首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Vision最新文献

英文 中文
The visual statistical learning overcomes scene dissimilarity through an independent clustering process. 视觉统计学习通过独立的聚类过程克服了场景的不相似性。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.5
Xiaoyu Chen, Jie Wang, Qiang Liu

Contextual cueing is a phenomenon of visual statistical learning observed in visual search tasks. Previous research has found that the degree of deviation of items from its centroid, known as variability, determines the extent of generalization for that repeated scene. Introducing variability increases dissimilarity between multiple occurrences of the same repeated layout significantly. However, current theories do not explain the mechanisms that help to overcome this dissimilarity during contextual cue learning. We propose that the cognitive system initially abstracts specific scenes into scene layouts through an automatic clustering unrelated to specific repeated scenes, and subsequently uses these abstracted scene layouts for contextual cue learning. Experiment 1 indicates that introducing greater variability in search scenes leads to a hindering in the contextual cue learning. Experiment 2 further establishes that conducting extensive visual searches involving spatial variability in entirely novel scenes facilitates subsequent contextual cue learning involving corresponding scene variability, confirming that learning clustering knowledge precedes the contextual cue learning and is independent of specific repeated scenes. Overall, this study demonstrates the existence of multiple levels of learning in visual statistical learning, where item-level learning can serve as material for layout-level learning, and the generalization reflects the constraining role of item-level knowledge on layout-level knowledge.

情境提示是在视觉搜索任务中观察到的一种视觉统计学习现象。以往的研究发现,项目偏离其中心点的程度(称为变异性)决定了该重复场景的泛化程度。引入变异性会显著增加同一重复布局的多次出现之间的不相似性。然而,目前的理论并不能解释在情境线索学习过程中帮助克服这种不相似性的机制。我们提出,认知系统最初是通过与特定重复场景无关的自动聚类将特定场景抽象为场景布局,然后利用这些抽象场景布局进行情境线索学习。实验 1 表明,在搜索场景中引入更大的可变性会阻碍情境线索学习。实验 2 进一步证实,在完全新颖的场景中进行涉及空间变化的广泛视觉搜索,会促进随后涉及相应场景变化的情境线索学习,从而证实聚类知识的学习先于情境线索学习,并且与具体的重复场景无关。总之,本研究证明了视觉统计学习中存在多层次的学习,其中项目层次的学习可以作为布局层次学习的材料,而泛化则反映了项目层次的知识对布局层次知识的制约作用。
{"title":"The visual statistical learning overcomes scene dissimilarity through an independent clustering process.","authors":"Xiaoyu Chen, Jie Wang, Qiang Liu","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Contextual cueing is a phenomenon of visual statistical learning observed in visual search tasks. Previous research has found that the degree of deviation of items from its centroid, known as variability, determines the extent of generalization for that repeated scene. Introducing variability increases dissimilarity between multiple occurrences of the same repeated layout significantly. However, current theories do not explain the mechanisms that help to overcome this dissimilarity during contextual cue learning. We propose that the cognitive system initially abstracts specific scenes into scene layouts through an automatic clustering unrelated to specific repeated scenes, and subsequently uses these abstracted scene layouts for contextual cue learning. Experiment 1 indicates that introducing greater variability in search scenes leads to a hindering in the contextual cue learning. Experiment 2 further establishes that conducting extensive visual searches involving spatial variability in entirely novel scenes facilitates subsequent contextual cue learning involving corresponding scene variability, confirming that learning clustering knowledge precedes the contextual cue learning and is independent of specific repeated scenes. Overall, this study demonstrates the existence of multiple levels of learning in visual statistical learning, where item-level learning can serve as material for layout-level learning, and the generalization reflects the constraining role of item-level knowledge on layout-level knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"5"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11314707/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flicker adaptation improves acuity for briefly presented stimuli by reducing crowding. 闪烁适应可通过减少拥挤来提高短暂刺激的敏锐度。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.15
Selassie Tagoh, Lisa M Hamm, Dietrich S Schwarzkopf, Steven C Dakin

