首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Human cognitive system privileges processing over short-term storage: Asymmetry in working memory limitations. 人类认知系统的处理优先于短期存储:工作记忆限制的不对称性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001371
Pierre Barrouillet, Valérie Camos, Julie Pougeon, Julien Beaudet, Pablo Croizet, Clément Belletier

The continuous flow of information in which we are immersed obliges our cognitive system to maintain accessible the relevant elements for the time necessary for their processing. The present study investigated how working memory balances the resource demands of this necessary storage in the face of demanding processing. In four experiments using a complex span task, we examined the residual performance in memory and processing of individuals who performed at their best in the other component. Reciprocal dual-task costs pointed toward a resource sharing between the two functions. However, whereas prioritizing processing almost abolished participants' memory performance, more than 60% of their processing capacities were preserved while maintaining memory performance at span. We argue that this asymmetry might be adaptive in nature. Working memory might have evolved as an action-oriented system in which short-term memory capacity is structurally limited to spare the resources needed for processing the information it holds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们沉浸在持续不断的信息流中,这迫使我们的认知系统在必要的处理时间内保持相关元素的可访问性。本研究调查了工作记忆如何在面对繁重的处理过程时平衡这种必要存储的资源需求。在四项使用复杂跨度任务的实验中,我们考察了在另一个部分表现最佳的人在记忆和处理过程中的剩余表现。互惠的双任务成本表明这两种功能之间存在资源共享。然而,优先处理任务几乎取消了参与者的记忆表现,而在保持跨度记忆表现的同时,却保留了超过 60% 的处理能力。我们认为,这种不对称性在本质上可能是适应性的。工作记忆可能是作为一个以行动为导向的系统进化而来的,在这个系统中,短时记忆容量在结构上是有限的,以便腾出处理所保存信息所需的资源。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Human cognitive system privileges processing over short-term storage: Asymmetry in working memory limitations.","authors":"Pierre Barrouillet, Valérie Camos, Julie Pougeon, Julien Beaudet, Pablo Croizet, Clément Belletier","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001371","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001371","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The continuous flow of information in which we are immersed obliges our cognitive system to maintain accessible the relevant elements for the time necessary for their processing. The present study investigated how working memory balances the resource demands of this necessary storage in the face of demanding processing. In four experiments using a complex span task, we examined the residual performance in memory and processing of individuals who performed at their best in the other component. Reciprocal dual-task costs pointed toward a resource sharing between the two functions. However, whereas prioritizing processing almost abolished participants' memory performance, more than 60% of their processing capacities were preserved while maintaining memory performance at span. We argue that this asymmetry might be adaptive in nature. Working memory might have evolved as an action-oriented system in which short-term memory capacity is structurally limited to spare the resources needed for processing the information it holds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1550-1578"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762242","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prosodic features in production reflect reading comprehension skill in high school students. 语音特征反映高中生的阅读理解能力
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001355
Mara Breen, Julie Van Dyke, Jelena Krivokapić, Nicole Landi

Young children's prosodic fluency correlates with their reading ability, as children who are better early readers also produce more adult-like prosodic cues to syntactic and semantic structure. But less work has explored this question for high school readers, who are more proficient readers, but still exhibit wide variability in reading comprehension skill and prosodic fluency. In the current study, we investigated acoustic indices of prosodic production in high school students (N = 40; ages 13-19) exhibiting a range of reading comprehension skill. Participants read aloud a series of 12 short stories which included simple statements, wh-questions, yes-no questions, quotatives, and ambiguous and unambiguous multiclausal sentences. In addition, to assess the contribution of discourse