daily ruminative thoughts than placebo training. There were no training-specific effects on participants' depressive symptoms or level of disability.

Conclusions

The robustness of the present null-findings, combined with the methodological strengths of the study, suggest that training currently depressed individuals to update emotional content in working memory does not affect the frequency or negativity of their daily ruminative thoughts.

目的反刍是抑郁症发作和复发的一个主要风险因素,与工作记忆内容更新方面的缺陷有关。本随机对照试验研究了对临床抑郁症患者进行更新特异性认知控制过程的训练是否能减少他们每天的反刍想法。方法65名目前有严重抑郁发作的患者被随机分配到认知控制训练(31人)或安慰剂训练(34人)中,共10个疗程。在训练前七天、训练后和为期三个月的随访中,采用经验抽样法对患者日常反刍想法的频率和消极性进行评估。次要结果包括抑郁症状、抑郁情绪和残疾程度。然而,与安慰剂训练相比,认知控制训练并没有导致日常反刍想法的频率或消极性有更大的降低。结论本结论的稳健性与研究方法的优势相结合,表明训练当前抑郁症患者更新工作记忆中的情绪内容并不会影响其日常反刍想法的频率或消极性。
{"title":"Computerized cognitive control training to reduce rumination in major depression: A randomized controlled trial","authors":"Ulrike Zetsche ,&nbsp;Pauline Neumann ,&nbsp;Paul-Christian Bürkner ,&nbsp;Babette Renneberg ,&nbsp;Ernst H.W. Koster ,&nbsp;Kristof Hoorelbeke","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104521","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104521","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Rumination is a major risk factor for the onset and recurrence of depressive episodes and has been associated with deficits in updating working memory content. This randomized controlled trial examines whether training updating-specific cognitive control processes reduces daily ruminative thoughts in clinically depressed individuals.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Sixty-five individuals with a current major depressive episode were randomized to 10 sessions of either cognitive control training (N = 31) or placebo training (N = 34). The frequency and negativity of individuals’ daily ruminative thoughts were assessed for seven days before training, after training, and at a 3-month follow-up using experience sampling methodology. Secondary outcomes were depressive symptoms, depressed mood, and level of disability.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Cognitive control training led to stronger improvements in the trained task than placebo training. However, cognitive control training did not lead to greater reductions in the frequency or negativity of daily ruminative thoughts than placebo training. There were no training-specific effects on participants' depressive symptoms or level of disability.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The robustness of the present null-findings, combined with the methodological strengths of the study, suggest that training currently depressed individuals to update emotional content in working memory does not affect the frequency or negativity of their daily ruminative thoughts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"177 ","pages":"Article 104521"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000482/pdfft?md5=3df78569497bc0973079c54a42fb9e9e&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000482-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140273466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optimizing indicated cognitive behavioral therapy to prevent child anxiety and depression: A cluster-randomized factorial trial 优化认知行为疗法,预防儿童焦虑和抑郁:分组随机因子试验
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104520
Carina Lisøy , Simon-Peter Neumer , Frode Adolfsen , Jo Magne Ingul , Lene-Mari Potulski Rasmussen , Tore Wentzel-Larsen , Joshua Patras , Anne Mari Sund , Kristin Ytreland , Trine Waaktaar , Solveig Holen , Anne Liv Askeland , Ida Mari Haug , Elisabeth Valmyr Bania , Kristin Martinsen

Identifying effective components can lead to interventions that are less resource-intensive and better suited for real-world needs. In this 2×2×2 cluster-randomized factorial trial (clinicaltrials.gov NCT04263558), we investigated the effects of three components of an indicated, transdiagnostic CBT intervention for children: 1) Intervention Delivery Format (child group format versus a blended format with group sessions and automated web-based sessions), 2) Parental Involvement in the intervention (group-based versus psychoeducational brochure), and 3) a Measurement Feedback System (MFS; on versus off). The intervention was delivered at schools in a group-based format. The participants (N = 701 children) were school children (age 8–12 years) with elevated symptoms of anxiety or depression, and their parents. The main outcomes were self-reported (N = 633) and parent-reported (N = 725) symptoms of child anxiety and depression post-intervention. The secondary outcome was children's user satisfaction with the intervention. We did not find significant main or interaction effects of Delivery Format, Parental Involvement, or MFS on children's symptom levels. There were no significant effects on children's user satisfaction. Results were compatible with retaining the least resource intensive combination (i.e., blended format, parental brochure, no MFS) in an optimized intervention.