Adaptation to flickering/dynamic noise improves visual acuity for briefly presented stimuli (Arnold et al., 2016). Here, we investigate whether such adaptation operates directly on our ability to see detail or by changing fixational eye movements and pupil size or by reducing visual crowding. Following earlier work, visual acuity was measured in observers who were either unadapted or who had adapted to a 60-Hz flickering noise pattern. Participants reported the orientation of a white tumbling-T target (four-alternative forced choice [4AFC], ⊤⊣⊥⊢). The target was presented for 110 ms either in isolation or flanked by randomly oriented T's (e.g., ⊣⊤⊢) followed by an isolated (+) or flanked (+++) mask, respectively. We measured fixation stability (using an infrared eye tracker) while observers performed the task (with and without adaptation). Visual acuity improved modestly (around 8.4%) for flanked optotypes following adaptation to flicker (mean, -0.038 ± 0.063 logMAR; p = 0.015; BF10 = 3.66) but did not when measured with isolated letters (mean, -0.008 ± 0.055 logMAR; p = 0.5; BF10 = 0.29). The magnitude of acuity improvement was associated with individuals' (unadapted) susceptibility to crowding (the ratio of crowded to uncrowded acuity; r = -0.58, p = 0.008, BF10 = 7.70) but to neither fixation stability nor pupil size. Confirming previous reports, flicker improved acuity for briefly presented stimuli, but we show that this was only the case for crowded letters. These improvements likely arise from attenuation of sensitivity to a transient low spatial frequency (SF) image structure (Arnold et al., 2016; Tagoh et al., 2022), which may, for example, reduce masking of high SFs by low SFs. We also suggest that this attenuation could reduce backward masking and so reduce foveal crowding.

对闪烁/动态噪音的适应可以提高短暂刺激的视觉敏锐度(Arnold 等人,2016 年)。在此,我们将研究这种适应是否直接作用于我们观察细节的能力,或通过改变固定眼球运动和瞳孔大小或减少视觉拥挤来实现。根据之前的研究,我们测量了未适应或已适应 60Hz 闪烁噪声模式的观察者的视觉敏锐度。受试者报告一个白色翻滚-T 目标的方向(四选一强迫选择 [4AFC],⊤⊣⊥⊢)。目标在 110 毫秒的时间内要么单独出现,要么在两侧出现随机方向的 T(例如⊣⊤⊢),然后分别出现一个单独(+)或两侧(+++)的掩码。我们测量了观察者在完成任务(有适应和无适应)时的固定稳定性(使用红外眼动仪)。在适应闪烁后,侧面光型的视敏度略有提高(约为 8.4%)(平均值为 -0.038 ± 0.063 logMAR;p = 0.015;BF10 = 3.66),但用孤立字母测量时视敏度没有提高(平均值为 -0.008 ± 0.055 logMAR;p = 0.5;BF10 = 0.29)。敏锐度提高的幅度与个体(未适应的)对拥挤的敏感性(拥挤与非拥挤敏锐度之比;r = -0.58,p = 0.008,BF10 = 7.70)有关,但与定点稳定性和瞳孔大小无关。与之前的报告相印证的是,闪烁提高了短暂呈现刺激的敏锐度,但我们的研究表明,这只适用于拥挤的字母。这些改善可能源于对瞬时低空间频率(SF)图像结构敏感性的衰减(Arnold 等人,2016 年;Tagoh 等人,2022 年),例如,这可能会减少低 SF 对高 SF 的掩蔽。我们还认为,这种衰减可能会减少后向遮蔽,从而减少眼窝拥挤。
{"title":"Flicker adaptation improves acuity for briefly presented stimuli by reducing crowding.","authors":"Selassie Tagoh, Lisa M Hamm, Dietrich S Schwarzkopf, Steven C Dakin","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.15","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.15","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adaptation to flickering/dynamic noise improves visual acuity for briefly presented stimuli (Arnold et al., 2016). Here, we investigate whether such adaptation operates directly on our ability to see detail or by changing fixational eye movements and pupil size or by reducing visual crowding. Following earlier work, visual acuity was measured in observers who were either unadapted or who had adapted to a 60-Hz flickering noise pattern. Participants reported the orientation of a white tumbling-T target (four-alternative forced choice [4AFC], ⊤⊣⊥⊢). The target was presented for 110 ms either in isolation or flanked by randomly oriented T's (e.g., ⊣⊤⊢) followed by an isolated (+) or flanked (+++) mask, respectively. We measured fixation stability (using an infrared eye tracker) while observers performed the task (with and without adaptation). Visual acuity improved modestly (around 8.4%) for flanked optotypes following adaptation to flicker (mean, -0.038 ± 0.063 logMAR; p = 0.015; BF10 = 3.66) but did not when measured with isolated letters (mean, -0.008 ± 0.055 logMAR; p = 0.5; BF10 = 0.29). The magnitude of acuity improvement was associated with individuals' (unadapted) susceptibility to crowding (the ratio of crowded to uncrowded acuity; r = -0.58, p = 0.008, BF10 = 7.70) but to neither fixation stability nor pupil size. Confirming previous reports, flicker improved acuity for briefly presented stimuli, but we show that this was only the case for crowded letters. These improvements likely arise from attenuation of sensitivity to a transient low spatial frequency (SF) image structure (Arnold et al., 2016; Tagoh et al., 2022), which may, for example, reduce masking of high SFs by low SFs. We also suggest that this attenuation could reduce backward masking and so reduce foveal crowding.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11364176/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142082392","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perception of #TheDress in childhood is influenced by age and green-leaf preference. 童年时期对 #TheDress 的认知受年龄和绿叶偏好的影响。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.11
Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva, Catalina Becerra-Revollo, Luis Antonio Rhoads-Avila, Julian García-Sánchez, Flor Angélica Jácome-Gutierrez, Linda Cernichiaro-Espinosa, Andrée Henaine-Berra, Axel Orozco-Hernandez, Humberto Ruiz-García, Eduardo Torres-Porras

