首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Vision最新文献

英文 中文
Embeddedness of Earth's gravity in visual perception. 地球引力在视觉感知中的嵌入性。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.4
Abdul-Rahim Deeb, Fulvio Domini

Falling objects are commonplace in daily life, requiring precise perceptual judgments for interception and avoidance. We argue that human judgments of projectile motion arise from the interplay between sensory information and predictions constrained by Newtonian mechanics. Our study investigates how individuals perceive falling objects under various gravitational conditions, aiming to understand the role of internalized gravity in visual perception. Through meticulously controlling the available information, we demonstrated that these phenomena cannot be explained solely by simple heuristics nor representational momentum. Instead, we found that the perceptual judgments of humans (n = 11, 13, 14, and 11, respectively, in Experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4) are influenced by a combination of sensory information and gravity predictions, highlighting the role of internalized physical constraints in the perception of projectile motion.

日常生活中,高空坠物司空见惯,需要精确的感知判断来拦截和躲避。我们认为,人类对抛射体运动的判断源于感官信息与受牛顿力学制约的预测之间的相互作用。我们的研究调查了个体在不同重力条件下如何感知坠落物体,旨在了解内化重力在视觉感知中的作用。通过对可用信息的细致控制,我们证明这些现象不能仅用简单的启发式或表象动量来解释。相反,我们发现人类(实验 1、2、3 和 4 中的人数分别为 11、13、14 和 11)的知觉判断受到了感官信息和重力预测的综合影响,这凸显了内化物理约束在弹丸运动知觉中的作用。
{"title":"Embeddedness of Earth's gravity in visual perception.","authors":"Abdul-Rahim Deeb, Fulvio Domini","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.4","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Falling objects are commonplace in daily life, requiring precise perceptual judgments for interception and avoidance. We argue that human judgments of projectile motion arise from the interplay between sensory information and predictions constrained by Newtonian mechanics. Our study investigates how individuals perceive falling objects under various gravitational conditions, aiming to understand the role of internalized gravity in visual perception. Through meticulously controlling the available information, we demonstrated that these phenomena cannot be explained solely by simple heuristics nor representational momentum. Instead, we found that the perceptual judgments of humans (n = 11, 13, 14, and 11, respectively, in Experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4) are influenced by a combination of sensory information and gravity predictions, highlighting the role of internalized physical constraints in the perception of projectile motion.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 11","pages":"4"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11463708/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142382185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measurements of chromatic adaptation and luminous efficiency while wearing colored filters. 测量佩戴彩色滤光片时的色度适应性和发光效率。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.9
Andrew J Coia, Joseph M Arizpe, Peter A Smith, Thomas K Kuyk, Julie A Lovell

The visual system adapts dynamically to stabilize perception over widely varying illuminations. Such adaptation allows the colors of objects to appear constant despite changes in spectral illumination. Similarly, the wearing of colored filters also alters spectral content, but this alteration can be more extreme than typically encountered in nature, presenting a unique challenge to color constancy mechanisms. While it is known that chromatic adaptation is affected by surrounding spatial context, a recent study reported a gradual temporal adaptation effect to colored filters such that colors initially appear strongly shifted but over hours of wear are perceived as closer to an unfiltered appearance. Presently, it is not clear whether the luminance system adapts spatially and temporally like the chromatic system. To address this, spatial and temporal adaptation effects to a colored filter were measured using tasks that assess chromatic and luminance adaptation separately. Prior to and for 1 hour after putting on a pair of colored filters, participants made achromatic and heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP) settings to measure chromatic and luminance adaptation, respectively. Results showed significant chromatic adaptation with achromatic settings moving closer to baseline settings over 1 hour of wearing the filters and greater adaptation with spatial context. Conversely, there was no significant luminance adaptation and HFP matches fell close to what was predicted photometrically. The results are discussed in the context of prior studies of chromatic and luminance adaptation.