找出有效的干预措施可以降低资源密集度,更好地满足现实世界的需求。在这项 2×2×2 分组随机析因试验(clinicaltrials.gov NCT04263558)中,我们研究了针对儿童的跨诊断 CBT 干预的三个组成部分的效果:1)干预实施形式(儿童小组形式与小组会议和自动网络会议的混合形式);2)家长参与干预(基于小组的手册与心理教育手册);3)测量反馈系统(MFS;开与关)。干预在学校以小组形式进行。参与者(N = 701 名儿童)为焦虑或抑郁症状加重的在校儿童(8-12 岁)及其家长。主要结果是干预后儿童焦虑和抑郁症状的自我报告(633 人)和家长报告(725 人)。次要结果是儿童用户对干预的满意度。我们没有发现提供形式、家长参与或 MFS 对儿童症状水平有明显的主效应或交互效应。对儿童用户满意度的影响也不明显。结果与在优化干预中保留资源消耗最小的组合(即混合形式、家长手册、无 MFS)相一致。
{"title":"Optimizing indicated cognitive behavioral therapy to prevent child anxiety and depression: A cluster-randomized factorial trial","authors":"Carina Lisøy ,&nbsp;Simon-Peter Neumer ,&nbsp;Frode Adolfsen ,&nbsp;Jo Magne Ingul ,&nbsp;Lene-Mari Potulski Rasmussen ,&nbsp;Tore Wentzel-Larsen ,&nbsp;Joshua Patras ,&nbsp;Anne Mari Sund ,&nbsp;Kristin Ytreland ,&nbsp;Trine Waaktaar ,&nbsp;Solveig Holen ,&nbsp;Anne Liv Askeland ,&nbsp;Ida Mari Haug ,&nbsp;Elisabeth Valmyr Bania ,&nbsp;Kristin Martinsen","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104520","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Identifying effective components can lead to interventions that are less resource-intensive and better suited for real-world needs. In this 2×2×2 cluster-randomized factorial trial (<span>clinicaltrials.gov</span><svg><path></path></svg> NCT04263558), we investigated the effects of three components of an indicated, transdiagnostic CBT intervention for children: 1) Intervention Delivery Format (child group format versus a blended format with group sessions and automated web-based sessions), 2) Parental Involvement in the intervention (group-based versus psychoeducational brochure), and 3) a Measurement Feedback System (MFS; on versus off). The intervention was delivered at schools in a group-based format. The participants (<em>N</em> = 701 children) were school children (age 8–12 years) with elevated symptoms of anxiety or depression, and their parents. The main outcomes were self-reported (<em>N</em> = 633) and parent-reported (<em>N</em> = 725) symptoms of child anxiety and depression post-intervention. The secondary outcome was children's user satisfaction with the intervention. We did not find significant main or interaction effects of Delivery Format, Parental Involvement, or MFS on children's symptom levels. There were no significant effects on children's user satisfaction. Results were compatible with retaining the least resource intensive combination (i.e., blended format, parental brochure, no MFS) in an optimized intervention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104520"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000470/pdfft?md5=aa9f350985b0f2e564b4edcaad931758&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000470-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140191864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unique and interactive effects of emotion regulation difficulties and perceived stress on COVID-19 traumatic stress, anxiety, and safety behavior use: A four-year prospective study 情绪调节困难和感知压力对 COVID-19 创伤压力、焦虑和安全行为使用的独特和交互影响:一项为期四年的前瞻性研究
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104503
Sarah C. Jessup , Alexandra M. Adamis , Catherine E. Rast , Rebecca C. Cox , Bunmi O. Olatunji

Given that emotion regulation difficulties confer risk for poor responses to stress, they may predict who is at risk for adverse psychological reactions to major, chronic stressors such as the COVID-19 pandemic. Specific adverse reactions to the pandemic may include more severe traumatic stress, anxiety, and excessive safety behavior use (i.e., hand washing). While emotion regulation difficulties may be a diathesis for adverse reactions to chronic stressors, the context(s) by which they may confer elevated risk is unclear. Accordingly, the present longitudinal study examined the interaction between pre-pandemic emotion regulation difficulties and early pandemic perceived stress in predicting subsequent COVID-related traumatic stress, anxiety, and safety behavior use over 32 weeks of the pandemic. Community adults (N = 145) who completed a measure of emotion regulation in 2016 as part of a larger study were recontacted at the start of the pandemic (March 2020) and assessed every two weeks for 32 weeks. Consistent with a diathesis-stress model, the interaction between difficulties in emotion regulation and perceived stress was significant in predicting COVID-19 anxiety (p = 0.003, d = 0.52) such that at high, but not low, levels of perceived stress, difficulties in emotion regulation in 2016 significantly predicted higher COVID-19 anxiety in 2020. The interaction between difficulties in emotion regulation in 2016 and perceived stress early in 2020 approached significance in predicting COVID-19 traumatic stress (p = 0.073, d = 0.31) and safety behavior use (p = 0.069, d = 0.31). These findings highlight that current perceived stress is an important context that potentiates the effects of preexisting emotion regulation difficulties on the emergence of anxiety-related symptoms during COVID-19, which has important implications for diathesis-stress models of adverse reactions to chronic stressors.

