首页 > 最新文献

Behaviour Research and Therapy最新文献

英文 中文
Does working memory training improve emotion regulation and reduce internalizing symptoms? A pair of three-level meta-analyses 工作记忆训练能改善情绪调节并减少内化症状吗?一对三级荟萃分析。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104549
Xiaobing Cui , Siyuan Zhang , Shuting Yu , Qingwen Ding , Xuebing Li

Background

Emotional dysfunction is a core feature of many mental disorders. Working memory training (WM-T) is promising to improve emotion regulation and reduce internalizing symptoms (anxiety and depressive symptoms), but the results are mixed. Therefore, we conducted meta-analyses to clarify these mixed results.

Methods

We searched Web of Science, PubMed, ScienceDirect, and EBSCO to identify relevant studies and screened the references. The effect size was calculated using Hedges’ g. Three-level, random-effects models were run using metafor in R.

Results

The current study included 44 articles, of which 29 were involved with emotion regulation, and 30 were involved with internalizing symptoms. The results showed that WM-T could yield emotional benefits, but the benefits were confined to enhancing explicit emotional regulation capacity and reducing anxiety symptoms. For the meta-analysis regarding the effect of WM-T on emotion regulation, there was no significant moderator. For the meta-analysis regarding the effect of WM-T on internalizing symptoms, the emotional valence of the material and control group were statistically significant moderators.

Conclusion

WM-T could yield certain emotional effects, but only to improve explicit emotion regulation capacity and reduce anxiety symptoms. In addition, some measures could enhance the effect, such as targeting specific populations, increasing the number of training sessions (≥15) or duration (>450 minutes), using negative material, and using n-back training tasks.

背景:情绪功能障碍是许多精神障碍的核心特征。工作记忆训练(WM-T)有望改善情绪调节并减轻内化症状(焦虑和抑郁症状),但研究结果不一。因此,我们进行了荟萃分析,以澄清这些混杂的结果:我们检索了 Web of Science、PubMed、ScienceDirect 和 EBSCO,以确定相关研究并筛选参考文献。使用 Hedges'g 计算效应大小,并使用 R 中的 metafor 运行三水平随机效应模型:本研究共纳入 44 篇文章,其中 29 篇涉及情绪调节,30 篇涉及内化症状。结果表明,WM-T 可以产生情绪方面的益处,但这种益处仅限于提高明确的情绪调节能力和减少焦虑症状。关于 WM-T 对情绪调节作用的荟萃分析,没有发现显著的调节因素。在有关 WM-T 对内化症状影响的荟萃分析中,材料和对照组的情绪价值在统计学上具有显著的调节作用:结论:WM-T 可以产生一定的情绪效应,但仅限于提高明确的情绪调节能力和减少焦虑症状。此外,一些措施可以增强效果,如针对特定人群、增加训练次数(≥15次)或持续时间(>450分钟)、使用负面材料和使用N-back训练任务。
{"title":"Does working memory training improve emotion regulation and reduce internalizing symptoms? A pair of three-level meta-analyses","authors":"Xiaobing Cui ,&nbsp;Siyuan Zhang ,&nbsp;Shuting Yu ,&nbsp;Qingwen Ding ,&nbsp;Xuebing Li","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104549","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104549","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Emotional dysfunction is a core feature of many mental disorders. Working memory training (WM-T) is promising to improve emotion regulation and reduce internalizing symptoms (anxiety and depressive symptoms), but the results are mixed. Therefore, we conducted meta-analyses to clarify these mixed results.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>We searched Web of Science, PubMed, ScienceDirect, and EBSCO to identify relevant studies and screened the references. The effect size was calculated using Hedges’ <em>g</em>. Three-level, random-effects models were run using metafor in R.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The current study included 44 articles, of which 29 were involved with emotion regulation, and 30 were involved with internalizing symptoms. The results showed that WM-T could yield emotional benefits, but the benefits were confined to enhancing explicit emotional regulation capacity and reducing anxiety symptoms. For the meta-analysis regarding the effect of WM-T on emotion regulation, there was no significant moderator. For the meta-analysis regarding the effect of WM-T on internalizing symptoms, the emotional valence of the material and control group were statistically significant moderators.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>WM-T could yield certain emotional effects, but only to improve explicit emotion regulation capacity and reduce anxiety symptoms. In addition, some measures could enhance the effect, such as targeting specific populations, increasing the number of training sessions (≥15) or duration (&gt;450 minutes), using negative material, and using n-back training tasks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"179 ","pages":"Article 104549"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
‘Nip it in the bud’: Low-frequency rTMS of the prefrontal cortex disrupts threat memory consolidation in humans 将威胁扼杀在萌芽状态对前额叶皮层进行低频经颅磁刺激可破坏人类的威胁记忆巩固
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104548
Simone Battaglia , Claudio Nazzi , Miquel A. Fullana , Giuseppe di Pellegrino , Sara Borgomaneri

It is still unclear how the human brain consolidates aversive (e.g., traumatic) memories and whether this process can be disrupted. We hypothesized that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is crucially involved in threat memory consolidation. To test this, we used low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (LF-rTMS) within the memory stabilization time window to disrupt the expression of threat memory. We combined a differential threat-conditioning paradigm with LF-rTMS targeting the dlPFC in the critical condition, and occipital cortex stimulation, delayed dlPFC stimulation, and sham stimulation as control conditions. In the critical condition, defensive reactions to threat were reduced immediately after brain stimulation, and 1 h and 24 h later. In stark contrast, no decrease was observed in the control conditions, thus showing both the anatomical and temporal specificity of our intervention. We provide causal evidence that selectively targeting the dlPFC within the early consolidation period prevents the persistence and return of conditioned responses. Furthermore, memory disruption lasted longer than the inhibitory window created by our TMS protocol, which suggests that we influenced dlPFC neural activity and hampered the underlying, time-dependent consolidation process. These results provide important insights for future clinical applications aimed at interfering with the consolidation of aversive, threat-related memories.