The perception of the ambiguous image of #TheDress may be influenced by optical factors, such as macular pigments. Their accumulation during childhood could increase with age and the ingestion of carotenoid-containing foods. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether the visual perception of the dress in children would differ based on age and carotenoid preference. This was a cross-sectional, observational, and comparative study. A poll was administered to children aged 2 to 10 years. Parents were instructed to inquire about the color of #TheDress from their children. A carotenoid preference survey was also completed. A total of 413 poll responses were analyzed. Responses were categorized based on the perceived color of the dress: blue/black (BB) (n = 204) and white/gold (WG) (n = 209). The mean and median age of the WG group was higher than the BB group (mean 6.1, median 6.0 years, standard deviation [SD] 2.2; mean 5.5, median 5.0 years, SD 2.3; p = 0.007). Spearman correlation between age and group was 0.133 (p = 0.007). Green-leaf preference (GLP) showed a statistically significant difference between groups (Mann-Whitney U: p = 0.038). Spearman correlation between GLP and group was 0.102 (p = 0.037). Logistic regression for the perception of the dress as WG indicated that age and GLP were significant predictors (age: B weight 0.109, p = 0.012, odds ratio: 1.115; GLP: B weight 0.317, p = 0.033, odds ratio: 1.373). Older children and those with a higher GLP were more likely to perceive #TheDress as WG. These results suggest a potential relationship with the gradual accumulation of macular pigments throughout a child's lifetime.

对#TheDress模糊图像的感知可能会受到黄斑色素等光学因素的影响。它们在儿童时期的积累可能会随着年龄的增长和摄入含类胡萝卜素食物的增加而增加。本研究旨在探讨儿童对裙子的视觉感知是否会因年龄和类胡萝卜素偏好而有所不同。这是一项横断面观察比较研究。研究人员对 2 至 10 岁的儿童进行了民意测验。指导家长向孩子询问 #TheDress 的颜色。同时还完成了类胡萝卜素偏好调查。共分析了 413 份投票回复。根据孩子对裙子颜色的看法,对回答进行了分类:蓝色/黑色(BB)(n = 204)和白色/金色(WG)(n = 209)。WG 组的平均年龄和中位年龄均高于 BB 组(平均 6.1 岁,中位 6.0 岁,标准差 [SD] 2.2;平均 5.5 岁,中位 5.0 岁,标准差 2.3;P = 0.007)。年龄与组别的 Spearman 相关性为 0.133(p = 0.007)。各组之间的绿叶偏好(GLP)差异具有统计学意义(Mann-Whitney U:p = 0.038)。GLP 与组别的 Spearman 相关性为 0.102(p = 0.037)。对穿着 WG 感知的逻辑回归表明,年龄和 GLP 是重要的预测因素(年龄:B 重量 0.109,p = 0.012,几率:1.115;GLP:B 重量 0.317,p = 0.033,几率:1.373)。年龄较大和 GLP 较高的儿童更有可能将 #TheDress 视为 WG。这些结果表明,这可能与儿童一生中黄斑色素的逐渐积累有关。
{"title":"Perception of #TheDress in childhood is influenced by age and green-leaf preference.","authors":"Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva, Catalina Becerra-Revollo, Luis Antonio Rhoads-Avila, Julian García-Sánchez, Flor Angélica Jácome-Gutierrez, Linda Cernichiaro-Espinosa, Andrée Henaine-Berra, Axel Orozco-Hernandez, Humberto Ruiz-García, Eduardo Torres-Porras","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.11","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.11","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of the ambiguous image of #TheDress may be influenced by optical factors, such as macular pigments. Their accumulation during childhood could increase with age and the ingestion of carotenoid-containing foods. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether the visual perception of the dress in children would differ based on age and carotenoid preference. This was a cross-sectional, observational, and comparative study. A poll was administered to children aged 2 to 10 years. Parents were instructed to inquire about the color of #TheDress from their children. A carotenoid preference survey was also completed. A total of 413 poll responses were analyzed. Responses were categorized based on the perceived color of the dress: blue/black (BB) (n = 204) and white/gold (WG) (n = 209). The mean and median age of the WG group was higher than the BB group (mean 6.1, median 6.0 years, standard deviation [SD] 2.2; mean 5.5, median 5.0 years, SD 2.3; p = 0.007). Spearman correlation between age and group was 0.133 (p = 0.007). Green-leaf preference (GLP) showed a statistically significant difference between groups (Mann-Whitney U: p = 0.038). Spearman correlation between GLP and group was 0.102 (p = 0.037). Logistic regression for the perception of the dress as WG indicated that age and GLP were significant predictors (age: B weight 0.109, p = 0.012, odds ratio: 1.115; GLP: B weight 0.317, p = 0.033, odds ratio: 1.373). Older children and those with a higher GLP were more likely to perceive #TheDress as WG. These results suggest a potential relationship with the gradual accumulation of macular pigments throughout a child's lifetime.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"11"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11353488/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019359","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrections to: Exploring the extent to which shared mechanisms contribute to motion-position illusions. 更正:探索共同机制对运动位置错觉的影响程度。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.9
{"title":"Corrections to: Exploring the extent to which shared mechanisms contribute to motion-position illusions.","authors":"","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.9","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"9"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11346168/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142005636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Determining the efficacy of visual inspections at detecting non-biosecurity-compliant goods. 确定目视检查在检测不符合生物安全要求的货物方面的功效。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.8
Kambiz Esfandi, Saeedeh Afsar, Kate Richards, Duncan Hedderley, Samuel D J Brown, Adriana Najar-Rodriguez, Mike Ormsby