鉴于情绪调节困难会导致对压力的反应不佳,因此可以预测哪些人可能会对 COVID-19 大流行病等重大、慢性压力源产生不良心理反应。对大流行病的具体不良反应可能包括更严重的创伤性压力、焦虑和过度使用安全行为(如洗手)。虽然情绪调节困难可能是对慢性应激源产生不良反应的一种病因,但其可能导致风险升高的背景尚不清楚。因此,本纵向研究考察了大流行前的情绪调节障碍与大流行早期的感知压力之间的相互作用,以预测大流行期间 32 周内与 COVID 相关的创伤性压力、焦虑和安全行为的使用情况。作为一项大型研究的一部分,社区成人(N = 145)在 2016 年完成了一项情绪调节测量,在大流行开始时(2020 年 3 月)与他们再次取得联系,并在 32 周内每两周进行一次评估。与症状-压力模型一致,情绪调节困难与感知压力之间的交互作用在预测 COVID-19 焦虑方面具有显著性(p = 0.003,d = 0.52),因此在感知压力水平较高而非较低的情况下,2016 年的情绪调节困难可显著预测 2020 年较高的 COVID-19 焦虑。在预测 COVID-19 创伤性压力(p = 0.073,d = 0.31)和安全行为使用(p = 0.069,d = 0.31)方面,2016 年情绪调节困难与 2020 年初感知到的压力之间的交互作用接近显著性。这些发现突出表明,在 COVID-19 期间,当前感知到的压力是一个重要的背景,它增强了先前存在的情绪调节困难对焦虑相关症状出现的影响,这对慢性压力源不良反应的病因-压力模型具有重要意义。
{"title":"Unique and interactive effects of emotion regulation difficulties and perceived stress on COVID-19 traumatic stress, anxiety, and safety behavior use: A four-year prospective study","authors":"Sarah C. Jessup ,&nbsp;Alexandra M. Adamis ,&nbsp;Catherine E. Rast ,&nbsp;Rebecca C. Cox ,&nbsp;Bunmi O. Olatunji","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104503","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Given that emotion regulation difficulties confer risk for poor responses to stress, they may predict who is at risk for adverse psychological reactions to major, chronic stressors such as the COVID-19 pandemic. Specific adverse reactions to the pandemic may include more severe traumatic stress, anxiety, and excessive safety behavior use (i.e., hand washing). While emotion regulation difficulties may be a diathesis for adverse reactions to chronic stressors, the context(s) by which they may confer elevated risk is unclear. Accordingly, the present longitudinal study examined the interaction between pre-pandemic emotion regulation difficulties and early pandemic perceived stress in predicting subsequent COVID-related traumatic stress, anxiety, and safety behavior use over 32 weeks of the pandemic. Community adults (<em>N</em> = 145) who completed a measure of emotion regulation in 2016 as part of a larger study were recontacted at the start of the pandemic (March 2020) and assessed every two weeks for 32 weeks. Consistent with a diathesis-stress model, the interaction between difficulties in emotion regulation and perceived stress was significant in predicting COVID-19 anxiety (<em>p</em> = 0.003, <em>d</em> = 0.52) such that at high, but not low, levels of perceived stress, difficulties in emotion regulation in 2016 significantly predicted higher COVID-19 anxiety in 2020. The interaction between difficulties in emotion regulation in 2016 and perceived stress early in 2020 approached significance in predicting COVID-19 traumatic stress (<em>p</em> = 0.073, <em>d =</em> 0.<em>31</em>) and safety behavior use (<em>p</em> = 0.069, <em>d</em> = 0.31). These findings highlight that current perceived stress is an important context that potentiates the effects of preexisting emotion regulation difficulties on the emergence of anxiety-related symptoms during COVID-19, which has important implications for diathesis-stress models of adverse reactions to chronic stressors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104503"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140187526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Amygdala real-time fMRI neurofeedback upregulation in treatment resistant depression: Proof of concept and dose determination 杏仁核实时 fMRI 神经反馈对耐药性抑郁症的上调作用:概念验证和剂量确定
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104523
Laurie Compère , Greg J. Siegle , Sair Lazzaro , Emily Riley , Marlene Strege , Gia Canovali , Scott Barb , Theodore Huppert , Kymberly Young

Previous work has shown that adults suffering from major depressive disorder (MDD) can increase their amygdala reactivity while recalling positive memories via real-time neurofeedback (rt-fMRI-nf) training, which is associated with reduction in depressive symptoms. This study investigated if this intervention could also be considered for patients suffering from MDD who do not respond to standard psychological and pharmacological interventions, i.e., treatment resistant (TR-MDD).

15 participants received 5 neurofeedback sessions. Outcome measures were depressive symptoms assessed by BDI scores up to 12 weeks following acute intervention, and amygdala activity changes from initial baseline to final transfer run during neurofeedback sessions (neurofeedback success).

Participants succeeded in increasing their amygdala activity. A main effect of visit on BDI scores indicated a significant reduction in depressive symptomatology. Percent signal change in the amygdala showed a learning curve during the first session only. Neurofeedback success computed by session was significantly positive only during the second session. When examining the baseline amygdala response, baseline activity stabilized/asymptoted by session 3.

This proof-of-concept study suggests that only two neurofeedback sessions are necessary to enable those patients to upregulate their amygdala activity, warranting a future RCT. Over the course of the rtfMRI-nf intervention, participants also reported reduced depressive symptomatology.

Clinical trial registration number: NCT03428828 on ClinicalTrials.gov.