coherence, sentences were read in either canonical or randomized order. Acoustic cues known to index prosodic phenomena-duration, fundamental frequency, and intensity-were extracted and compared across structures and participants. Results demonstrated that high school readers as a group consistently signal syntactic and semantic structure with prosody, and that reading comprehension skill, above and beyond lower-level skills, correlates with prosodic fluency, as better comprehenders produced stronger prosodic cues. However, discourse coherence did not produce consistent effects. These results strengthen the finding that prosodic fluency and reading comprehension are linked, even for older, proficient readers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

幼儿的拟声流畅性与他们的阅读能力相关,因为早期阅读能力较强的儿童在句法和语义结构方面也会产生更多类似成人的拟声线索。但针对高中生读者的研究较少,他们的阅读能力较强,但在阅读理解能力和拟声流利性方面仍表现出很大的差异。在本研究中,我们调查了高中生(40 人,13-19 岁)在阅读理解能力方面的不同发音指标。受试者朗读了一系列共 12 个短篇故事,其中包括简单陈述句、问句、是非问句、引语以及模棱两可和毫不含糊的多义句。此外,为了评估语篇连贯性的作用,句子的朗读顺序有正则表达式和随机排列两种。我们提取了已知的能反映前音现象的声学线索--持续时间、基频和强度,并在不同结构和参与者之间进行了比较。结果表明,高中生作为一个群体,其句法和语义结构的信号始终与拟声有关,而且阅读理解能力与拟声的流畅性相关,超越了低级技能,因为理解能力强的人产生的拟声线索更强。然而,语篇连贯性并没有产生一致的效果。这些结果进一步证明,即使对于年长的熟练读者来说,拟声流畅性和阅读理解能力也是相关联的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Prosodic features in production reflect reading comprehension skill in high school students.","authors":"Mara Breen, Julie Van Dyke, Jelena Krivokapić, Nicole Landi","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001355","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001355","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Young children's prosodic fluency correlates with their reading ability, as children who are better early readers also produce more adult-like prosodic cues to syntactic and semantic structure. But less work has explored this question for high school readers, who are more proficient readers, but still exhibit wide variability in reading comprehension skill and prosodic fluency. In the current study, we investigated acoustic indices of prosodic production in high school students (<i>N</i> = 40; ages 13-19) exhibiting a range of reading comprehension skill. Participants read aloud a series of 12 short stories which included simple statements, wh-questions, yes-no questions, quotatives, and ambiguous and unambiguous multiclausal sentences. In addition, to assess the contribution of discourse coherence, sentences were read in either canonical or randomized order. Acoustic cues known to index prosodic phenomena-duration, fundamental frequency, and intensity-were extracted and compared across structures and participants. Results demonstrated that high school readers as a group consistently signal syntactic and semantic structure with prosody, and that reading comprehension skill, above and beyond lower-level skills, correlates with prosodic fluency, as better comprehenders produced stronger prosodic cues. However, discourse coherence did not produce consistent effects. These results strengthen the finding that prosodic fluency and reading comprehension are linked, even for older, proficient readers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1662-1682"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11493852/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ensemble memory of a scene interacts with current perception regardless of attentional requirements. 场景的集合记忆与当前感知相互作用,与注意力要求无关。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001332
Yong Min Choi, Jieun Cho, Sang Chul Chong

How do we maintain a rich and stable perceptual experience across the entire visual scene, even when we are focusing on a subset of visual inputs? The current study explored this question by investigating whether the visual system processes summary statistics of multiple features regardless of task relevance, and how they interact with subsequent perception. To test the processing of multifeature summary statistics under different attentional requirements, we presented multiple Gabor patches with heterogeneous orientations/colors and asked participants to attend to a single feature dimension (Experiments 1 and 3) or a single item (Experiment 2) for the memory task. During the memory maintenance period (before memory response), we asked the participants to perform a discrimination task (Experiments 1 and 2) or a boundary localization task (Experiment 3) to test how the memory of the ensemble