视觉系统可以动态地适应光线的变化,从而稳定感知。这种适应性使物体的颜色在光谱光照变化的情况下仍然保持不变。同样,佩戴彩色滤光镜也会改变光谱内容,但这种改变可能比自然界中通常遇到的更为剧烈,这对色彩恒定机制提出了独特的挑战。众所周知,色度适应受周围空间环境的影响,而最近的一项研究则报告了对彩色滤光片的渐进式时间适应效应,即最初出现的色彩强烈偏移,但在佩戴数小时后会被认为更接近未过滤的外观。目前,尚不清楚亮度系统是否会像色度系统那样在空间和时间上进行适应。为了解决这个问题,我们使用分别评估色度和亮度适应性的任务来测量对彩色滤光片的空间和时间适应效应。在戴上一对彩色滤光片之前和之后的 1 小时内,参与者分别进行消色差和异色差闪烁测光(HFP)设置,以测量色度和亮度适应性。结果表明,在佩戴滤色镜 1 小时后,消色差设置更接近基线设置,色度适应性明显增强,空间环境适应性也更强。相反,亮度适应性不明显,HFP 匹配度接近光度预测值。本研究结合之前的色度和亮度适应性研究对结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Measurements of chromatic adaptation and luminous efficiency while wearing colored filters.","authors":"Andrew J Coia, Joseph M Arizpe, Peter A Smith, Thomas K Kuyk, Julie A Lovell","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.9","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The visual system adapts dynamically to stabilize perception over widely varying illuminations. Such adaptation allows the colors of objects to appear constant despite changes in spectral illumination. Similarly, the wearing of colored filters also alters spectral content, but this alteration can be more extreme than typically encountered in nature, presenting a unique challenge to color constancy mechanisms. While it is known that chromatic adaptation is affected by surrounding spatial context, a recent study reported a gradual temporal adaptation effect to colored filters such that colors initially appear strongly shifted but over hours of wear are perceived as closer to an unfiltered appearance. Presently, it is not clear whether the luminance system adapts spatially and temporally like the chromatic system. To address this, spatial and temporal adaptation effects to a colored filter were measured using tasks that assess chromatic and luminance adaptation separately. Prior to and for 1 hour after putting on a pair of colored filters, participants made achromatic and heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP) settings to measure chromatic and luminance adaptation, respectively. Results showed significant chromatic adaptation with achromatic settings moving closer to baseline settings over 1 hour of wearing the filters and greater adaptation with spatial context. Conversely, there was no significant luminance adaptation and HFP matches fell close to what was predicted photometrically. The results are discussed in the context of prior studies of chromatic and luminance adaptation.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 11","pages":"9"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11472893/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Seeing on the fly: Physiological and behavioral evidence show that space-to-space representation and processing enable fast and efficient performance by the visual system. 飞速视觉生理学和行为学证据表明,空间对空间的表征和处理使视觉系统能够快速高效地工作。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.11
Moshe Gur

When we view the world, our eyes saccade quickly between points of interest. Even when fixating a target our eyes are not completely at rest but execute small fixational eye movements (FEMs). That vision is not blurred despite this ever-present jitter has seemingly motivated an increasingly popular theory denying the reliance of the visual system on pure spatial processing in favor of a space-to-time mechanism generated by the eye drifting across the image. Accordingly, FEMs are not detrimental but rather essential to good visibility. However, the space-to-time theory is incompatible with physiological data showing that all information is conveyed by the short neural volleys generated when the eyes land on a target, and with our faithful perception of briefly displayed objects, during which time FEMs have no effect. Another difficulty in rejecting the idea of image representation by the locations and nature of responding cells in favor of a timecode, is that somewhere, somehow, this code must be decoded into a parallel spatial one when reaching perception. Thus, in addition to the implausibility of generating meaningful responses during retinal drift, the space-to-time hypothesis calls for replacing efficient point-to-point parallel transmission with a cumbersome, delayed, space-to-time-to-space process. A novel physiological framework is presented here wherein the ability of the visual system to quickly process information is mediated by the short, powerful neural volleys generated by the landing saccades. These volleys are necessary and sufficient for normal perception without FEMs contribution. This mechanism enables our excellent perception of brief stimuli and explains that visibility is not blurred by FEMs because they do not generate useful information.