先前的研究表明,患有重度抑郁症(MDD)的成年人可以通过实时神经反馈(rt-fMRI-nf)训练在回忆积极记忆时提高杏仁核的反应性,这与抑郁症状的减轻有关。本研究调查了这种干预措施是否也适用于对标准心理和药物干预措施无反应的 MDD 患者,即对治疗有抵抗力的患者(TR-MDD)。结果测量指标为急性干预后12周内的BDI评分评估的抑郁症状,以及神经反馈疗程中杏仁核活动从初始基线到最终转移运行的变化(神经反馈成功率)。访问对 BDI 评分的主效应表明,抑郁症状明显减轻。杏仁核信号变化百分比仅在第一次治疗中呈现出学习曲线。按疗程计算的神经反馈成功率仅在第二次疗程中呈现显著正值。这项概念验证研究表明,只需进行两次神经反馈治疗,就能使患者的杏仁核活动得到上调,因此有必要在未来进行一项临床试验。在rtfMRI-nf干预过程中,参与者还报告了抑郁症状的减轻:临床试验注册号:NCT03428828。
{"title":"Amygdala real-time fMRI neurofeedback upregulation in treatment resistant depression: Proof of concept and dose determination","authors":"Laurie Compère ,&nbsp;Greg J. Siegle ,&nbsp;Sair Lazzaro ,&nbsp;Emily Riley ,&nbsp;Marlene Strege ,&nbsp;Gia Canovali ,&nbsp;Scott Barb ,&nbsp;Theodore Huppert ,&nbsp;Kymberly Young","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104523","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104523","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous work has shown that adults suffering from major depressive disorder (MDD) can increase their amygdala reactivity while recalling positive memories via real-time neurofeedback (rt-fMRI-nf) training, which is associated with reduction in depressive symptoms. This study investigated if this intervention could also be considered for patients suffering from MDD who do not respond to standard psychological and pharmacological interventions, i.e., treatment resistant (TR-MDD).</p><p>15 participants received 5 neurofeedback sessions. Outcome measures were depressive symptoms assessed by BDI scores up to 12 weeks following acute intervention, and amygdala activity changes from initial baseline to final transfer run during neurofeedback sessions (neurofeedback success).</p><p>Participants succeeded in increasing their amygdala activity. A main effect of visit on BDI scores indicated a significant reduction in depressive symptomatology. Percent signal change in the amygdala showed a learning curve during the first session only. Neurofeedback success computed by session was significantly positive only during the second session. When examining the baseline amygdala response, baseline activity stabilized/asymptoted by session 3.</p><p>This proof-of-concept study suggests that only two neurofeedback sessions are necessary to enable those patients to upregulate their amygdala activity, warranting a future RCT. Over the course of the rtfMRI-nf intervention, participants also reported reduced depressive symptomatology.</p><p>Clinical trial registration number: NCT03428828 on <span>ClinicalTrials.gov</span><svg><path></path></svg>.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104523"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000500/pdfft?md5=f4e0e65a012ea8d9e4067a5603181a61&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000500-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140179805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Heart rate reactivity during trauma recall as a predictor of treatment outcome in cognitive processing therapy for PTSD 创伤回忆时的心率反应是创伤后应激障碍认知加工疗法治疗效果的预测因素
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104519
Joseph K. Carpenter , Suzanne L. Pineles , Michael G. Griffin , Shivani Pandey , Kimberly Werner , Natalia M. Kecala , Patricia A. Resick , Tara E. Galovski

Emotional engagement when recollecting a trauma memory is considered a key element of effective trauma-focused therapy. Research has shown that reduced physiological reactivity during trauma recall is associated with worse treatment outcomes for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but this has yet to be examined in a cognitively oriented treatment. This study examined whether pretreatment heart rate (HR) reactivity during trauma recall predicts PTSD symptom improvement and treatment dropout during Cognitive Processing Therapy (CPT) for PTSD. Participants were 142 women with PTSD secondary to interpersonal violence enrolled in one of two clinicals trials. HR reactivity reflected the mean increase in HR after listening to two 30-s scripts of the trauma memory prior to treatment. Linear mixed-effects models showed the effect of HR reactivity on change in total PTSD symptoms was not significant, but lower HR reactivity predicted less improvement in reexperiencing and avoidance and was associated with increased dropout. Findings suggest pretreatment physiological reactivity to the trauma memory may be a prognostic indicator of some elements of treatment response in CPT. Results tentatively support the importance of emotional activation during trauma recall in cognitive treatment of PTSD, though more research is needed to clarify how low HR reactivity impacts treatment.