representation alters the subsequent perceptual experience. We found evidence for obligatory processing of scene summary statistics presented for the memory task, which interacted with the subsequent perceptual sensitivity. Specifically, not only summary statistics relevant but also those of task-irrelevant feature (Experiments 1 and 3) and outside the focus of attention (Experiment 2) were encoded and bidirectionally interacted with subsequent perception. These results suggest obligatory processing of summary statistics of a scene, which may allow rich and stable visual experience by integrating temporally adjacent visual inputs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们是如何在整个视觉场景中保持丰富而稳定的感知体验的?目前的研究通过调查视觉系统是否会处理多个特征的汇总统计而不管任务的相关性,以及它们如何与后续感知相互作用来探索这个问题。为了测试不同注意要求下的多特征汇总统计处理情况,我们呈现了多个具有不同方向/颜色的 Gabor 补丁,并要求参与者在记忆任务中注意单一特征维度(实验 1 和 3)或单一项目(实验 2)。在记忆维持期(记忆反应之前),我们要求参与者执行辨别任务(实验 1 和 2)或边界定位任务(实验 3),以测试对集合表征的记忆如何改变随后的知觉体验。我们发现,有证据表明,在记忆任务中,受试者必须对所呈现的场景摘要统计数据进行处理,而这与随后的感知敏感度相互影响。具体来说,不仅与任务无关的特征(实验 1 和 3)和注意力焦点之外的特征(实验 2)的汇总统计信息被编码,而且与随后的感知产生双向作用。这些结果表明,对场景的概要统计进行强制性处理,可以通过整合时间上相邻的视觉输入,获得丰富而稳定的视觉体验。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Ensemble memory of a scene interacts with current perception regardless of attentional requirements.","authors":"Yong Min Choi, Jieun Cho, Sang Chul Chong","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001332","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001332","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do we maintain a rich and stable perceptual experience across the entire visual scene, even when we are focusing on a subset of visual inputs? The current study explored this question by investigating whether the visual system processes summary statistics of multiple features regardless of task relevance, and how they interact with subsequent perception. To test the processing of multifeature summary statistics under different attentional requirements, we presented multiple Gabor patches with heterogeneous orientations/colors and asked participants to attend to a single feature dimension (Experiments 1 and 3) or a single item (Experiment 2) for the memory task. During the memory maintenance period (before memory response), we asked the participants to perform a discrimination task (Experiments 1 and 2) or a boundary localization task (Experiment 3) to test how the memory of the ensemble representation alters the subsequent perceptual experience. We found evidence for obligatory processing of scene summary statistics presented for the memory task, which interacted with the subsequent perceptual sensitivity. Specifically, not only summary statistics relevant but also those of task-irrelevant feature (Experiments 1 and 3) and outside the focus of attention (Experiment 2) were encoded and bidirectionally interacted with subsequent perception. These results suggest obligatory processing of summary statistics of a scene, which may allow rich and stable visual experience by integrating temporally adjacent visual inputs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1529-1549"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708337","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Better source memory for remembered to-be-forgotten items than for remembered to-be-remembered items. 对已记忆待遗忘项目的源记忆优于对已记忆待记忆项目的源记忆。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001362
Vincent L Ott, Johanna M Höhs, Jan Rummel

When participants study items one-by-one and are directed to either remember or forget the respective item directly after its presentation, retention of to-be-forgotten items is regularly worse than of to-be-remembered items. We tested whether this directed forgetting effect which is regularly observed for item memory generalizes to source memory. In three experiments, participants studied items in two different source colors (N = 101) or at two different source locations (N = 64; N = 81). Sources were manipulated orthogonally to item type (remember vs. forget). At test, we asked participants to recognize all studied items and also to identify their source. We used a multinomial processing tree model to disentangle item memory, source memory, and guessing. In all three experiments, we replicated the directed forgetting effect in item memory. Source memory for to-be-forgotten items that were recognized despite the intention to forget, however, tended to be even better than source memory for to-be-remembered items that were recognized. These results suggest that the directed forgetting effect does not simply translate from item to source memory. Rather source memory seems to be disproportionally increased in to-be-forgotten items that are remembered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当被试者逐个学习项目,并在项目呈现后直接被指示记忆或遗忘相应项目时,待遗忘项目的保持通常比待记忆项目的保持要差。