目前还不清楚人脑是如何巩固厌恶(如创伤)记忆的,也不清楚这一过程是否会被破坏。我们假设背外侧前额叶皮层(dlPFC)是威胁记忆巩固的关键。为了验证这一点,我们在记忆稳定时间窗内使用低频重复经颅磁刺激(LF-rTMS)来破坏威胁记忆的表达。我们将不同的威胁条件范式与针对dlPFC的LF-rTMS结合在一起,在临界状态下,枕叶皮层刺激、延迟dlPFC刺激和假刺激作为对照条件。在临界状态下,脑刺激后立即、1 小时后和 24 小时后,对威胁的防御反应都有所降低。与此形成鲜明对比的是,在对照组条件下没有观察到防御反应的减少,这表明我们的干预具有解剖学和时间上的特异性。我们提供的因果关系证据表明,在早期巩固期选择性地针对大脑前交叉皮质(dlPFC)进行干预可以防止条件反应的持续和恢复。此外,记忆中断持续的时间比我们的 TMS 方案所创建的抑制窗口更长,这表明我们影响了 dlPFC 的神经活动,阻碍了潜在的、随时间变化的巩固过程。这些结果为今后旨在干扰厌恶、威胁相关记忆巩固的临床应用提供了重要启示。
{"title":"‘Nip it in the bud’: Low-frequency rTMS of the prefrontal cortex disrupts threat memory consolidation in humans","authors":"Simone Battaglia ,&nbsp;Claudio Nazzi ,&nbsp;Miquel A. Fullana ,&nbsp;Giuseppe di Pellegrino ,&nbsp;Sara Borgomaneri","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104548","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104548","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It is still unclear how the human brain consolidates aversive (e.g., traumatic) memories and whether this process can be disrupted. We hypothesized that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is crucially involved in threat memory consolidation. To test this, we used low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (LF-rTMS) within the memory stabilization time window to disrupt the expression of threat memory. We combined a differential threat-conditioning paradigm with LF-rTMS targeting the dlPFC in the critical condition, and occipital cortex stimulation, delayed dlPFC stimulation, and sham stimulation as control conditions. In the critical condition, defensive reactions to threat were reduced immediately after brain stimulation, and 1 h and 24 h later. In stark contrast, no decrease was observed in the control conditions, thus showing both the anatomical and temporal specificity of our intervention. We provide causal evidence that selectively targeting the dlPFC within the early consolidation period prevents the persistence and return of conditioned responses. Furthermore, memory disruption lasted longer than the inhibitory window created by our TMS protocol, which suggests that we influenced dlPFC neural activity and hampered the underlying, time-dependent consolidation process. These results provide important insights for future clinical applications aimed at interfering with the consolidation of aversive, threat-related memories.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104548"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000755/pdfft?md5=d684a27be26a103c0cf0b24202920fa1&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000755-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140825319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Imagery rescripting and extinction: Effects on US expectancy, US revaluation, and the generalization of fear reduction 意象重描和消减:对美国预期、美国重估和恐惧减少的普遍性的影响
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104544
M. Woelk , M.A. Hagenaars , F. Raes , B. Vervliet , J. Krans

Exposure therapy consists of exposing patients to their fears and thereby diminishing their harm expectancies (i.e., extinction or expectancy learning). Although effective for many anxiety patients, its long-term success depends on the generalization of these harm expectancies to other stimuli. However, research shows that this generalization of extinction is limited. Besides decreasing harm expectancies, fear reduction may also be achieved by changing the meaning of an aversive memory representation (US revaluation). Imagery rescripting (ImRs) may be more successful in generalizing fear reduction because it allegedly works through US revaluation. The current experiment aimed to test working mechanisms for ImRs and extinction (revaluation and expectancy learning, respectively), and to examine generalization of fear reduction. In a fear conditioning paradigm, 113 healthy participants watched an aversive film clip that was used as the US. The manipulation consisted of imagining a script with a positive ending to the film clip (ImRs-only), extinction (extinction-only), or both (ImRs + extinction). Results showed enhanced US revaluation in ImRs + extinction. US expectancy decreased more strongly in the extinction conditions. Generalization of fear reduction was found in all conditions. Our results suggest different working mechanisms for ImRs and exposure. Future research should replicate this in (sub)clinical samples.