回忆创伤记忆时的情绪参与被认为是有效的创伤焦点疗法的关键因素。研究表明,创伤回忆时生理反应的降低与创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)治疗效果的恶化有关,但这一问题尚未在认知导向治疗中得到研究。本研究考察了创伤回忆时的预处理心率(HR)反应性是否能预测创伤后应激障碍症状的改善以及认知加工疗法(CPT)治疗创伤后应激障碍时的治疗退出情况。142 名因人际暴力而继发创伤后应激障碍的女性参加了两项临床试验中的一项。心率反应性反映了在治疗前聆听两段30秒的创伤记忆脚本后心率的平均增幅。线性混合效应模型显示,心率反应性对创伤后应激障碍总症状变化的影响并不显著,但较低的心率反应性预示着再体验和回避方面的改善较少,并且与辍学率增加有关。研究结果表明,治疗前对创伤记忆的生理反应性可能是CPT治疗反应中某些因素的预后指标。研究结果初步支持了创伤回忆中的情绪激活在创伤后应激障碍认知治疗中的重要性,但还需要更多的研究来阐明低心率反应性对治疗的影响。
{"title":"Heart rate reactivity during trauma recall as a predictor of treatment outcome in cognitive processing therapy for PTSD","authors":"Joseph K. Carpenter ,&nbsp;Suzanne L. Pineles ,&nbsp;Michael G. Griffin ,&nbsp;Shivani Pandey ,&nbsp;Kimberly Werner ,&nbsp;Natalia M. Kecala ,&nbsp;Patricia A. Resick ,&nbsp;Tara E. Galovski","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104519","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104519","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotional engagement when recollecting a trauma memory is considered a key element of effective trauma-focused therapy. Research has shown that reduced physiological reactivity during trauma recall is associated with worse treatment outcomes for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but this has yet to be examined in a cognitively oriented treatment. This study examined whether pretreatment heart rate (HR) reactivity during trauma recall predicts PTSD symptom improvement and treatment dropout during Cognitive Processing Therapy (CPT) for PTSD. Participants were 142 women with PTSD secondary to interpersonal violence enrolled in one of two clinicals trials. HR reactivity reflected the mean increase in HR after listening to two 30-s scripts of the trauma memory prior to treatment. Linear mixed-effects models showed the effect of HR reactivity on change in total PTSD symptoms was not significant, but lower HR reactivity predicted less improvement in reexperiencing and avoidance and was associated with increased dropout. Findings suggest pretreatment physiological reactivity to the trauma memory may be a prognostic indicator of some elements of treatment response in CPT. Results tentatively support the importance of emotional activation during trauma recall in cognitive treatment of PTSD, though more research is needed to clarify how low HR reactivity impacts treatment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104519"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140160872","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reciprocal effects between negative affect and emotion regulation in daily life 日常生活中负面情绪与情绪调节之间的相互影响
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104518
Xiaoqin Wang , Shiyu Shao , Zhouqu Cai , Chenyue Ma , Lei Jia , Scott D. Blain , Yafei Tan

The extended process model of emotion regulation provides a framework for understanding how emotional experiences and emotion regulation (ER) mutually influence each other over time. To investigate this reciprocal relationship, 202 adults completed a ten-day experience-sampling survey capturing levels of negative affect (NA) experience and use of ten ER strategies in daily life. Residual dynamic structural equation models (DSEMs) were used to examine within-person cross-lagged and autoregressive effects of NA and ER (strategy use and between-strategy variability). Results showed that NA predicted lower between-strategy variability, lower subsequent use of acceptance and problem-solving, but higher subsequent use of rumination and worry. Moreover, reappraisal and between-strategy variability predicted lower subsequent NA levels, while expressive suppression and worry predicted higher subsequent NA levels. Stable autoregressive effects were found for NA and for maladaptive ER strategies (e.g., rumination and worry). Exploratory correlation analyses revealed positive associations between NA inertia and maladaptive ER strategies. Together, these findings provide evidence of a dynamic interplay between NA and ER. This work deepens how we understand the challenges of applying ER strategies in daily life. Future clinical and translational research should consider these dynamic perspectives on ER and affect.

情绪调节的扩展过程模型为理解情绪体验和情绪调节(ER)如何随着时间的推移相互影响提供了一个框架。为了研究这种相互影响的关系,202 名成年人完成了一项为期十天的经验取样调查,调查内容包括负性情绪(NA)的经验水平以及日常生活中十种情绪调节策略的使用情况。残差动态结构方程模型(DSEMs)被用来研究NA和ER(策略使用和策略间变异性)的人内交叉滞后效应和自回归效应。结果表明,NA 预测了较低的策略间变异性,较低的接受和问题解决的后续使用,但较高的反刍和担忧的后续使用。此外,重新评估和策略间变异性预示着较低的后续 NA 水平,而表达性抑制和担忧则预示着较高的后续 NA 水平。在 NA 和适应不良的 ER 策略(如反刍和担忧)方面发现了稳定的自回归效应。探索性相关分析显示,NA惰性与适应不良的ER策略之间存在正相关。这些发现共同证明了NA和ER之间的动态相互作用。这项工作加深了我们对在日常生活中应用ER策略所面临挑战的理解。未来的临床和转化研究应考虑ER和情感的这些动态视角。
{"title":"Reciprocal effects between negative affect and emotion regulation in daily life","authors":"Xiaoqin Wang ,&nbsp;Shiyu Shao ,&nbsp;Zhouqu Cai ,&nbsp;Chenyue Ma ,&nbsp;Lei Jia ,&nbsp;Scott D. Blain ,&nbsp;Yafei Tan","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104518","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104518","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The extended process model of emotion regulation provides a framework for understanding how emotional experiences and emotion regulation (ER) mutually influence each other over time. To investigate this reciprocal relationship, 202 adults completed a ten-day experience-sampling survey capturing levels of negative affect (NA) experience and use of ten ER strategies in daily life. Residual dynamic structural equation models (DSEMs) were used to examine within-person cross-lagged and autoregressive effects of NA and ER (strategy use and between-strategy variability). Results showed that NA predicted lower between-strategy variability, lower subsequent use of acceptance and problem-solving, but higher subsequent use of rumination and worry. Moreover, reappraisal and between-strategy variability predicted lower subsequent NA levels, while expressive suppression and worry predicted higher subsequent NA levels. Stable autoregressive effects were found for NA and for maladaptive ER strategies (e.g., rumination and worry). Exploratory correlation analyses revealed positive associations between NA inertia and maladaptive ER strategies. Together, these findings provide evidence of a dynamic interplay between NA and ER. This work deepens how we understand the challenges of applying ER strategies in daily life. Future clinical and translational research should consider these dynamic perspectives on ER and affect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104518"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140138380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of a retrieval cue on renewal of conditioned responses in human appetitive conditioning 在人类食欲条件反射中,检索线索对条件反应更新的影响
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-03-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104501
Frank Lörsch, Ines Kollei, Sabine Steins-Loeber