我们测试了在项目记忆中经常观察到的这种定向遗忘效应是否也适用于源记忆。在三个实验中,参与者学习了两种不同来源颜色(N = 101)或两种不同来源位置(N = 64; N = 81)的项目。来源与项目类型(记忆与遗忘)正交排列。测试时,我们要求被试识别所有学习过的项目,并识别其来源。我们使用多叉加工树模型来区分项目记忆、来源记忆和猜测。在所有三个实验中,我们都复制了项目记忆中的定向遗忘效应。然而,对于那些尽管有遗忘意图但还是被识别出来的待遗忘项目,其来源记忆往往比被识别出来的待记忆项目的来源记忆更好。这些结果表明,定向遗忘效应并不能简单地从项目记忆转化为源记忆。相反,在记忆的待遗忘项目中,源记忆似乎不成比例地增加了。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Better source memory for remembered to-be-forgotten items than for remembered to-be-remembered items.","authors":"Vincent L Ott, Johanna M Höhs, Jan Rummel","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001362","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When participants study items one-by-one and are directed to either remember or forget the respective item directly after its presentation, retention of to-be-forgotten items is regularly worse than of to-be-remembered items. We tested whether this directed forgetting effect which is regularly observed for item memory generalizes to source memory. In three experiments, participants studied items in two different source colors (<i>N</i> = 101) or at two different source locations (<i>N</i> = 64; <i>N</i> = 81). Sources were manipulated orthogonally to item type (remember vs. forget). At test, we asked participants to recognize all studied items and also to identify their source. We used a multinomial processing tree model to disentangle item memory, source memory, and guessing. In all three experiments, we replicated the directed forgetting effect in item memory. Source memory for to-be-forgotten items that were recognized despite the intention to forget, however, tended to be even better than source memory for to-be-remembered items that were recognized. These results suggest that the directed forgetting effect does not simply translate from item to source memory. Rather source memory seems to be disproportionally increased in to-be-forgotten items that are remembered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1616-1636"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141200536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Letter identity and position coding in the parafovea. 视网膜旁的字母识别和位置编码
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001393
Aaron Veldre, Lili Yu, Sally Andrews, Erik D Reichle

Letter position coding has been extensively examined in studies of isolated word identification, spurring the development of computational models. However, these models are largely restricted to explaining word identification in foveal vision, despite the fact that early lexical processing during reading occurs in the parafovea. We report four experiments that examined the flexibility of parafoveal letter identity and position coding using a variant of the same-different match task. Participants matched transposed- and substituted-letter strings to reference words, with the former being displayed at various retinal eccentricities for 100 ms versus 300 ms to respectively preclude or allow eye movements. The first pair of experiments demonstrated the relative difficulty of coding parafoveal letter positions as compared to their identities, as well as the standard benefit in identifying words displayed in the right visual field. The second pair of experiments further demonstrated that the location of letter-position uncertainty (i.e., transposed letters) interacts with both eccentricity and visual field. Initial letter transpositions were more easily detected in the left visual field, whereas transpositions of the final letters were more accurately detected in the right visual field. As discussed, these results are challenging for existing models of reading, which can individually account for some of our findings but not the results in their entirety. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