暴露疗法包括让患者暴露在他们的恐惧中,从而减少他们的伤害预期(即消退或预期学习)。虽然这种疗法对许多焦虑症患者有效,但其长期成功与否取决于能否将这些伤害预期泛化到其他刺激上。然而,研究表明,消退法的这种泛化是有限的。除了减少伤害预期,还可以通过改变厌恶记忆表征的意义(美国重估)来减少恐惧。意象重描(ImRs)在普遍化减少恐惧方面可能会更成功,因为它据称是通过美国重估来发挥作用的。目前的实验旨在测试 ImRs 和消退的工作机制(分别是重估和期望学习),并研究减少恐惧的普遍性。在恐惧条件反射范式中,113 名健康参与者观看了一段作为 US 的厌恶电影片段。操作包括想象电影片段结局积极的剧本(仅重估学习)、消退学习(仅消退学习)或两者兼而有之(重估学习+消退学习)。结果表明,在 "想象+消减 "中,美国重估增强。在消减条件下,US 期望值下降得更厉害。在所有条件下都发现了恐惧减少的普遍性。我们的研究结果表明,ImRs 和暴露有不同的工作机制。未来的研究应该在(亚)临床样本中重复这一结果。
{"title":"Imagery rescripting and extinction: Effects on US expectancy, US revaluation, and the generalization of fear reduction","authors":"M. Woelk ,&nbsp;M.A. Hagenaars ,&nbsp;F. Raes ,&nbsp;B. Vervliet ,&nbsp;J. Krans","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104544","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104544","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Exposure therapy consists of exposing patients to their fears and thereby diminishing their harm expectancies (i.e., extinction or expectancy learning). Although effective for many anxiety patients, its long-term success depends on the generalization of these harm expectancies to other stimuli. However, research shows that this generalization of extinction is limited. Besides decreasing harm expectancies, fear reduction may also be achieved by changing the meaning of an aversive memory representation (US revaluation). Imagery rescripting (ImRs) may be more successful in generalizing fear reduction because it allegedly works through US revaluation. The current experiment aimed to test working mechanisms for ImRs and extinction (revaluation and expectancy learning, respectively), and to examine generalization of fear reduction. In a fear conditioning paradigm, 113 healthy participants watched an aversive film clip that was used as the US. The manipulation consisted of imagining a script with a positive ending to the film clip (ImRs-only), extinction (extinction-only), or both (ImRs + extinction). Results showed enhanced US revaluation in ImRs + extinction. US expectancy decreased more strongly in the extinction conditions. Generalization of fear reduction was found in all conditions. Our results suggest different working mechanisms for ImRs and exposure. Future research should replicate this in (sub)clinical samples.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104544"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140825318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the ‘black box’ of anxiety: An ERP study of non-consciously triggered fear generalization 探索焦虑的 "黑匣子":对非有意识触发的恐惧泛化的 ERP 研究
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104552
Ying Mei , Benjamin Becker , Paavo H.T. Leppänen , Yi Lei

Individuals with anxiety disorders frequently display heightened fear responses, even in situations where there is no imminent danger. We hypothesize that these irrational fear responses are related to automatic processing of fear generalization. The initial automatic detection of stimuli often operates at a non-conscious level. However, whether fear generalization can occur when the cues are not perceived consciously remains unclear. The current study investigated the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying fear conditioning and its non-conscious and conscious generalization using a backward masking paradigm, combined with analysis of event-related potentials from electroencephalographic recordings. Behaviorally, participants showed heightened shock expectancy in response to non-conscious perceived generalization stimuli compared to those perceived consciously. Nonetheless, participants could not consciously distinguish between danger and safe cues in non-conscious trials. Physiologically, danger cues evoked larger frontal N1 amplitudes than safety cues in non-conscious trials, suggesting enhanced attention vigilance towards danger cues in the early sensory processing stage. Meanwhile, when fear generalization was conscious, it was accompanied by a larger P2 amplitude, indicating attention orientation or stimulus evaluation. In addition, fear conditioning was associated with sustained discrimination on P2, P3, and LPP. These findings collectively suggest that non-conscious fear generalization occurs at the neural level, yet additional control conditions are required to confirm this phenomenon on the US expectancy. Thus, non-consciously fear generalization may represent a mechanism that could trigger automatic irrational fear, highlighting the need for further research to explore therapeutic targets in anxiety disorders.