Contextual renewal of reward anticipation may be one potential mechanism underlying relapse in eating and substance use disorders. We therefore tested retrieval cues, a method derived from an inhibitory retrieval-based model of extinction learning to attenuate contextual renewal using an appetitive conditioning paradigm. A pilot study was carried out in Experiment 1 to validate a differential chocolate conditioning paradigm, in which a specific tray was set up as a conditioned stimulus (CS) for eating chocolate (unconditioned stimulus, US). Using an ABA renewal design in Experiment 2, half of the participants were presented with a retrieval cue in the acquisition phase (group AC) and the other half in the extinction phase (group EC). Presentation of the retrieval cue in the EC was associated with reduced renewal of US-expectancy, while there was a clear renewal effect for US-expectancy in the AC. One limitation was the difference in cue presentations between both groups due to the number of trials in acquisition and extinction. Experiment 3 therefore aimed at replicating the results of Experiment 2, but with fewer cue presentations for the EC to match the AC. No significant group differences were observed indicating no effect of the retrieval cue. Theoretical and clinical implications in light of the differing results are discussed.

奖励预期的情境更新可能是饮食和药物使用障碍复发的潜在机制之一。因此,我们对检索线索进行了测试。检索线索是从基于抑制性检索的消减学习模型中衍生出来的一种方法,它可以利用食欲条件反射范式来减弱情境更新。在实验 1 中,我们进行了一项试验研究,以验证差异巧克力条件反射范式。在该范式中,特定的托盘被设定为吃巧克力的条件刺激(CS)(非条件刺激,US)。实验 2 采用 ABA 更新设计,一半参与者在获得阶段(AC 组)看到检索线索,另一半参与者在消退阶段(EC 组)看到检索线索。在EC组中呈现检索线索与US-expectancy的更新减少有关,而在AC组中US-expectancy有明显的更新效应。一个局限性是,由于获得和消退的试验次数不同,两组的线索呈现方式也不同。因此,实验 3 的目的是复制实验 2 的结果,但为了与 AC 相匹配,减少了 EC 的提示次数。实验结果没有观察到明显的组间差异,表明检索线索没有影响。本文讨论了不同结果的理论和临床意义。
{"title":"The effects of a retrieval cue on renewal of conditioned responses in human appetitive conditioning","authors":"Frank Lörsch,&nbsp;Ines Kollei,&nbsp;Sabine Steins-Loeber","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104501","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104501","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Contextual renewal of reward anticipation may be one potential mechanism underlying relapse in eating and substance use disorders. We therefore tested retrieval cues, a method derived from an inhibitory retrieval-based model of extinction learning to attenuate contextual renewal using an appetitive conditioning paradigm. A pilot study was carried out in Experiment 1 to validate a differential chocolate conditioning paradigm, in which a specific tray was set up as a conditioned stimulus (CS) for eating chocolate (unconditioned stimulus, US). Using an ABA renewal design in Experiment 2, half of the participants were presented with a retrieval cue in the acquisition phase (group AC) and the other half in the extinction phase (group EC). Presentation of the retrieval cue in the EC was associated with reduced renewal of US-expectancy, while there was a clear renewal effect for US-expectancy in the AC. One limitation was the difference in cue presentations between both groups due to the number of trials in acquisition and extinction. Experiment 3 therefore aimed at replicating the results of Experiment 2, but with fewer cue presentations for the EC to match the AC. No significant group differences were observed indicating no effect of the retrieval cue. Theoretical and clinical implications in light of the differing results are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104501"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140082797","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Risk estimation in relation to anxiety and depression for low probability negative events 低概率负面事件与焦虑和抑郁相关的风险评估
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-02-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104500
Amelia S. Dev, Hannah C. Broos, Maria M. Llabre, Patrice G. Saab, Kiara R. Timpano

Foundational cognitive models propose that people with anxiety and depression show risk estimation bias, but most literature does not compute true risk estimation bias by comparing people's subjective risk estimates to their individualized reality (i.e., person-level objective risk). In a diverse community sample (N = 319), we calculated risk estimation bias by comparing people's subjective risk estimates for contracting COVID-19 to their individualized objective risk. Person-level objective risk was consistently low and did not differ across symptom levels, suggesting that for low probability negative events, people with greater symptoms show risk estimation bias that is driven by subjective risk estimates. Greater levels of anxiety, depression, and COVID-specific perseverative cognition separately predicted higher subjective risk estimates. In a model including COVID-specific perseverative cognition alongside anxiety and depression scores, the only significant predictor of subjective risk estimates was COVID-specific perseverative cognition, indicating that symptoms more closely tied to feared outcomes may more strongly influence risk estimation. Finally, subjective risk estimates predicted information-seeking behavior and eating when anxious, but did not significantly predict alcohol or marijuana use, drinking to cope, or information avoidance. Implications for clinical practitioners and future research are discussed.