Examination of imported commodities by trained inspectors searching for pest organisms is a common practice that phytosanitary regulatory agencies use to mitigate biosecurity risks along trade pathways. To investigate the effects of target size and color on the efficacy of these visual assessments, we affixed square decals to polystyrene models of mandarins. Sample units of 100 model fruit containing up to 10 marked models were examined by inspectors. Six sizes in six shades of brown were tested across two prevalence levels. The experiment consisted of five inspection rounds where 11 inspectors examined 77 sample units within an allocated time. The probability that decals were detected increased with mark size and color contrast. Smaller, low-contrast marks were mainly missed. The prevalence rate did not affect the detectability. Through the experiment, the false-positive rate dropped from 6% to 3%, whereas false-negative rates were constant throughout. Large, dark targets were readily found with a mean recall of >90%, whereas small, pale marks had a mean recall of 9%. Increased experience made inspectors more competent at recognizing decals, reducing the false positive rate. However, constant false-negative rates indicate that experience did not prevent inspectors from overlooking targets they could not perceive.

由训练有素的检查员对进口商品进行有害生物检查,是植物检疫监管机构用来降低贸易通道生物安全风险的常用方法。为了研究目标大小和颜色对这些视觉评估效果的影响,我们在柑橘的聚苯乙烯模型上贴上了方形贴纸。检查员以 100 个模型水果为单位进行抽样检查,其中包含多达 10 个有标记的模型。在两个流行水平上测试了六种大小、六种深浅的棕色。实验包括五轮检查,11 名检查员在规定时间内检查了 77 个样本单位。检测到贴花的概率随着标记大小和颜色对比度的增加而增加。较小、对比度低的标记主要被漏检。普遍率并不影响检测率。在整个实验过程中,假阳性率从 6% 下降到 3%,而假阴性率则始终保持不变。大而深的目标很容易被发现,平均召回率大于 90%,而小而浅的目标平均召回率为 9%。经验的增加使检查员识别标记的能力增强,从而降低了假阳性率。然而,恒定的假阴性率表明,经验并不能防止检查员忽略他们无法感知的目标。
{"title":"Determining the efficacy of visual inspections at detecting non-biosecurity-compliant goods.","authors":"Kambiz Esfandi, Saeedeh Afsar, Kate Richards, Duncan Hedderley, Samuel D J Brown, Adriana Najar-Rodriguez, Mike Ormsby","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.8","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Examination of imported commodities by trained inspectors searching for pest organisms is a common practice that phytosanitary regulatory agencies use to mitigate biosecurity risks along trade pathways. To investigate the effects of target size and color on the efficacy of these visual assessments, we affixed square decals to polystyrene models of mandarins. Sample units of 100 model fruit containing up to 10 marked models were examined by inspectors. Six sizes in six shades of brown were tested across two prevalence levels. The experiment consisted of five inspection rounds where 11 inspectors examined 77 sample units within an allocated time. The probability that decals were detected increased with mark size and color contrast. Smaller, low-contrast marks were mainly missed. The prevalence rate did not affect the detectability. Through the experiment, the false-positive rate dropped from 6% to 3%, whereas false-negative rates were constant throughout. Large, dark targets were readily found with a mean recall of >90%, whereas small, pale marks had a mean recall of 9%. Increased experience made inspectors more competent at recognizing decals, reducing the false positive rate. However, constant false-negative rates indicate that experience did not prevent inspectors from overlooking targets they could not perceive.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"8"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11343003/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active mutual conjoint estimation of multiple contrast sensitivity functions. 多种对比敏感度函数的主动相互联合估计。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.6
Dom C P Marticorena, Quinn Wai Wong, Jake Browning, Ken Wilbur, Pinakin Gunvant Davey, Aaron R Seitz, Jacob R Gardner, Dennis L Barbour