焦虑症患者经常表现出强烈的恐惧反应,即使在没有迫在眉睫的危险的情况下也是如此。我们假设这些非理性的恐惧反应与恐惧泛化的自动处理有关。最初对刺激的自动检测往往是在非意识层面上进行的。然而,当线索未被有意识地感知时,是否会发生恐惧泛化仍不清楚。本研究采用后向遮蔽范式,结合脑电记录的事件相关电位分析,研究了恐惧条件反射及其非意识和意识泛化的神经认知机制。从行为上看,与有意识感知的刺激相比,参与者对非有意识感知的泛化刺激表现出更高的电击预期。然而,在非有意识试验中,参与者无法有意识地区分危险和安全提示。从生理学角度来看,在非有意识试验中,危险线索比安全线索引起的额叶 N1 振幅更大,这表明在早期感觉处理阶段,参与者对危险线索的注意警惕性有所提高。同时,当恐惧泛化有意识时,伴随着更大的 P2 振幅,这表明了注意定向或刺激评估。此外,恐惧条件反射与 P2、P3 和 LPP 的持续辨别有关。这些发现共同表明,非有意识的恐惧泛化发生在神经水平上,但还需要额外的控制条件来证实这种现象对美国期望值的影响。因此,非有意识的恐惧泛化可能代表了一种引发自动非理性恐惧的机制,突出了进一步研究探索焦虑症治疗目标的必要性。
{"title":"Exploring the ‘black box’ of anxiety: An ERP study of non-consciously triggered fear generalization","authors":"Ying Mei ,&nbsp;Benjamin Becker ,&nbsp;Paavo H.T. Leppänen ,&nbsp;Yi Lei","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104552","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104552","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Individuals with anxiety disorders frequently display heightened fear responses, even in situations where there is no imminent danger. We hypothesize that these irrational fear responses are related to automatic processing of fear generalization. The initial automatic detection of stimuli often operates at a non-conscious level. However, whether fear generalization can occur when the cues are not perceived consciously remains unclear. The current study investigated the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying fear conditioning and its non-conscious and conscious generalization using a backward masking paradigm, combined with analysis of event-related potentials from electroencephalographic recordings. Behaviorally, participants showed heightened shock expectancy in response to non-conscious perceived generalization stimuli compared to those perceived consciously. Nonetheless, participants could not consciously distinguish between danger and safe cues in non-conscious trials. Physiologically, danger cues evoked larger frontal N1 amplitudes than safety cues in non-conscious trials, suggesting enhanced attention vigilance towards danger cues in the early sensory processing stage. Meanwhile, when fear generalization was conscious, it was accompanied by a larger P2 amplitude, indicating attention orientation or stimulus evaluation. In addition, fear conditioning was associated with sustained discrimination on P2, P3, and LPP. These findings collectively suggest that non-conscious fear generalization occurs at the neural level, yet additional control conditions are required to confirm this phenomenon on the US expectancy. Thus, non-consciously fear generalization may represent a mechanism that could trigger automatic irrational fear, highlighting the need for further research to explore therapeutic targets in anxiety disorders.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104552"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140844231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not just emotion regulation, but cognition: An experience sampling study testing the relations of ecological interpretation biases and use of emotion regulation strategies with momentary affective states during daily life functioning 不仅是情绪调节,还有认知一项经验取样研究,测试日常生活中生态解释偏差和情绪调节策略的使用与瞬间情绪状态之间的关系
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104550
Teresa Boemo , Oscar Martín-Garcia , Ana Mar Pacheco-Romero , Ivan Blanco , Ginette Lafit , Inez Myin-Germeys , Alvaro Sanchez-Lopez

Background

Current research is moving from studying cognitive biases and maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) as relatively stable phenomena contributing to affective disturbances, adopting ecological methodologies, such as Experience Sampling Methods (ESM). However, there is still limited ESM evidence on the interactions between stress and ER strategies’ use, and negative interpretation biases, regarding their relations with momentary affective states. In this study, we used a new ESM design to disentangle the contextual, regulatory and cognitive processes implicated in daily affective experiences.

Method

A sample of 103 participants completed an ESM study (3 times a day for 10 days) that included self-reports of momentary affect, stress intensity, ER strategies’ use and a cognitive task measuring momentary negative interpretation biases.

Results

Multilevel analyses supported significant interactions of both rumination and worry with stress intensity, to account for momentary higher negative and lower positive affect levels. Furthermore, higher state negative interpretation bias levels uniquely predicted both higher negative and lower positive momentary affect levels.

Conclusion

This study implemented a novel online cognitive task within an ESM procedure, which helped to disentangle how contextual ER strategies’ use and momentary cognitive biases uniquely relate to affective experiences in daily life.

背景目前的研究正从将认知偏差和不良情绪调节(ER)作为导致情绪紊乱的相对稳定的现象来研究,转向采用生态学方法,如经验取样法(ESM)。然而,关于压力和情绪调节策略的使用之间的相互作用,以及负面解释偏差与瞬间情绪状态之间的关系,ESM 的证据仍然有限。在这项研究中,我们采用了一种新的 ESM 设计,以厘清日常情感体验中的情境、调节和认知过程。方法:103 名参与者完成了一项 ESM 研究(每天 3 次,持续 10 天),其中包括对瞬间情感、压力强度、ER 策略使用情况的自我报告,以及一项测量瞬间负面解释偏差的认知任务。结果:多重分析支持反刍和担忧与压力强度之间的显著交互作用,以解释瞬间较高的负面和较低的正面情感水平。此外,较高的状态消极解释偏差水平可以独特地预测较高的消极和较低的瞬间积极情绪水平。结论本研究在ESM程序中实施了一项新颖的在线认知任务,有助于厘清情境性应急反应策略的使用和瞬间认知偏差如何与日常生活中的情绪体验独特地联系在一起。
{"title":"Not just emotion regulation, but cognition: An experience sampling study testing the relations of ecological interpretation biases and use of emotion regulation strategies with momentary affective states during daily life functioning","authors":"Teresa Boemo ,&nbsp;Oscar Martín-Garcia ,&nbsp;Ana Mar Pacheco-Romero ,&nbsp;Ivan Blanco ,&nbsp;Ginette Lafit ,&nbsp;Inez Myin-Germeys ,&nbsp;Alvaro Sanchez-Lopez","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104550","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104550","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Current research is moving from studying cognitive biases and maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) as relatively stable phenomena contributing to affective disturbances, adopting ecological methodologies, such as Experience Sampling Methods (ESM). However, there is still limited ESM evidence on the interactions between stress and ER strategies’ use, and negative interpretation biases, regarding their relations with momentary affective states. In this study, we used a new ESM design to disentangle the contextual, regulatory and cognitive processes implicated in daily affective experiences.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>A sample of 103 participants completed an ESM study (3 times a day for 10 days) that included self-reports of momentary affect, stress intensity, ER strategies’ use and a cognitive task measuring momentary negative interpretation biases.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Multilevel analyses supported significant interactions of both rumination and worry with stress intensity, to account for momentary higher negative and lower positive affect levels. Furthermore, higher state negative interpretation bias levels uniquely predicted both higher negative and lower positive momentary affect levels.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>This study implemented a novel online cognitive task within an ESM procedure, which helped to disentangle how contextual ER strategies’ use and momentary cognitive biases uniquely relate to affective experiences in daily life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"177 ","pages":"Article 104550"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140785302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Amplification of positivity for depression and anxiety: Neural prediction of treatment response 积极情绪对抑郁和焦虑的放大作用:治疗反应的神经预测
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104545
Maria Kryza-Lacombe , Isabella Spaulding , Cheuk King Ku , Nana Pearson , Murray B. Stein , Charles T. Taylor