基本认知模型认为,焦虑症和抑郁症患者会出现风险估计偏差,但大多数文献并没有通过比较患者的主观风险估计和他们的个体化现实(即个人层面的客观风险)来计算真正的风险估计偏差。在一个多样化的社区样本(N = 319)中,我们通过比较人们对感染 COVID-19 的主观风险估计和他们的个体化客观风险来计算风险估计偏差。个人水平的客观风险一直较低,且在不同症状水平的人群中并无差异,这表明对于低概率的负面事件,症状较重的人群会因主观风险估计而出现风险估计偏差。焦虑、抑郁和 COVID 特异性持久性认知水平越高,主观风险估计值就越高。在一个包括 COVID 特异性持久性认知以及焦虑和抑郁得分的模型中,主观风险估计的唯一显著预测因素是 COVID 特异性持久性认知,这表明与所担心的结果更密切相关的症状可能会更强烈地影响风险估计。最后,主观风险估计值预测了焦虑时的信息搜寻行为和进食行为,但对酒精或大麻的使用、为应对而饮酒或信息回避没有显著的预测作用。本文讨论了对临床从业人员和未来研究的启示。
{"title":"Risk estimation in relation to anxiety and depression for low probability negative events","authors":"Amelia S. Dev,&nbsp;Hannah C. Broos,&nbsp;Maria M. Llabre,&nbsp;Patrice G. Saab,&nbsp;Kiara R. Timpano","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104500","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104500","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Foundational cognitive models propose that people with anxiety and depression show risk estimation bias, but most literature does not compute true risk estimation bias by comparing people's subjective risk estimates to their individualized reality (i.e., person-level objective risk). In a diverse community sample (<em>N</em> = 319), we calculated risk estimation bias by comparing people's subjective risk estimates for contracting COVID-19 to their individualized objective risk. Person-level objective risk was consistently low and did not differ across symptom levels, suggesting that for low probability negative events, people with greater symptoms show risk estimation bias that is driven by subjective risk estimates. Greater levels of anxiety, depression, and COVID-specific perseverative cognition separately predicted higher subjective risk estimates. In a model including COVID-specific perseverative cognition alongside anxiety and depression scores, the only significant predictor of subjective risk estimates was COVID-specific perseverative cognition, indicating that symptoms more closely tied to feared outcomes may more strongly influence risk estimation. Finally, subjective risk estimates predicted information-seeking behavior and eating when anxious, but did not significantly predict alcohol or marijuana use, drinking to cope, or information avoidance. Implications for clinical practitioners and future research are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104500"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140014539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contamination fear and attention bias variability early in the COVID-19 pandemic COVID-19 大流行早期的污染恐惧和注意偏差变异性
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-02-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104497
Patrick J.F. Clarke , Elise Szeremeta , Bram Van Bockstaele , Lies Notebaert , Frances Meeten , Jemma Todd

The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic resulted in a dramatic increase in the salience and importance of information relating to both the risk of infection, and factors that could mitigate against such risk. This is likely to have contributed to elevated contamination fear concerns in the general population. Biased attention for contamination-related information has been proposed as a potential mechanism underlying contamination fear, though evidence regarding the presence of such biased attention has been inconsistent. A possible reason for this is that contamination fear may be characterised by variability in attention bias that has not yet been examined. The current study examined the potential association between attention bias variability for both contamination-related and mitigation-related stimuli, and contamination fear during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. A final sample of 315 participants completed measures of attention bias and contamination fear. The measure of average attention bias for contamination-related stimuli and mitigation-related stimuli was not associated with contamination fear (r = 0.055 and r = 0.051, p > 0.10), though both attention bias variability measures did show a small but statistically significant relationship with contamination fear (r = 0.133, p < 0.05; r = 0.147, p < 0.01). These attention bias variability measures also accounted for significant additional variance in contamination fear above the average attention bias measure (and controlling for response time variability). These findings provide initial evidence for the association between attention bias variability and contamination fear, underscoring a potential target for cognitive bias interventions for clinical contamination fear.