当我们观察世界时,我们的眼睛会在感兴趣的点之间快速移动。即使在盯住一个目标时,我们的眼睛也不会完全静止,而是会执行微小的定睛眼动(FEM)。尽管这种抖动一直存在,但视觉却并不模糊,这似乎推动了一种日益流行的理论,即否认视觉系统依赖于纯粹的空间处理,而倾向于由眼睛在图像上漂移产生的时空机制。据此,FEM 对良好的可视性并非有害,而是必不可少的。然而,空间-时间理论与生理数据不符,生理数据显示,所有信息都是由眼睛落在目标上时产生的短暂神经波动传递的,而且与我们对短暂显示物体的忠实感知也不符,在这段时间内,FEM 没有任何作用。要否定通过反应细胞的位置和性质进行图像表征的观点,转而支持时间码的另一个困难在于,在达到感知时,这种编码必须在某处以某种方式解码成平行的空间编码。因此,除了不可能在视网膜漂移过程中产生有意义的反应之外,空间-时间假说还要求用一个繁琐、延迟的空间-时间-空间过程取代高效的点对点平行传输。本文提出了一个新颖的生理学框架,即视觉系统快速处理信息的能力是由着陆囊视产生的短促而有力的神经波动所促成的。这些波动是正常感知的必要条件和充分条件,而不需要 FEMs 的贡献。这种机制使我们能够很好地感知短暂的刺激,并解释了为什么可见度不会因快速眼动而模糊,因为快速眼动不会产生有用的信息。
{"title":"Seeing on the fly: Physiological and behavioral evidence show that space-to-space representation and processing enable fast and efficient performance by the visual system.","authors":"Moshe Gur","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.11","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.11","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When we view the world, our eyes saccade quickly between points of interest. Even when fixating a target our eyes are not completely at rest but execute small fixational eye movements (FEMs). That vision is not blurred despite this ever-present jitter has seemingly motivated an increasingly popular theory denying the reliance of the visual system on pure spatial processing in favor of a space-to-time mechanism generated by the eye drifting across the image. Accordingly, FEMs are not detrimental but rather essential to good visibility. However, the space-to-time theory is incompatible with physiological data showing that all information is conveyed by the short neural volleys generated when the eyes land on a target, and with our faithful perception of briefly displayed objects, during which time FEMs have no effect. Another difficulty in rejecting the idea of image representation by the locations and nature of responding cells in favor of a timecode, is that somewhere, somehow, this code must be decoded into a parallel spatial one when reaching perception. Thus, in addition to the implausibility of generating meaningful responses during retinal drift, the space-to-time hypothesis calls for replacing efficient point-to-point parallel transmission with a cumbersome, delayed, space-to-time-to-space process. A novel physiological framework is presented here wherein the ability of the visual system to quickly process information is mediated by the short, powerful neural volleys generated by the landing saccades. These volleys are necessary and sufficient for normal perception without FEMs contribution. This mechanism enables our excellent perception of brief stimuli and explains that visibility is not blurred by FEMs because they do not generate useful information.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 11","pages":"11"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11472890/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Concurrent perception of competing predictions: A "split-stimulus effect". 同时感知相互竞争的预测:分裂刺激效应
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.5
Joseph Melling, William Turner, Hinze Hogendoorn

Visual illusions are systematic misperceptions that can help us glean the heuristics with which the brain constructs visual experience. In a recently discovered visual illusion (the "frame effect"), it has been shown that flashing a stimulus inside of a moving frame produces a large misperception of that stimulus's position. Across two experiments, we investigated a novel illusion (the "split stimulus effect") where the symmetrical motion of two overlaid frames produces two simultaneous positional misperceptions of a single stimulus. That is, one stimulus is presented but two are perceived. In both experiments, a single red dot was flashed when the moving frames reversed direction, and participants were asked to report how many dots they saw. Naïve participants sometimes reported seeing two dots when only one was presented, indicating spontaneous perception of the illusion. A Bayesian analysis of the population prevalence of this effect was conducted. The dependence of this effect on the frames' speed, the dot's opacity, spatial attention, as the presence/absence of pre-flash motion ("postdiction") was also investigated, and the features of this illusion were compared to similar motion position illusions within a predictive processing framework. In demonstrating this illusory "splitting" effect, this study is the first to show that it is possible to be simultaneously aware of two opposing perceptual predictions about a single object and provides evidence of the hyperpriors that limit and inform the structure of visual experience.