字母位置编码已在孤立词识别研究中得到广泛检验,并推动了计算模型的发展。然而,尽管阅读过程中的早期词汇处理发生在视网膜旁,但这些模型在很大程度上仅限于解释视网膜视觉中的单词识别。我们报告了四项实验,这些实验使用同-异匹配任务的变体考察了视网膜旁字母身份和位置编码的灵活性。参与者将换位字母串和替代字母串与参考单词进行匹配,前者在不同的视网膜偏心率下分别显示 100 毫秒和 300 毫秒,以阻止或允许眼球运动。第一对实验表明,与字母的身份相比,眼底旁字母位置的编码相对困难,而且在识别右侧视野中显示的单词方面也有标准优势。第二组实验进一步证明,字母位置的不确定性(即字母移位)与偏心率和视野都有相互作用。在左侧视野中更容易检测到初始字母的移位,而在右侧视野中更能准确检测到最终字母的移位。如前所述,这些结果对现有的阅读模型提出了挑战,因为现有的阅读模型可以单独解释我们的某些发现,但不能解释全部结果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Letter identity and position coding in the parafovea.","authors":"Aaron Veldre, Lili Yu, Sally Andrews, Erik D Reichle","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001393","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001393","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Letter position coding has been extensively examined in studies of isolated word identification, spurring the development of computational models. However, these models are largely restricted to explaining word identification in foveal vision, despite the fact that early lexical processing during reading occurs in the parafovea. We report four experiments that examined the flexibility of parafoveal letter identity and position coding using a variant of the same-different match task. Participants matched transposed- and substituted-letter strings to reference words, with the former being displayed at various retinal eccentricities for 100 ms versus 300 ms to respectively preclude or allow eye movements. The first pair of experiments demonstrated the relative difficulty of coding parafoveal letter positions as compared to their identities, as well as the standard benefit in identifying words displayed in the right visual field. The second pair of experiments further demonstrated that the location of letter-position uncertainty (i.e., transposed letters) interacts with both eccentricity and visual field. Initial letter transpositions were more easily detected in the left visual field, whereas transpositions of the final letters were more accurately detected in the right visual field. As discussed, these results are challenging for existing models of reading, which can individually account for some of our findings but not the results in their entirety. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"50 10","pages":"1683-1702"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142576655","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Access to inner language enhances memory for events. 获取内在语言可增强对事件的记忆。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001351
Briony Banks, Louise Connell

Events are temporally bounded experiences involving people, objects, and actions that can be segmented into sequences of smaller, meaningful events (e.g., steps involved in constructing a piece of furniture), but the role of inner language in remembering such events has been unclear. We investigated whether inner language enhances memory for events in a naturalistic, nonverbal task where participants constructed simple models from memory. Across three experiments, we used linguistic suppression in a dual-task paradigm to test whether inner language improved overall memory performance and completion time, additionally exploring the number of events that could be recalled. We found that access to inner language at encoding consistently affected memory performance: when inner language was disrupted at encoding, participants were poorer at recalling the models and remembered fewer events. This effect was present whether or not the number of events to be recalled exceed event memory capacity (estimated as approximately seven to eight events). Critically, linguistic suppression impaired memory performance to a greater extent than a control secondary task that did not affect access to language; that is, impairment was not solely due to dual-task interference. The results support the proposal that inner language enhances event memory via a mechanism of linguistic bootstrapping, which makes event representation more efficient by allowing more information to be encoded in an event model even when language is not being used in the task. These findings therefore extend theories of event memory and add to a growing body of evidence that inner language is a highly valuable cognitive tool. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