Recent advances in nonparametric contrast sensitivity function (CSF) estimation have yielded a new tradeoff between accuracy and efficiency not available to classical parametric estimators. An additional advantage of this new framework is the ability to independently tune multiple aspects of the estimator to seek further improvements. Machine learning CSF estimation with Gaussian processes allows for design optimization in the kernel, acquisition function, and underlying task representation, to name a few. This article describes a novel kernel for CSF estimation that is more flexible than a kernel based on strictly functional forms. Despite being more flexible, it can result in a more efficient estimator. Further, trial selection for data acquisition that is generalized beyond pure information gain can also improve estimator quality. Finally, introducing latent variable representations underlying general CSF shapes can enable simultaneous estimation of multiple CSFs, such as from different eyes, eccentricities, or luminances. The conditions under which the new procedures perform better than previous nonparametric estimation procedures are presented and quantified.

非参数对比灵敏度函数(CSF)估算的最新进展在准确性和效率之间实现了新的权衡,这是传统参数估算方法所不具备的。这种新框架的另一个优势是能够独立调整估计器的多个方面,以寻求进一步的改进。利用高斯过程的机器学习 CSF 估计可以在内核、获取函数和底层任务表示等方面进行设计优化。本文介绍了一种用于 CSF 估计的新型核,它比基于严格函数形式的核更加灵活。尽管更灵活,但它能产生更高效的估计器。此外,超越纯信息增益的数据采集试验选择也能提高估算质量。最后,在一般 CSF 形状的基础上引入潜变量表征,可以同时估算多个 CSF,例如来自不同眼睛、偏心率或亮度的 CSF。本文介绍了新程序比以前的非参数估计程序表现更好的条件,并对其进行了量化。
{"title":"Active mutual conjoint estimation of multiple contrast sensitivity functions.","authors":"Dom C P Marticorena, Quinn Wai Wong, Jake Browning, Ken Wilbur, Pinakin Gunvant Davey, Aaron R Seitz, Jacob R Gardner, Dennis L Barbour","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.6","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent advances in nonparametric contrast sensitivity function (CSF) estimation have yielded a new tradeoff between accuracy and efficiency not available to classical parametric estimators. An additional advantage of this new framework is the ability to independently tune multiple aspects of the estimator to seek further improvements. Machine learning CSF estimation with Gaussian processes allows for design optimization in the kernel, acquisition function, and underlying task representation, to name a few. This article describes a novel kernel for CSF estimation that is more flexible than a kernel based on strictly functional forms. Despite being more flexible, it can result in a more efficient estimator. Further, trial selection for data acquisition that is generalized beyond pure information gain can also improve estimator quality. Finally, introducing latent variable representations underlying general CSF shapes can enable simultaneous estimation of multiple CSFs, such as from different eyes, eccentricities, or luminances. The conditions under which the new procedures perform better than previous nonparametric estimation procedures are presented and quantified.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"6"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11314691/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistent metacognitive efficiency and variable response biases in peripheral vision. 外围视觉中一致的元认知效率和可变的反应偏差。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.4
Joseph Pruitt, J D Knotts, Brian Odegaard

Across the visual periphery, perceptual and metacognitive abilities differ depending on the locus of visual attention, the location of peripheral stimulus presentation, the task design, and many other factors. In this investigation, we aimed to illuminate the relationship between attention and eccentricity in the visual periphery by estimating perceptual sensitivity, metacognitive sensitivity, and response biases across the visual field. In a 2AFC detection task, participants were asked to determine whether a signal was present or absent at one of eight peripheral locations (±10°, 20°, 30°, and 40°), using either a valid or invalid attentional cue. As expected, results revealed that perceptual sensitivity declined with eccentricity and was modulated by attention, with higher sensitivity on validly cued trials. Furthermore, a significant main effect of eccentricity on response bias emerged, with variable (but relatively unbiased) c'a values from 10° to 30°, and conservative c'a values at 40°. Regarding metacognitive sensitivity, significant main effects of attention and eccentricity were found, with metacognitive sensitivity decreasing with eccentricity, and decreasing in the invalid cue condition. Interestingly, metacognitive efficiency, as measured by the ratio of meta-d'a/d'a, was not modulated by attention or eccentricity. Overall, these findings demonstrate (1) that in some circumstances, observers have surprisingly robust metacognitive insights into how performance changes across the visual field and (2) that the periphery may be subject to variable detection biases that are contingent on the exact location in peripheral space.