视错觉是一种系统性错觉,可以帮助我们了解大脑构建视觉经验的启发式方法。最近发现的一种视觉错觉("框架效应")表明,在一个移动的框架内闪烁一个刺激物,会对该刺激物的位置产生很大的错觉。在两个实验中,我们研究了一种新的错觉("分割刺激效应"),即两个重叠框架的对称运动会同时对一个刺激产生两种位置错觉。也就是说,呈现的是一个刺激,但感知到的却是两个刺激。在这两项实验中,当运动框架反向运动时,会闪现一个红点,参与者被要求报告他们看到了几个红点。天真的参与者有时会报告看到了两个点,而实际上只出现了一个点,这表明他们自发地感知到了幻觉。我们对这种效应在人群中的普遍性进行了贝叶斯分析。此外,还研究了这种效应对帧的速度、点的不透明度、空间注意力以及闪烁前运动("后预测")的存在/不存在的依赖性,并在预测处理框架内将这种错觉的特征与类似的运动位置错觉进行了比较。通过展示这种虚幻的 "分裂 "效应,本研究首次证明了同时意识到关于单一物体的两种相反的知觉预测是可能的,并提供了限制视觉经验结构并为其提供信息的超先验因素的证据。
{"title":"Concurrent perception of competing predictions: A \"split-stimulus effect\".","authors":"Joseph Melling, William Turner, Hinze Hogendoorn","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual illusions are systematic misperceptions that can help us glean the heuristics with which the brain constructs visual experience. In a recently discovered visual illusion (the \"frame effect\"), it has been shown that flashing a stimulus inside of a moving frame produces a large misperception of that stimulus's position. Across two experiments, we investigated a novel illusion (the \"split stimulus effect\") where the symmetrical motion of two overlaid frames produces two simultaneous positional misperceptions of a single stimulus. That is, one stimulus is presented but two are perceived. In both experiments, a single red dot was flashed when the moving frames reversed direction, and participants were asked to report how many dots they saw. Naïve participants sometimes reported seeing two dots when only one was presented, indicating spontaneous perception of the illusion. A Bayesian analysis of the population prevalence of this effect was conducted. The dependence of this effect on the frames' speed, the dot's opacity, spatial attention, as the presence/absence of pre-flash motion (\"postdiction\") was also investigated, and the features of this illusion were compared to similar motion position illusions within a predictive processing framework. In demonstrating this illusory \"splitting\" effect, this study is the first to show that it is possible to be simultaneously aware of two opposing perceptual predictions about a single object and provides evidence of the hyperpriors that limit and inform the structure of visual experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 11","pages":"5"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11463704/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two faces of holistic face processing: Facilitation and interference underlying part-whole and composite effects. 整体人脸处理的两面性:部分-整体效应和复合效应背后的促进和干扰。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.13
Haiyang Jin, William G Hayward, Olivia S Cheung

Holistic processing, a strong tendency to process multiple features together, is regarded as a hallmark of face perception. Holistic effects can be revealed by several tasks, including the part-whole task, standard composite task, and complete composite task. Although holistic effects are readily observed using these tasks, the lack of correlations among these effects and the mixed findings across these tasks when examining the effects among various populations or manipulations pose questions about how these effects should be understood. We distinguished facilitation and interference effects within the holistic effects in the complete composite task and found that the holistic effect in the part-whole task appeared to be correlated with facilitation but not interference in the complete composite task, whereas the holistic effect in the standard composite task was correlated with interference but not facilitation in the complete composite task. These findings suggest that clarifying the roles of facilitation and interference is critical for understanding holistic face processing.

整体处理,即把多个特征放在一起处理的强烈倾向,被认为是人脸感知的一个标志。整体效应可以通过几项任务来揭示,包括部分-整体任务、标准复合任务和完整复合任务。虽然通过这些任务很容易观察到整体效应,但这些效应之间缺乏相关性,而且在研究不同人群或不同操作之间的效应时,这些任务的研究结果也不尽相同,这就提出了如何理解这些效应的问题。我们区分了完整综合任务中整体效应的促进效应和干扰效应,发现部分-整体任务中的整体效应似乎与完整综合任务中的促进效应相关,但与干扰效应无关;而标准综合任务中的整体效应与干扰效应相关,但与完整综合任务中的促进效应无关。这些发现表明,明确促进和干扰的作用对于理解整体人脸加工至关重要。
{"title":"Two faces of holistic face processing: Facilitation and interference underlying part-whole and composite effects.","authors":"Haiyang Jin, William G Hayward, Olivia S Cheung","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.13","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.13","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Holistic processing, a strong tendency to process multiple features together, is regarded as a hallmark of face perception. Holistic effects can be revealed by several tasks, including the part-whole task, standard composite task, and complete composite task. Although holistic effects are readily observed using these tasks, the lack of correlations among these effects and the mixed findings across these tasks when examining the effects among various populations or manipulations pose questions about how these effects should be understood. We distinguished facilitation and interference effects within the holistic effects in the complete composite task and found that the holistic effect in the part-whole task appeared to be correlated with facilitation but not interference in the complete composite task, whereas the holistic effect in the standard composite task was correlated with interference but not facilitation in the complete composite task. These findings suggest that clarifying the roles of facilitation and interference is critical for understanding holistic face processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 11","pages":"13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11512570/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Color category and inter-item interaction influence color working memory codependently. 颜色类别和项目间的交互作用对颜色工作记忆的影响是相互依存的。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.5
Mengdan Sun, Xinyue Yang, Chundi Wang

Our brains do not always encode visual information in a veridical way. Visual working memory (WM) for features such as color can be biased. WM bias comes from several sources. Category priors can lead to WM bias. For example, color WM is biased toward or away from category prototypes. In addition to category knowledge, contextual factors can induce and modulate WM bias; however, these biases of different sources have usually been investigated independently with different tasks. The present study sought to explore how color WM is influenced by both color category and concurrent distractor. Specifically, we asked participants to retain two color items in WM to investigate how the WM representation of the target color is biased by learned category knowledge and contextual inter-item interactions. Our study found that the WM representation of the target color is biased toward or away from the category prototypes and away from the distractor color that is simultaneously held in WM, indicating that both color category and concurrent distractor bias color WM. More importantly, the weight of these two biases depends on the specific color category, suggesting that category priors and inter-item interaction biases are not simply additive but flexible. Furthermore, we revealed that both types of biases arise from perceptual processes.