事件是有时间限制的经历,涉及人、物和动作,可以分割成更小的、有意义的事件序列(例如,建造一件家具的步骤),但内心语言在记忆这类事件中的作用还不清楚。我们通过一项自然的非语言任务,让参与者根据记忆构建简单的模型,来研究内心语言是否能增强对事件的记忆。在三项实验中,我们在双任务范式中使用了语言抑制,以测试内在语言是否能提高整体记忆效果和完成时间,同时还探索了可回忆事件的数量。我们发现,在编码时使用内心语言会持续影响记忆表现:当内心语言在编码时被中断时,参与者回忆模型的能力较差,记住的事件也较少。无论需要回忆的事件数量是否超过事件记忆容量(估计约为 7 到 8 个事件),这种影响都会存在。重要的是,与不影响语言存取的对照次要任务相比,语言抑制对记忆成绩的损害程度更大;也就是说,损害并不完全是由于双重任务干扰造成的。这些结果支持这样一种观点,即内部语言通过语言引导机制增强了事件记忆,即使在任务中没有使用语言时,这种机制也能将更多信息编码到事件模型中,从而使事件表征更加有效。因此,这些发现扩展了事件记忆的理论,并为越来越多的证据增添了新的内容,即内部语言是一种非常有价值的认知工具。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Access to inner language enhances memory for events.","authors":"Briony Banks, Louise Connell","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001351","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001351","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Events are temporally bounded experiences involving people, objects, and actions that can be segmented into sequences of smaller, meaningful events (e.g., steps involved in constructing a piece of furniture), but the role of inner language in remembering such events has been unclear. We investigated whether inner language enhances memory for events in a naturalistic, nonverbal task where participants constructed simple models from memory. Across three experiments, we used linguistic suppression in a dual-task paradigm to test whether inner language improved overall memory performance and completion time, additionally exploring the number of events that could be recalled. We found that access to inner language at encoding consistently affected memory performance: when inner language was disrupted at encoding, participants were poorer at recalling the models and remembered fewer events. This effect was present whether or not the number of events to be recalled exceed event memory capacity (estimated as approximately seven to eight events). Critically, linguistic suppression impaired memory performance to a greater extent than a control secondary task that did not affect access to language; that is, impairment was not solely due to dual-task interference. The results support the proposal that inner language enhances event memory via a mechanism of linguistic bootstrapping, which makes event representation more efficient by allowing more information to be encoded in an event model even when language is not being used in the task. These findings therefore extend theories of event memory and add to a growing body of evidence that inner language is a highly valuable cognitive tool. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1592-1615"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141460491","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Involuntary and voluntary processes compete for entering the focus of attention of working memory. 非自愿过程和自愿过程竞相进入工作记忆的注意力焦点。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001374
Jiaofeng Li, Yubo Qiu, Weijie Guo, Huayu Liao, Mowei Shen, Zaifeng Gao

Representations in the focus of attention (FoA) of working memory (WM) have the highest activation state and processing privilege among representations in WM. There are two distinct processes for representations entering the FoA: involuntary and voluntary. The former is an automatic attentional response to stimuli, while the latter is directed by the central executive. Although extensive WM research has examined these processes individually, their interaction, particularly in competitive scenarios, remains poorly understood. To address this, we conducted experiments by displaying memorized stimuli that contain a color singleton to trigger an involuntary process, followed by a retro-cue in the WM maintenance phase to initiate a voluntary process. By manipulating the retro-cue validity, we probed how the singleton effect was modulated when the two processes had distinct targets. Our findings indicated that when the retro-cue validity was low, the singleton effect remained unaffected by a retro-cue directing to a nonsingleton target. However, when the retro-cue validity was high, the singleton effect was eliminated on reaction time, suggesting that involuntary and voluntary processes compete for a limited capacity of the FoA, with the stronger one prevailing in this competition. These findings illuminate the intricate interplay between involuntary and voluntary attentional processes in WM and offer critical insights into the nature and allocation mechanisms of the FoA. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在工作记忆(WM)的表征中,注意力焦点(FoA)中的表征具有最高的激活状态和处理特权。表征进入 FoA 有两个不同的过程:非自主和自主。前者是对刺激的自动注意反应,而后者则是由中央执行器引导的。尽管大量的 WM 研究已经对这两个过程进行了单独研究,但对它们之间的相互作用,尤其是在竞争情景中的相互作用,仍然知之甚少。为了解决这个问题,我们进行了一些实验,通过显示包含单个颜色的记忆性刺激来触发非自主过程,然后在 WM 维持阶段通过回溯提示来启动自主过程。