COVID-19 大流行的爆发导致与感染风险和可降低这种风险的因素有关的信息的显著性和重要性急剧增加。这很可能是造成普通民众对污染恐惧担忧加剧的原因之一。有人提出,对污染相关信息的偏向性注意是造成污染恐惧的潜在机制,但有关这种偏向性注意存在的证据并不一致。其中一个可能的原因是,污染恐惧的特点可能是注意偏差的可变性,而这种可变性尚未得到研究。本研究探讨了在 COVID-19 大流行的早期阶段,污染相关刺激和缓解相关刺激的注意偏差变异性与污染恐惧之间的潜在关联。最终有 315 名参与者完成了注意力偏差和污染恐惧的测量。对污染相关刺激和缓解相关刺激的平均注意偏差测量与污染恐惧无关(r = 0.055 和 r = 0.051,p >0.10),尽管两种注意偏差变异性测量与污染恐惧有微小但有统计学意义的关系(r = 0.133,p <0.05;r = 0.147,p <0.01)。这些注意偏差变异性测量结果还能在平均注意偏差测量结果(并控制反应时间变异性)之上解释污染恐惧的其他重大变异。这些发现为注意偏差变异性与污染恐惧之间的联系提供了初步证据,强调了认知偏差干预治疗临床污染恐惧的潜在目标。
{"title":"Contamination fear and attention bias variability early in the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Patrick J.F. Clarke ,&nbsp;Elise Szeremeta ,&nbsp;Bram Van Bockstaele ,&nbsp;Lies Notebaert ,&nbsp;Frances Meeten ,&nbsp;Jemma Todd","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104497","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104497","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic resulted in a dramatic increase in the salience and importance of information relating to both the risk of infection, and factors that could mitigate against such risk. This is likely to have contributed to elevated contamination fear concerns in the general population. Biased attention for contamination-related information has been proposed as a potential mechanism underlying contamination fear, though evidence regarding the presence of such biased attention has been inconsistent. A possible reason for this is that contamination fear may be characterised by variability in attention bias that has not yet been examined. The current study examined the potential association between attention bias variability for both contamination-related and mitigation-related stimuli, and contamination fear during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. A final sample of 315 participants completed measures of attention bias and contamination fear. The measure of average attention bias for contamination-related stimuli and mitigation-related stimuli was not associated with contamination fear (<em>r</em> = 0.055 and <em>r</em> = 0.051, <em>p</em> &gt; 0.10), though both attention bias variability measures did show a small but statistically significant relationship with contamination fear (<em>r</em> = 0.133, <em>p</em> &lt; 0.05; r = 0.147, <em>p</em> &lt; 0.01). These attention bias variability measures also accounted for significant additional variance in contamination fear above the average attention bias measure (and controlling for response time variability). These findings provide initial evidence for the association between attention bias variability and contamination fear, underscoring a potential target for cognitive bias interventions for clinical contamination fear.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"175 ","pages":"Article 104497"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S000579672400024X/pdfft?md5=6bc0d90941c8d2b1fd0343d6bbeef3a2&pid=1-s2.0-S000579672400024X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139993146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in individuals with chronic pain 慢性疼痛患者从巴甫洛夫到仪器的转移
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104491
Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos , Rachel Sjouwerman , Mathijs Teppers , Johan W.S. Vlaeyen

Avoidance of pain has been argued to be key factor leading pain events to chronic disability. In this respect, research has focused on investigating the working mechanisms of avoidance's acquisition. Avoidance of painful stimuli has been traditionally studied using a combination of Pavlovian and Instrumental procedures. However, such approach seems to go against real-life scenarios where avoidance is commonly acquired more readily. Using a novel pain avoidance paradigm, we tested whether pain avoidance can be installed in absence of associations between a cue and pain omission, and whether such avoidance differs between pain patients and healthy controls. Participants first learned to avoid painful stimuli by pressing a grip bar. Then, they passively encountered pairings of one geometrical shape with pain and of another geometrical shape without pain. Lastly, participants encountered the geometrical shapes while being able to use the grip bar. Results showed that participants pressed the bar more vigorously when encountering the previously pain-related shape compared to the pain-unrelated shape. This effect did not seem to differ between pain patients and healthy control. Our study could inspire a new way in measuring avoidance in pain, possibly paving the way to better understanding how avoidance is installed in chronic pain.

回避疼痛被认为是导致疼痛事件发展为慢性残疾的关键因素。在这方面,研究的重点是调查获得回避的工作机制。对疼痛刺激的回避,传统上采用巴甫洛夫法和工具法相结合的方法进行研究。然而,这种方法似乎与现实生活中更容易获得回避的场景相悖。我们使用了一种新颖的疼痛回避范式,测试了在线索和疼痛遗漏之间没有关联的情况下,是否可以建立疼痛回避,以及这种回避在疼痛患者和健康对照组之间是否存在差异。参与者首先通过按压握杆学会回避疼痛刺激。然后,他们被动地遇到一种有疼痛的几何形状和另一种无疼痛的几何形状的配对。最后,参与者在能够使用握杆的情况下接触几何图形。结果表明,与没有疼痛感的几何图形相比,参与者在遇到先前与疼痛有关的几何图形时会更用力地按压握杆。这种效果在疼痛患者和健康对照组之间似乎没有差异。我们的研究可以为测量疼痛中的回避现象提供一种新的方法,从而为更好地理解回避现象是如何在慢性疼痛中产生的铺平道路。
{"title":"Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in individuals with chronic pain","authors":"Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos ,&nbsp;Rachel Sjouwerman ,&nbsp;Mathijs Teppers ,&nbsp;Johan W.S. Vlaeyen","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104491","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Avoidance of pain has been argued to be key factor leading pain events to chronic disability. In this respect, research has focused on investigating the working mechanisms of avoidance's acquisition. Avoidance of painful stimuli has been traditionally studied using a combination of Pavlovian and Instrumental procedures. However, such approach seems to go against real-life scenarios where avoidance is commonly acquired more readily. Using a novel pain avoidance paradigm, we tested whether pain avoidance can be installed in absence of associations between a cue and pain omission, and whether such avoidance differs between pain patients and healthy controls. Participants first learned to avoid painful stimuli by pressing a grip bar. Then, they passively encountered pairings of one geometrical shape with pain and of another geometrical shape without pain. Lastly, participants encountered the geometrical shapes while being able to use the grip bar. Results showed that participants pressed the bar more vigorously when encountering the previously pain-related shape compared to the pain-unrelated shape. This effect did not seem to differ between pain patients and healthy control. Our study could inspire a new way in measuring avoidance in pain, possibly paving the way to better understanding how avoidance is installed in chronic pain.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"176 ","pages":"Article 104491"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000184/pdfft?md5=2850a84d5d5fc108f3f43ddb55608882&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000184-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139947988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behaviour Research and Therapy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1