我们的大脑并不总是以真实的方式对视觉信息进行编码。针对颜色等特征的视觉工作记忆(WM)可能存在偏差。WM偏差有多种来源。类别先验会导致 WM 偏差。例如,颜色工作记忆会偏向或偏离类别原型。除了类别知识外,情境因素也会诱发和调节 WM 偏差;然而,这些不同来源的偏差通常都是通过不同的任务进行独立研究的。本研究试图探讨颜色 WM 如何同时受到颜色类别和同时出现的分心物的影响。具体来说,我们要求被试在 WM 中保留两个颜色项目,以研究目标颜色的 WM 表征如何受到所学类别知识和项目间上下文相互作用的影响。我们的研究发现,目标颜色的 WM 表征偏向或偏离类别原型,偏离同时保留在 WM 中的分心颜色,这表明颜色类别和同时出现的分心颜色都会偏向颜色 WM。更重要的是,这两种偏向的权重取决于具体的颜色类别,这表明类别先验和项目间交互偏向不是简单的相加,而是灵活的。此外,我们还发现这两种偏差都来自知觉过程。
{"title":"Color category and inter-item interaction influence color working memory codependently.","authors":"Mengdan Sun, Xinyue Yang, Chundi Wang","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.9.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our brains do not always encode visual information in a veridical way. Visual working memory (WM) for features such as color can be biased. WM bias comes from several sources. Category priors can lead to WM bias. For example, color WM is biased toward or away from category prototypes. In addition to category knowledge, contextual factors can induce and modulate WM bias; however, these biases of different sources have usually been investigated independently with different tasks. The present study sought to explore how color WM is influenced by both color category and concurrent distractor. Specifically, we asked participants to retain two color items in WM to investigate how the WM representation of the target color is biased by learned category knowledge and contextual inter-item interactions. Our study found that the WM representation of the target color is biased toward or away from the category prototypes and away from the distractor color that is simultaneously held in WM, indicating that both color category and concurrent distractor bias color WM. More importantly, the weight of these two biases depends on the specific color category, suggesting that category priors and inter-item interaction biases are not simply additive but flexible. Furthermore, we revealed that both types of biases arise from perceptual processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 9","pages":"5"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11383193/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141628","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Foveal neural adaptation to optically induced contrast reduction. 眼窝神经对光学诱导的对比度降低的适应。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.13
Antonia Roth,Katharina Breher,Niklas Domdei,Siegfried Wahl
Contrast processing is suggested to interact with eye growth and myopia development. A novel contrast-reducing myopia control lens design decreases image contrast and was shown to slow myopia progression. Limited insights exist regarding neural visual processing following adaptation to image contrast reduction. This study investigated foveal neural contrast sensitivity in 29 young adults following a 30-minute adaptation to scattering using a Bangerter occlusion foil 0.8, +0.5-diopter defocus, and a clear lens control condition. Neural contrast sensitivity at its peak sensitivity of 6 cycles per degree was assessed before and after adaptation to the lens conditions, employing a unique interferometric system. Pre-adaptation measurements were averaged from six replicates and post-adaptation measurements by the first and last three of six replicates. The change in neural contrast sensitivity was largest for scattering across the first and last three post-adaptation measurements (+0.05 ± 0.01 logCS and +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS, respectively) compared with control and defocus (all +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS). For scattering, the observed increase of neural contrast sensitivity within the first three measurements differed significantly from the pre-adaptation baseline (p = 0.04) and was significantly higher compared with the control condition (p = 0.04). The sensitivity increases in the control and defocus conditions were not significant (all p > 0.05). As the adaptation effect diminished, no significant differences were found from baseline or between the conditions in the last three measurements (all p > 0.05). When post-adaptation neural contrast sensitivities were clustered into 25-second sequences, a significant effect was observed between the conditions, with only a significant relevant effect between control and scattering at 25 seconds (p = 0.04) and no further significant effects (all p > 0.05). The alteration in neural contrast sensitivity at peak sensitivity was most pronounced following adaptation to the scattering condition compared with defocus and control, suggesting that induced scattering might be considered for myopia control.
对比度处理被认为与眼睛的生长和近视的发展相互影响。一种新颖的降低对比度的近视控制镜片设计可降低图像对比度,并能减缓近视的发展。关于适应图像对比度降低后的神经视觉处理,目前的研究还很有限。本研究调查了 29 名年轻成年人在使用 Bangerter 遮盖箔 0.8、+0.5-diopter 散焦和透明镜片控制条件下进行 30 分钟散焦适应后的眼窝神经对比敏感度。采用独特的干涉测量系统,在适应镜片条件之前和之后,评估了神经对比敏感度在每度 6 个周期的峰值敏感度。适应前的测量结果取六个重复样本的平均值,适应后的测量结果取六个重复样本中前三个和后三个的平均值。与对照组和散焦组(均为 +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS)相比,散射组神经对比敏感度在适应后的前三次和后三次测量中变化最大(分别为 +0.05 ± 0.01 logCS 和 +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS)。对于散焦,在前三次测量中观察到的神经对比灵敏度的增加与适应前的基线有显著差异(p = 0.04),并且与对照组相比显著更高(p = 0.04)。在对照和散焦条件下,灵敏度的增加并不显著(所有 p > 0.05)。随着适应效应的减弱,最后三次测量结果与基线或不同条件下的测量结果均无显著差异(均 p > 0.05)。当将适应后的神经对比敏感度集中到 25 秒序列时,观察到不同条件之间存在显著效应,只有在 25 秒时对照组和散射组之间存在显著相关效应(p = 0.04),并且没有进一步的显著效应(所有 p > 0.05)。与散焦和对照组相比,适应散射条件后神经对比敏感度峰值的改变最为明显,这表明可考虑将诱导散射用于近视控制。