Psychosocial treatments targeting the positive valence system (PVS) in depression and anxiety demonstrate efficacy in enhancing positive affect (PA), but response to treatment varies. We examined whether individual differences in neural activation to positive and negative valence incentive cues underlies differences in benefitting from a PVS-targeted treatment. Individuals with clinically elevated depression and/or anxiety (N = 88, ages 18 to 55) participated in one of two randomized, waitlist-controlled trials of Amplification of Positivity (AMP; NCT02330627, NCT03196544), a cognitive and behavioral intervention targeting the PVS. Participants completed a monetary incentive delay (MID) task during fMRI acquisition at baseline measuring neural activation to the possibility of gaining or losing money. Change in PA from before to after treatment was assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. No significant associations were observed between baseline neural activation during gain anticipation and AMP-related changes in PA in regions of interest (striatum and insula) or whole-brain analyses. However, higher baseline striatal and insula activation during loss anticipation was associated with greater increases in PA post-AMP. This study provides preliminary evidence suggesting neural reactivity to negative valence cues may inform who stands to benefit most from treatments targeting the PVS.

针对抑郁症和焦虑症患者积极情绪系统(PVS)的社会心理治疗在增强积极情绪(PA)方面具有疗效,但治疗反应却各不相同。我们研究了神经激活对积极和消极情绪激励线索的个体差异是否是从以积极情绪系统为目标的治疗中获益的差异的基础。抑郁症和/或焦虑症患者(88 人,18 至 55 岁)参加了 "积极性放大"(AMP;NCT02330627、NCT03196544)两项随机、候选对照试验中的一项。参试者在基线时的 fMRI 采集过程中完成一项货币激励延迟(MID)任务,测量神经对获得或失去金钱的可能性的激活情况。使用积极和消极情绪表评估治疗前和治疗后 PA 的变化。在相关区域(纹状体和脑岛)或全脑分析中,未观察到收益预期期间的基线神经激活与 AMP 相关 PA 变化之间存在明显关联。然而,损失预期时纹状体和脑岛的基线激活较高与AMP后PA的增加有关。这项研究提供的初步证据表明,神经对负面情绪线索的反应可能会告知哪些人从针对PVS的治疗中获益最多。
{"title":"Amplification of positivity for depression and anxiety: Neural prediction of treatment response","authors":"Maria Kryza-Lacombe ,&nbsp;Isabella Spaulding ,&nbsp;Cheuk King Ku ,&nbsp;Nana Pearson ,&nbsp;Murray B. Stein ,&nbsp;Charles T. Taylor","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104545","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104545","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Psychosocial treatments targeting the positive valence system (PVS) in depression and anxiety demonstrate efficacy in enhancing positive affect (PA), but response to treatment varies. We examined whether individual differences in neural activation to positive and negative valence incentive cues underlies differences in benefitting from a PVS-targeted treatment. Individuals with clinically elevated depression and/or anxiety (N = 88, ages 18 to 55) participated in one of two randomized, waitlist-controlled trials of Amplification of Positivity (AMP; NCT02330627, NCT03196544), a cognitive and behavioral intervention targeting the PVS. Participants completed a monetary incentive delay (MID) task during fMRI acquisition at baseline measuring neural activation to the possibility of gaining or losing money. Change in PA from before to after treatment was assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. No significant associations were observed between baseline neural activation during gain anticipation and AMP-related changes in PA in regions of interest (striatum and insula) or whole-brain analyses. However, higher baseline striatal and insula activation during loss anticipation was associated with greater increases in PA post-AMP. This study provides preliminary evidence suggesting neural reactivity to negative valence cues may inform who stands to benefit most from treatments targeting the PVS.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104545"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140772602","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can an intervention designed to reduce repetitive negative thinking alter the response to a psychosocial stressor? A randomized controlled study 旨在减少重复负面思维的干预措施能否改变对社会心理压力的反应?随机对照研究
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104547
Julia Funk , Keisuke Takano , Marina Babl , Rebecca Goldstein , Regina Oberwestersberger , Johannes Kopf-Beck , Nicolas Rohleder , Thomas Ehring

Prior research suggests that repetitive negative thinking (RNT) negatively impacts mental health by intensifying and prolonging emotional reactivity to stress. This study investigated whether an intervention designed to reduce RNT alters emotional reactivity.