在整个视觉外围,知觉和元认知能力因视觉注意力的位置、外围刺激呈现的位置、任务设计和许多其他因素的不同而不同。在这项研究中,我们旨在通过估计整个视野中的知觉灵敏度、元认知灵敏度和反应偏差,来阐明视觉周边的注意力和偏心率之间的关系。在一项 2AFC 检测任务中,受试者被要求使用有效或无效的注意线索,判断信号在八个外围位置(±10°、20°、30° 和 40°)中的一个位置是存在还是不存在。不出所料,结果显示感知灵敏度随偏心率下降,并受注意力调节,在有效提示的试验中灵敏度较高。此外,偏心率对反应偏差有明显的主效应,10°到30°之间的c'a值不尽相同(但相对无偏),而40°时的c'a值较保守。在元认知灵敏度方面,注意力和偏心率具有显著的主效应,元认知灵敏度随偏心率的增加而降低,并且在无效线索条件下降低。有趣的是,元认知效率(以元-d'a/d'a 的比率来衡量)并不受注意力或偏心率的调节。总之,这些研究结果表明:(1) 在某些情况下,观察者对整个视野中的表现如何变化具有令人惊讶的强大元认知洞察力;(2) 外围可能会受到可变检测偏差的影响,而这些偏差取决于外围空间的确切位置。
{"title":"Consistent metacognitive efficiency and variable response biases in peripheral vision.","authors":"Joseph Pruitt, J D Knotts, Brian Odegaard","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.4","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across the visual periphery, perceptual and metacognitive abilities differ depending on the locus of visual attention, the location of peripheral stimulus presentation, the task design, and many other factors. In this investigation, we aimed to illuminate the relationship between attention and eccentricity in the visual periphery by estimating perceptual sensitivity, metacognitive sensitivity, and response biases across the visual field. In a 2AFC detection task, participants were asked to determine whether a signal was present or absent at one of eight peripheral locations (±10°, 20°, 30°, and 40°), using either a valid or invalid attentional cue. As expected, results revealed that perceptual sensitivity declined with eccentricity and was modulated by attention, with higher sensitivity on validly cued trials. Furthermore, a significant main effect of eccentricity on response bias emerged, with variable (but relatively unbiased) c'a values from 10° to 30°, and conservative c'a values at 40°. Regarding metacognitive sensitivity, significant main effects of attention and eccentricity were found, with metacognitive sensitivity decreasing with eccentricity, and decreasing in the invalid cue condition. Interestingly, metacognitive efficiency, as measured by the ratio of meta-d'a/d'a, was not modulated by attention or eccentricity. Overall, these findings demonstrate (1) that in some circumstances, observers have surprisingly robust metacognitive insights into how performance changes across the visual field and (2) that the periphery may be subject to variable detection biases that are contingent on the exact location in peripheral space.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"4"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11314628/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direction-selective adaptation from implied motion in infancy. 婴儿期对隐含运动的方向选择性适应。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.7
Riku Umekawa, So Kanazawa, Masami K Yamaguchi

We investigated whether adaptation from implied motion (IM) is transferred to real motion using optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) in infants. Specifically, we examined whether viewing a series of images depicting motion shifted infants' OKN responses to the opposite direction of random dot kinematograms (RDKs). Each RDK was presented 10 times in a pre-test, followed by 10 trials of IM adaptation and test. During the pre-test, the signal dots of the RDK moved left or right. During IM adaptation, 10 randomly selected images depicting leftward (or rightward) IM were presented. In the test, the RDK was presented immediately after the last IM image. An observer, blinded to the motion direction, assessed the OKN direction. The number of matches in OKN responses for each RDK direction was calculated as the match ratio of OKN. We conducted a two-way mixed analysis of variance, with age group (5-6 months and 7-8 months) as the between-participant factor and adaptation (pre-test and test) as the within-participant factor. Only in 7-8 months the OKN responses were shifted in the opposite direction of RDK by viewing a series of images depicting motion, and these infants could detect both IM and RDK motion directions in the pre-test. Our results indicate that detecting the IM and RDK directions might induce direction-selective adaptation in 7-8 months.