{"title":"Foveal neural adaptation to optically induced contrast reduction.","authors":"Antonia Roth,Katharina Breher,Niklas Domdei,Siegfried Wahl","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.13","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.13","url":null,"abstract":"Contrast processing is suggested to interact with eye growth and myopia development. A novel contrast-reducing myopia control lens design decreases image contrast and was shown to slow myopia progression. Limited insights exist regarding neural visual processing following adaptation to image contrast reduction. This study investigated foveal neural contrast sensitivity in 29 young adults following a 30-minute adaptation to scattering using a Bangerter occlusion foil 0.8, +0.5-diopter defocus, and a clear lens control condition. Neural contrast sensitivity at its peak sensitivity of 6 cycles per degree was assessed before and after adaptation to the lens conditions, employing a unique interferometric system. Pre-adaptation measurements were averaged from six replicates and post-adaptation measurements by the first and last three of six replicates. The change in neural contrast sensitivity was largest for scattering across the first and last three post-adaptation measurements (+0.05 ± 0.01 logCS and +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS, respectively) compared with control and defocus (all +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS). For scattering, the observed increase of neural contrast sensitivity within the first three measurements differed significantly from the pre-adaptation baseline (p = 0.04) and was significantly higher compared with the control condition (p = 0.04). The sensitivity increases in the control and defocus conditions were not significant (all p > 0.05). As the adaptation effect diminished, no significant differences were found from baseline or between the conditions in the last three measurements (all p > 0.05). When post-adaptation neural contrast sensitivities were clustered into 25-second sequences, a significant effect was observed between the conditions, with only a significant relevant effect between control and scattering at 25 seconds (p = 0.04) and no further significant effects (all p > 0.05). The alteration in neural contrast sensitivity at peak sensitivity was most pronounced following adaptation to the scattering condition compared with defocus and control, suggesting that induced scattering might be considered for myopia control.","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"26 1","pages":"13"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142266496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual event boundaries trigger forgetting despite active maintenance in visual working memory. 尽管视觉工作记忆中存在着积极的维护,但视觉事件边界仍会引发遗忘。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.9
Joan Danielle K Ongchoco,Yaoda Xu
The contents of visual perception are inherently dynamic-just as we experience objects in space, so too events in time. The boundaries between these events have downstream consequences. For example, memory for incidentally encountered items is impaired when walking through a doorway, perhaps because event boundaries serve as cues to clear obsolete information from previous events. Although this kind of "memory flushing" can be adaptive, work on visual working memory (VWM) has focused on the opposite function of active maintenance in the face of distraction. How do these two cognitive operations interact? In this study, observers watched animations in which they walked through three-dimensionally rendered rooms with picture frames on the walls. Within the frames, observers either saw images that they had to remember ("encoding") or recalled images they had seen in the immediately preceding frame ("test"). Half of the time, a doorway was crossed during the delay between encoding and test. Across experiments, there was a consistent memory decrement for the first image encoded in the doorway compared to the no-doorway condition while equating time elapsed, distance traveled, and distractibility of the doorway. This decrement despite top-down VWM efforts highlights the power of event boundaries to structure what and when we forget.
视觉感知的内容本质上是动态的--就像我们体验空间中的物体一样,时间中的事件也是动态的。这些事件之间的界限会产生下游影响。例如,当穿过一个门洞时,对偶然遇到的物品的记忆就会受损,这可能是因为事件的边界是清除先前事件中过时信息的线索。尽管这种 "记忆冲洗 "可能是适应性的,但有关视觉工作记忆(VWM)的研究却集中在面对分心时主动维护的相反功能上。这两种认知操作是如何相互作用的呢?在这项研究中,观察者观看动画,在动画中,他们穿过三维渲染的房间,房间的墙壁上有画框。在画框中,观察者要么看到他们必须记住的图像("编码"),要么回忆起他们在前一画框中看到的图像("测试")。在编码和测试之间的延迟时间内,有一半的时间是穿过门廊的。在所有实验中,与无门道条件相比,在门道中编码的第一幅图像的记忆力会持续下降,而所花时间、所走距离和门道的分心程度都是相同的。尽管自上而下的 VWM 发挥了作用,但这种记忆力下降的现象还是突显了事件边界对我们遗忘的内容和遗忘时间的影响。
{"title":"Visual event boundaries trigger forgetting despite active maintenance in visual working memory.","authors":"Joan Danielle K Ongchoco,Yaoda Xu","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.9","url":null,"abstract":"The contents of visual perception are inherently dynamic-just as we experience objects in space, so too events in time. The boundaries between these events have downstream consequences. For example, memory for incidentally encountered items is impaired when walking through a doorway, perhaps because event boundaries serve as cues to clear obsolete information from previous events. Although this kind of \"memory flushing\" can be adaptive, work on visual working memory (VWM) has focused on the opposite function of active maintenance in the face of distraction. How do these two cognitive operations interact? In this study, observers watched animations in which they walked through three-dimensionally rendered rooms with picture frames on the walls. Within the frames, observers either saw images that they had to remember (\"encoding\") or recalled images they had seen in the immediately preceding frame (\"test\"). Half of the time, a doorway was crossed during the delay between encoding and test. Across experiments, there was a consistent memory decrement for the first image encoded in the doorway compared to the no-doorway condition while equating time elapsed, distance traveled, and distractibility of the doorway. This decrement despite top-down VWM efforts highlights the power of event boundaries to structure what and when we forget.","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"122 1","pages":"9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142182510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial ambiguity and perception: How face-likeness affects breaking time in continuous flash suppression. 面部模糊性与感知:脸部相似性如何影响连续闪光抑制的中断时间。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.18
Michael Makoto Martinsen, Kairi Yoshino, Yuya Kinzuka, Fumiaki Sato, Hideki Tamura, Tetsuto Minami, Shigeki Nakauchi