Young adults with high trait RNT (N = 79) were randomly allocated to an RNT-focused intervention (smartphone app-based, 10 days) or a waiting list before exposure to a standardized stressor.

The pre-registered analysis did not reveal a significant condition * time interaction for negative affect. However, exploratory analyses showed that whilst initial increases in negative affect in response to the stressor did not differ between conditions, participants in the intervention condition reported less negative affect throughout the following recovery phase. Additionally, participants in the intervention condition appraised their ability to cope with the stressor as higher and reported less RNT in the recovery phase. In contrast, the intervention did not affect biological stress responses.

The findings indicate that RNT-focused interventions might have positive effects on mental health by breaking the self-reinforcing cycle of RNT, negative affect and maladaptive appraisals in response to stress. However, as findings are partly based on exploratory analyses, further research is needed to confirm whether reduced subjective stress reactivity mediates the effects of RNT-focused interventions on psychopathological symptoms.

先前的研究表明,重复性消极思维(RNT)会加剧和延长对压力的情绪反应,从而对心理健康产生负面影响。本研究调查了旨在减少RNT的干预措施是否会改变情绪反应性。在暴露于标准化压力源之前,具有高特质RNT的年轻人(N = 79)被随机分配到以RNT为重点的干预措施(基于智能手机应用程序,为期10天)或等待名单中。然而,探索性分析表明,虽然不同干预条件下的参与者最初对压力源的负面情绪增加并无差异,但在随后的恢复阶段,干预条件下的参与者报告的负面情绪较少。此外,干预条件下的参与者对自己应对压力源的能力评价较高,在恢复阶段报告的 RNT 也较少。研究结果表明,以 RNT 为重点的干预措施可能会打破 RNT、负面情绪和应对压力的不良评估的自我强化循环,从而对心理健康产生积极影响。然而,由于研究结果部分是基于探索性分析,因此还需要进一步的研究来证实主观压力反应性的降低是否介导了以 RNT 为重点的干预措施对精神病理症状的影响。
{"title":"Can an intervention designed to reduce repetitive negative thinking alter the response to a psychosocial stressor? A randomized controlled study","authors":"Julia Funk ,&nbsp;Keisuke Takano ,&nbsp;Marina Babl ,&nbsp;Rebecca Goldstein ,&nbsp;Regina Oberwestersberger ,&nbsp;Johannes Kopf-Beck ,&nbsp;Nicolas Rohleder ,&nbsp;Thomas Ehring","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104547","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104547","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prior research suggests that repetitive negative thinking (RNT) negatively impacts mental health by intensifying and prolonging emotional reactivity to stress. This study investigated whether an intervention designed to reduce RNT alters emotional reactivity.</p><p>Young adults with high trait RNT (<em>N</em> = 79) were randomly allocated to an RNT-focused intervention (smartphone app-based, 10 days) or a waiting list before exposure to a standardized stressor.</p><p>The pre-registered analysis did not reveal a significant condition * time interaction for negative affect. However, exploratory analyses showed that whilst initial increases in negative affect in response to the stressor did not differ between conditions, participants in the intervention condition reported less negative affect throughout the following recovery phase. Additionally, participants in the intervention condition appraised their ability to cope with the stressor as higher and reported less RNT in the recovery phase. In contrast, the intervention did not affect biological stress responses.</p><p>The findings indicate that RNT-focused interventions might have positive effects on mental health by breaking the self-reinforcing cycle of RNT, negative affect and maladaptive appraisals in response to stress. However, as findings are partly based on exploratory analyses, further research is needed to confirm whether reduced subjective stress reactivity mediates the effects of RNT-focused interventions on psychopathological symptoms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104547"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000743/pdfft?md5=2b8ac514c9f44749311f51ad9e64761d&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000743-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140650804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shared and distinct effect mediators in exposure-based and traditional cognitive behavior therapy for fibromyalgia: Secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial 暴露疗法和传统认知行为疗法治疗纤维肌痛的共同效应和不同效应中介:随机对照试验的二次分析
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104546
Maria Hedman-Lagerlöf , Monica Buhrman , Erik Hedman-Lagerlöf , Brjánn Ljótsson , Erland Axelsson

Fibromyalgia is a chronic pain condition associated with substantial suffering and societal costs. Traditional cognitive behavior therapy (T-CBT) is the most evaluated psychological treatment, but exposure therapy (Exp-CBT) has shown promise with a pronounced focus on the reduction of pain-related avoidance behaviors. In a recent randomized controlled trial (N = 274), we found that Exp-CBT was not superior to T-CBT (d = −0.10) in reducing overall fibromyalgia severity. This study investigated pain-related avoidance behaviors, pain catastrophizing, hypervigilance, pacing, overdoing and physical activity as potential mediators of the treatment effect. Mediation analyses were based on parallel process growth models fitted on 11 weekly measurement points, and week-by-week time-lagged effects were tested using random intercepts cross-lagged panel models. Results indicated that a reduction in avoidance behaviors, pain catastrophizing, and hypervigilance were significant mediators of change in both treatments. An increase in pacing and a reduction in overdoing were significant mediators in T-CBT only. Physical activity was not a mediator. In the time-lagged analyses, an unequivocal effect on subsequent fibromyalgia severity was seen of avoidance and catastrophizing in Exp-CBT, and of overdoing in T-CBT. Exposure-based and traditional CBT for fibromyalgia appear to share common treatment mediators, namely pain-related avoidance behavior, catastrophizing and hypervigilance.