我们利用婴儿的视运动眼震(OKN)研究了隐含运动(IM)的适应是否会转移到真实运动中。具体来说,我们研究了观看一系列描述运动的图像是否会使婴儿的 OKN 反应转向随机点运动图(RDK)的相反方向。每个 RDK 在预测试中呈现 10 次,然后进行 10 次 IM 适应和测试。在预测试期间,随机点运动图的信号点向左或向右移动。在 IM 适应过程中,随机选择 10 幅描述向左(或向右)IM 的图像。在测试中,RDK 紧随最后一幅 IM 图像之后出现。一名对运动方向视而不见的观察者对 OKN 方向进行评估。每个 RDK 方向的 OKN 反应的匹配数被计算为 OKN 的匹配率。我们进行了双向混合方差分析,年龄组(5-6 个月和 7-8 个月)为参与者间因素,适应(测试前和测试)为参与者内因素。只有 7-8 个月大的婴儿在观看一系列运动图像时,OKN 反应才会向 RDK 的反方向移动,而且这些婴儿在前测中既能检测到 IM 运动方向,也能检测到 RDK 运动方向。我们的结果表明,在 7-8 个月时,检测 IM 和 RDK 方向可能会诱发方向选择性适应。
{"title":"Direction-selective adaptation from implied motion in infancy.","authors":"Riku Umekawa, So Kanazawa, Masami K Yamaguchi","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.7","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated whether adaptation from implied motion (IM) is transferred to real motion using optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) in infants. Specifically, we examined whether viewing a series of images depicting motion shifted infants' OKN responses to the opposite direction of random dot kinematograms (RDKs). Each RDK was presented 10 times in a pre-test, followed by 10 trials of IM adaptation and test. During the pre-test, the signal dots of the RDK moved left or right. During IM adaptation, 10 randomly selected images depicting leftward (or rightward) IM were presented. In the test, the RDK was presented immediately after the last IM image. An observer, blinded to the motion direction, assessed the OKN direction. The number of matches in OKN responses for each RDK direction was calculated as the match ratio of OKN. We conducted a two-way mixed analysis of variance, with age group (5-6 months and 7-8 months) as the between-participant factor and adaptation (pre-test and test) as the within-participant factor. Only in 7-8 months the OKN responses were shifted in the opposite direction of RDK by viewing a series of images depicting motion, and these infants could detect both IM and RDK motion directions in the pre-test. Our results indicate that detecting the IM and RDK directions might induce direction-selective adaptation in 7-8 months.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"7"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11343005/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Retinotopy drives the variation in scene responses across visual field map divisions of the occipital place area. 视网膜视图驱动了枕叶位置区视场图各分区场景反应的变化。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.10
Catriona L Scrivener, Elisa Zamboni, Antony B Morland, Edward H Silson

The occipital place area (OPA) is a scene-selective region on the lateral surface of human occipitotemporal cortex that spatially overlaps multiple visual field maps, as well as portions of cortex that are not currently defined as retinotopic. Here we combined population receptive field modeling and responses to scenes in a representational similarity analysis (RSA) framework to test the prediction that the OPA's visual field map divisions contribute uniquely to the overall pattern of scene selectivity within the OPA. Consistent with this prediction, the patterns of response to a set of complex scenes were heterogeneous between maps. To explain this heterogeneity, we tested the explanatory power of seven candidate models using RSA. These models spanned different scene dimensions (Content, Expanse, Distance), low- and high-level visual features, and navigational affordances. None of the tested models could account for the variation in scene response observed between the OPA's visual field maps. However, the heterogeneity in scene response was correlated with the differences in retinotopic profiles across maps. These data highlight the need to carefully examine the relationship between regions defined as category-selective and the underlying retinotopy, and they suggest that, in the case of the OPA, it may not be appropriate to conceptualize it as a single scene-selective region.