Previous studies have elucidated that humans can implicitly process faces faster than they process objects. However, the mechanism through which the brain unconsciously processes ambiguous facial images remains unclear. In our experiment, upright and inverted black-and-white binary face stimuli were presented in a two-alternative forced-choice location discrimination task combined with continuous flash suppression, a technique that suppresses visual stimuli perception using rapidly changing masks. The breaking time (BT) or the time required for a stimulus to be perceptually recognized was recorded for each face stimulus. The results showed that the BT for inverted grayscale images was significantly longer than that for upright grayscale faces, whereas the BT for upright and inverted binary faces did not reach statistical significance. A significant correlation between face likeness and BT was established after evaluating face likeness for each binary face stimulus, with high-face-like binary faces exhibiting shorter BT and low-face-like stimuli resulting in a more prolonged BT. Our results suggest that even an ambiguous object rated highly in face likeness can reduce the BT under implicit processing, indicating the possibility that facial parts such as the eyes and nose are subconsciously detected in ambiguous facial stimuli, enabling facial perception.

以往的研究已经阐明,人类对人脸的内隐处理速度快于对物体的处理速度。然而,大脑在无意识中处理模棱两可的面部图像的机制仍不清楚。在我们的实验中,直立和倒立的黑白二元人脸刺激被呈现在一个两选一的强迫选择位置辨别任务中,并结合了连续闪光抑制,这是一种利用快速变化的遮罩抑制视觉刺激感知的技术。研究人员记录了每个人脸刺激的中断时间(BT)或刺激被感知识别所需的时间。结果显示,倒置灰度图像的断裂时间明显长于直立灰度人脸的断裂时间,而直立和倒置二元人脸的断裂时间没有达到统计学意义。在对每个二元人脸刺激的人脸相似度进行评估后,人脸相似度与 BT 之间建立了明显的相关性,高人脸相似度的二元人脸表现出更短的 BT,而低人脸相似度的刺激则导致更长的 BT。我们的研究结果表明,即使是脸部相似度高的模糊物体也能在内隐处理中缩短BT,这表明在模糊的脸部刺激中,眼睛和鼻子等脸部部位可能会被潜意识检测到,从而实现脸部感知。
{"title":"Facial ambiguity and perception: How face-likeness affects breaking time in continuous flash suppression.","authors":"Michael Makoto Martinsen, Kairi Yoshino, Yuya Kinzuka, Fumiaki Sato, Hideki Tamura, Tetsuto Minami, Shigeki Nakauchi","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.18","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.18","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have elucidated that humans can implicitly process faces faster than they process objects. However, the mechanism through which the brain unconsciously processes ambiguous facial images remains unclear. In our experiment, upright and inverted black-and-white binary face stimuli were presented in a two-alternative forced-choice location discrimination task combined with continuous flash suppression, a technique that suppresses visual stimuli perception using rapidly changing masks. The breaking time (BT) or the time required for a stimulus to be perceptually recognized was recorded for each face stimulus. The results showed that the BT for inverted grayscale images was significantly longer than that for upright grayscale faces, whereas the BT for upright and inverted binary faces did not reach statistical significance. A significant correlation between face likeness and BT was established after evaluating face likeness for each binary face stimulus, with high-face-like binary faces exhibiting shorter BT and low-face-like stimuli resulting in a more prolonged BT. Our results suggest that even an ambiguous object rated highly in face likeness can reduce the BT under implicit processing, indicating the possibility that facial parts such as the eyes and nose are subconsciously detected in ambiguous facial stimuli, enabling facial perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 9","pages":"18"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11437706/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331294","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterizing serial dependence as an attraction to prior response. 将序列依赖性描述为对先前反应的吸引力。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.16
Geoffrey K Gallagher, Christopher P Benton