纤维肌痛是一种慢性疼痛,会带来巨大的痛苦和社会成本。传统的认知行为疗法(T-CBT)是最受好评的心理治疗方法,但暴露疗法(Exp-CBT)在减少与疼痛相关的回避行为方面有明显优势。在最近的一项随机对照试验(N = 274)中,我们发现暴露疗法(Exp-CBT)在减轻纤维肌痛的总体严重程度方面并不优于暴露疗法(d = -0.10)。本研究调查了与疼痛相关的回避行为、疼痛灾难化、过度警觉、踱步、过度运动和体育活动作为治疗效果的潜在中介。中介分析基于 11 个每周测量点的平行过程增长模型,并使用随机截距交叉滞后面板模型检验了逐周时间滞后效应。结果表明,回避行为、疼痛灾难化和过度警觉的减少对两种治疗方法的变化都有显著的中介作用。仅在 T-CBT 治疗中,步调的增加和过度行为的减少具有显著的中介作用。体育锻炼不是中介因素。在时滞分析中,Exp-CBT疗法中的回避和灾难化以及T-CBT疗法中的过度行为对纤维肌痛随后的严重程度有明确的影响。暴露疗法和传统的纤维肌痛 CBT 似乎有共同的治疗介导因素,即与疼痛相关的回避行为、灾难化和过度警觉。
{"title":"Shared and distinct effect mediators in exposure-based and traditional cognitive behavior therapy for fibromyalgia: Secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial","authors":"Maria Hedman-Lagerlöf ,&nbsp;Monica Buhrman ,&nbsp;Erik Hedman-Lagerlöf ,&nbsp;Brjánn Ljótsson ,&nbsp;Erland Axelsson","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104546","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104546","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Fibromyalgia is a chronic pain condition associated with substantial suffering and societal costs. Traditional cognitive behavior therapy (T-CBT) is the most evaluated psychological treatment, but exposure therapy (Exp-CBT) has shown promise with a pronounced focus on the reduction of pain-related avoidance behaviors. In a recent randomized controlled trial (<em>N</em> = 274), we found that Exp-CBT was not superior to T-CBT (<em>d</em> = −0.10) in reducing overall fibromyalgia severity. This study investigated pain-related avoidance behaviors, pain catastrophizing, hypervigilance, pacing, overdoing and physical activity as potential mediators of the treatment effect. Mediation analyses were based on parallel process growth models fitted on 11 weekly measurement points, and week-by-week time-lagged effects were tested using random intercepts cross-lagged panel models. Results indicated that a reduction in avoidance behaviors, pain catastrophizing, and hypervigilance were significant mediators of change in both treatments. An increase in pacing and a reduction in overdoing were significant mediators in T-CBT only. Physical activity was not a mediator. In the time-lagged analyses, an unequivocal effect on subsequent fibromyalgia severity was seen of avoidance and catastrophizing in Exp-CBT, and of overdoing in T-CBT. Exposure-based and traditional CBT for fibromyalgia appear to share common treatment mediators, namely pain-related avoidance behavior, catastrophizing and hypervigilance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104546"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796724000731/pdfft?md5=785c65d83d6d890973d77dd3205bbae5&pid=1-s2.0-S0005796724000731-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140763140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Threat sensitivity in emotion dynamics: Negativity effects and sex differences 情绪动态中的威胁敏感性:消极效应和性别差异
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104542
Michael D. Robinson, Roberta L. Irvin, Muhammad R. Asad

Evolutionary theorizing has given rise to the idea that responding to any particular threat may be more mandatory than responding to any particular reward. The present three experiments (total N = 375) sought to provide support for this perspective in an emotion dynamics task in which participants continuously rated their affective state in response to appetitive (reward-related) versus aversive (threat-related) images. Even when equating images for arousal and extremity, several negativity effects (e.g., steeper reactivity slopes in response to aversive images) were found. These negativity effects can serve as an experimental model of threat sensitivity, which should predispose some individuals, more than others, to symptoms related to fear and anxiety. This point was made with respect to sex differences, given that women (relative to men) are diagnosed with anxiety disorders at higher rates. Sex differences were pronounced and extensions of this work, both basic and applied, are proposed.