枕叶位置区(OPA)是人类枕颞皮层外侧表面的一个场景选择区,它在空间上与多个视场图以及目前尚未定义为视网膜视位的皮层部分重叠。在这里,我们在表象相似性分析(RSA)框架中结合了群体感受野建模和对场景的反应,以检验 OPA 的视野图划分对 OPA 内场景选择性整体模式的独特贡献这一预测。与这一预测相一致的是,不同视场图之间对一组复杂场景的反应模式是不同的。为了解释这种异质性,我们使用 RSA 测试了七个候选模型的解释力。这些模型涵盖了不同的场景维度(内容、广度、距离)、低级和高级视觉特征以及导航能力。没有一个测试模型能够解释在 OPA 视场图之间观察到的场景反应差异。然而,场景反应的异质性与不同视网膜图之间视网膜图谱的差异相关。这些数据突出表明,有必要仔细研究被定义为类别选择性的区域与底层视网膜视图之间的关系,它们还表明,就 OPA 而言,将其概念化为单一的场景选择性区域可能并不合适。
{"title":"Retinotopy drives the variation in scene responses across visual field map divisions of the occipital place area.","authors":"Catriona L Scrivener, Elisa Zamboni, Antony B Morland, Edward H Silson","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.10","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.10","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The occipital place area (OPA) is a scene-selective region on the lateral surface of human occipitotemporal cortex that spatially overlaps multiple visual field maps, as well as portions of cortex that are not currently defined as retinotopic. Here we combined population receptive field modeling and responses to scenes in a representational similarity analysis (RSA) framework to test the prediction that the OPA's visual field map divisions contribute uniquely to the overall pattern of scene selectivity within the OPA. Consistent with this prediction, the patterns of response to a set of complex scenes were heterogeneous between maps. To explain this heterogeneity, we tested the explanatory power of seven candidate models using RSA. These models spanned different scene dimensions (Content, Expanse, Distance), low- and high-level visual features, and navigational affordances. None of the tested models could account for the variation in scene response observed between the OPA's visual field maps. However, the heterogeneity in scene response was correlated with the differences in retinotopic profiles across maps. These data highlight the need to carefully examine the relationship between regions defined as category-selective and the underlying retinotopy, and they suggest that, in the case of the OPA, it may not be appropriate to conceptualize it as a single scene-selective region.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"10"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11343012/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Weighted power summation and contrast normalization mechanisms account for short-latency eye movements to motion and disparity of sine-wave gratings and broadband visual stimuli in humans. 加权功率求和与对比度归一化机制解释了人类对正弦波光栅和宽带视觉刺激的运动和差异的短时眼动。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.14
Boris M Sheliga, Edmond J FitzGibbon

In this paper, we show that the model we proposed earlier to account for the disparity vergence eye movements (disparity vergence responses, or DVRs) in response to horizontal and vertical disparity steps of white noise visual stimuli also provides an excellent description of the short-latency ocular following responses (OFRs) to broadband stimuli in the visual motion domain. In addition, we reanalyzed the data and applied the model to several earlier studies that used sine-wave gratings (single or a combination of two or three gratings) and white noise stimuli. The model provides a very good account of all of these data. The model postulates that the short-latency eye movements-OFRs and DVRs-can be accounted for by the operation of two factors: an excitatory drive, determined by a weighted sum of contributions of stimulus Fourier components, scaled by a global contrast normalization mechanism. The output of the operation of these two factors is then nonlinearly scaled by the total contrast of the stimulus. Despite different roles of disparity (horizontal and vertical) and motion signals in visual scene analyses, the earliest processing stages of these different signals appear to be very similar.

在本文中,我们证明了早先提出的用于解释对白噪声视觉刺激的水平和垂直差距阶跃反应的差距辐辏眼动(差距辐辏反应或DVRs)的模型,也能很好地描述视觉运动域中对宽带刺激的短时眼球跟随反应(OFRs)。此外,我们还对数据进行了重新分析,并将该模型应用于早期的几项使用正弦波光栅(单个或两个或三个光栅的组合)和白噪声刺激的研究。该模型很好地解释了所有这些数据。该模型假定,短时眼动--OFR和DVR--可以通过两个因素的作用来解释:一个是兴奋驱动,由刺激傅立叶成分贡献的加权和决定,并通过全局对比度归一化机制进行缩放。然后,这两个因素运作的输出被刺激的总对比度非线性地缩放。尽管色差(水平和垂直)和运动信号在视觉场景分析中扮演着不同的角色,但这些不同信号的最初处理阶段似乎非常相似。
{"title":"Weighted power summation and contrast normalization mechanisms account for short-latency eye movements to motion and disparity of sine-wave gratings and broadband visual stimuli in humans.","authors":"Boris M Sheliga, Edmond J FitzGibbon","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.14","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.14","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this paper, we show that the model we proposed earlier to account for the disparity vergence eye movements (disparity vergence responses, or DVRs) in response to horizontal and vertical disparity steps of white noise visual stimuli also provides an excellent description of the short-latency ocular following responses (OFRs) to broadband stimuli in the visual motion domain. In addition, we reanalyzed the data and applied the model to several earlier studies that used sine-wave gratings (single or a combination of two or three gratings) and white noise stimuli. The model provides a very good account of all of these data. The model postulates that the short-latency eye movements-OFRs and DVRs-can be accounted for by the operation of two factors: an excitatory drive, determined by a weighted sum of contributions of stimulus Fourier components, scaled by a global contrast normalization mechanism. The output of the operation of these two factors is then nonlinearly scaled by the total contrast of the stimulus. Despite different roles of disparity (horizontal and vertical) and motion signals in visual scene analyses, the earliest processing stages of these different signals appear to be very similar.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 8","pages":"14"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363211/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142057109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Vision
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1