Serial dependence refers to a common misperception that can occur between subsequently observed stimuli. Observers misreport the current stimulus as being more similar to the previous stimulus than it objectively is. It has been proposed that this bias may reflect an attraction of the current percept to prior percept (Fischer & Whitney, 2014). Alternatively, serial dependence has also been proposed to be the result of an assimilative effect between observer decisions (Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019). Lying within this debate is the issue of how we quantify serial dependence. Should this be as a bias induced by prior stimuli or by prior responses? We investigated this by manipulating the orientation of the current stimuli such that they fell between previous stimulus and previous response. We observed an attraction to previous response and a concomitant repulsion from previous stimulus. This suggests that the attractive effect of serial dependence in orientation judgments is best quantified in relation to prior response.

序列依赖指的是在随后观察到的刺激之间可能出现的一种常见错觉。观察者会误认为当前刺激与之前刺激的相似度高于客观相似度。有人认为,这种偏差可能反映了当前感知对先前感知的吸引力(Fischer 和 Whitney,2014 年)。另外,序列依赖也被认为是观察者决策之间同化效应的结果(Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019)。这场争论的焦点在于如何量化序列依赖性。这应该是由先前的刺激还是先前的反应引起的偏差?我们通过操纵当前刺激的方向,使其位于先前刺激和先前反应之间,对这一问题进行了研究。我们观察到了对先前反应的吸引和对先前刺激的排斥。这表明,在方位判断中,序列依赖的吸引效应最好与先前的反应联系起来进行量化。
{"title":"Characterizing serial dependence as an attraction to prior response.","authors":"Geoffrey K Gallagher, Christopher P Benton","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.16","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.16","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Serial dependence refers to a common misperception that can occur between subsequently observed stimuli. Observers misreport the current stimulus as being more similar to the previous stimulus than it objectively is. It has been proposed that this bias may reflect an attraction of the current percept to prior percept (Fischer & Whitney, 2014). Alternatively, serial dependence has also been proposed to be the result of an assimilative effect between observer decisions (Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019). Lying within this debate is the issue of how we quantify serial dependence. Should this be as a bias induced by prior stimuli or by prior responses? We investigated this by manipulating the orientation of the current stimuli such that they fell between previous stimulus and previous response. We observed an attraction to previous response and a concomitant repulsion from previous stimulus. This suggests that the attractive effect of serial dependence in orientation judgments is best quantified in relation to prior response.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 9","pages":"16"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11437684/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331293","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Vision
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1