进化理论认为,对任何特定威胁做出反应可能比对任何特定奖励做出反应更具强制性。本实验的三个参与者(总人数= 375)试图在一项情绪动力学任务中为这一观点提供支持,在这项任务中,参与者会持续评定他们对开胃(与奖励相关)和厌恶(与威胁相关)图像的情绪状态。即使将图像等同于唤醒性和极端性,也发现了一些负性效应(例如,对厌恶性图像的反应斜率更陡)。这些负性效应可以作为威胁敏感性的实验模型,使某些人比其他人更容易出现与恐惧和焦虑有关的症状。鉴于女性(相对于男性)被诊断出患有焦虑症的比例更高,这一点与性别差异有关。性别差异是显而易见的,并提出了这一工作在基础和应用两方面的延伸。
{"title":"Threat sensitivity in emotion dynamics: Negativity effects and sex differences","authors":"Michael D. Robinson,&nbsp;Roberta L. Irvin,&nbsp;Muhammad R. Asad","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104542","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104542","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Evolutionary theorizing has given rise to the idea that responding to any particular threat may be more mandatory than responding to any particular reward. The present three experiments (total <em>N</em> = 375) sought to provide support for this perspective in an emotion dynamics task in which participants continuously rated their affective state in response to appetitive (reward-related) versus aversive (threat-related) images. Even when equating images for arousal and extremity, several negativity effects (e.g., steeper reactivity slopes in response to aversive images) were found. These negativity effects can serve as an experimental model of threat sensitivity, which should predispose some individuals, more than others, to symptoms related to fear and anxiety. This point was made with respect to sex differences, given that women (relative to men) are diagnosed with anxiety disorders at higher rates. Sex differences were pronounced and extensions of this work, both basic and applied, are proposed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"178 ","pages":"Article 104542"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140638627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Savoring changes novel positive mindset targets of GAD treatment: Optimism, prioritizing positivity, kill-joy thinking, and worry mediation 品味改变 GAD 治疗的新的积极心态目标:乐观、积极优先、杀戮思维和忧虑调解
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, CLINICAL Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104541
Lucas S. LaFreniere , Michelle G. Newman

This study analyzed effects of savoring on unstudied positive mindset targets of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) treatment (ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT05040061). 85 students with GAD were randomly assigned to one of two ecological momentary interventions (EMIs) on smartphone for seven days. The SkillJoy EMI promoted practices for savoring positive emotions. An active control EMI mirrored SkillJoy, yet did not include savoring or positive emotion. Optimism, worry, kill-joy thinking (lessening positive emotion with cognition), and prioritization of positive emotion activities and goals were assessed at pre-trial, eighth-day, post-trial, and 30th-day follow-up. Savoring was assessed pre-trial and fifth-day mid-trial. Longitudinal linear mixed models and simple slope analyses examined change between and within conditions. Bias-corrected bootstrapping path analysis examined mediation of worry change by increased savoring. SkillJoy led to significantly greater increases in both optimism and prioritizing positivity than the control from pre-trial to post-trial and pre-trial to follow-up. Both interventions significantly reduced kill-joy thinking at both timepoints with Skilljoy leading to marginally greater change at post-trial. Pre- to mid-trial increases in savoring mediated the relationships between treatment condition and reductions in worry at both post-trial and follow-up.

本研究分析了品味对广泛性焦虑症(GAD)治疗中尚未研究的积极心态目标的影响(ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT05040061)。85 名患有 GAD 的学生被随机分配到智能手机上的两种生态瞬间干预(EMI)中的一种,为期七天。SkillJoy EMI促进了品味积极情绪的实践。主动对照 EMI 与 SkillJoy 类似,但不包括品味或积极情绪。在试验前、试验第八天、试验后和试验后 30 天的随访中,对乐观、担忧、消沉思维(通过认知减少积极情绪)以及积极情绪活动和目标的优先级进行了评估。在试验前和试验中期的第五天对品味进行了评估。纵向线性混合模型和简单斜率分析检验了条件之间和条件之内的变化。经过偏差校正的引导路径分析检验了忧虑变化对品味增加的中介作用。从试验前到试验后,以及从试验前到随访,与对照组相比,SkillJoy 所带来的乐观情绪和积极优先的增加幅度都明显更大。在两个时间点上,两种干预措施都明显减少了 "杀戮快乐 "的想法,而 Skilljoy 在试验后的变化略大于对照组。从试验前到试验中期,"品味 "的增加在治疗条件与试验后和随访中的担忧减少之间起到了中介作用。
{"title":"Savoring changes novel positive mindset targets of GAD treatment: Optimism, prioritizing positivity, kill-joy thinking, and worry mediation","authors":"Lucas S. LaFreniere ,&nbsp;Michelle G. Newman","doi":"10.1016/j.brat.2024.104541","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104541","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study analyzed effects of savoring on unstudied positive mindset targets of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) treatment (ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT05040061). 85 students with GAD were randomly assigned to one of two ecological momentary interventions (EMIs) on smartphone for seven days. The SkillJoy EMI promoted practices for savoring positive emotions. An active control EMI mirrored SkillJoy, yet did not include savoring or positive emotion. Optimism, worry, kill-joy thinking (lessening positive emotion with cognition), and prioritization of positive emotion activities and goals were assessed at pre-trial, eighth-day, post-trial, and 30<sup>th</sup>-day follow-up. Savoring was assessed pre-trial and fifth-day mid-trial. Longitudinal linear mixed models and simple slope analyses examined change between and within conditions. Bias-corrected bootstrapping path analysis examined mediation of worry change by increased savoring. SkillJoy led to significantly greater increases in both optimism and prioritizing positivity than the control from pre-trial to post-trial and pre-trial to follow-up. Both interventions significantly reduced kill-joy thinking at both timepoints with Skilljoy leading to marginally greater change at post-trial. Pre- to mid-trial increases in savoring mediated the relationships between treatment condition and reductions in worry at both post-trial and follow-up.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48457,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour Research and Therapy","volume":"177 ","pages":"Article 104541"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140618515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behaviour Research and Therapy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1