通过操纵回溯线索的有效性,我们探究了当两个过程具有不同的目标时,单子效应是如何被调节的。我们的研究结果表明,当回溯线索有效性较低时,单子效应不受指向非单子目标的回溯线索的影响。然而,当回溯线索有效性较高时,单子效应对反应时间的影响被消除,这表明非自主和自主过程在争夺有限的FoA容量,而较强的过程在竞争中占上风。这些发现揭示了WM中非自主和自主注意过程之间错综复杂的相互作用,并对FoA的性质和分配机制提供了重要的启示。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Involuntary and voluntary processes compete for entering the focus of attention of working memory.","authors":"Jiaofeng Li, Yubo Qiu, Weijie Guo, Huayu Liao, Mowei Shen, Zaifeng Gao","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001374","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001374","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Representations in the focus of attention (FoA) of working memory (WM) have the highest activation state and processing privilege among representations in WM. There are two distinct processes for representations entering the FoA: involuntary and voluntary. The former is an automatic attentional response to stimuli, while the latter is directed by the central executive. Although extensive WM research has examined these processes individually, their interaction, particularly in competitive scenarios, remains poorly understood. To address this, we conducted experiments by displaying memorized stimuli that contain a color singleton to trigger an involuntary process, followed by a retro-cue in the WM maintenance phase to initiate a voluntary process. By manipulating the retro-cue validity, we probed how the singleton effect was modulated when the two processes had distinct targets. Our findings indicated that when the retro-cue validity was low, the singleton effect remained unaffected by a retro-cue directing to a nonsingleton target. However, when the retro-cue validity was high, the singleton effect was eliminated on reaction time, suggesting that involuntary and voluntary processes compete for a limited capacity of the FoA, with the stronger one prevailing in this competition. These findings illuminate the intricate interplay between involuntary and voluntary attentional processes in WM and offer critical insights into the nature and allocation mechanisms of the FoA. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331490","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Valence sound symbolism facilitates classification of vowels and emotional facial expressions. 价音符号学有助于对元音和情绪面部表情进行分类。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001389
David Schmidtke, Anita Körner, Sarah Glim, Ralf Rummer

Research on iconicity or sound symbolism chronicles associations between aspects of word meaning and word form. In valence sound symbolism, specifically, vowels were demonstrated to be associated with valence, so that the vowel /i/ (as in English sea) is judged to fit positively valenced stimuli, whereas the vowel /o/ (as in German Boot) is judged to fit negatively valenced stimuli. However, because previous findings were based on judgments or name invention tasks, they may reflect deliberate strategies. The present research aims to demonstrate the automaticity of valence sound symbolism in online language processing by means of a speeded response task. A total of 163 participants performed a speeded categorization task in which they were requested to classify one of two simultaneously presented stimuli, pseudowords containing the vowels /i/ or /o/ and pictures with positive or negative emotional facial expressions. Thus, stimulus pairings could be either congruent (/i/-words and positive expressions; /o/-words and negative expressions) or incongruent (/i/-words and negative expressions; /o/-words and positive expressions). We observed faster and more accurate responses for congruent over incongruent combinations (the simple effects were significant for positive but not for negative expressions). Thus, sound symbolic congruency (vs. incongruency) of an irrelevant stimulus facilitated responses, which accords with an automatic process contributing to valence sound symbolism and indicates that valence sound symbolism is deeply engrained in the human cognitive system. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

图标性或声音符号学研究记录了词义与词形之间的关联。具体来说,在价音符号学中,元音被证明与价相关联,因此元音/i/(如英语中的sea)被判断为适合正面的价音刺激,而元音/o/(如德语中的Boot)则被判断为适合负面的价音刺激。然而,由于之前的研究结果是基于判断或名称发明任务得出的,它们可能反映了刻意的策略。本研究旨在通过一项加速反应任务来证明在线语言处理中的价音符号自动性。共有 163 名受试者完成了一项快速分类任务,要求他们对同时呈现的两个刺激物(包含元音 /i/ 或 /o/ 的假词和带有积极或消极情绪面部表情的图片)中的一个进行分类。因此,刺激配对既可以是一致的(/i/-单词和正面表情;/o/-单词和负面表情),也可以是不一致的(/i/-单词和负面表情;/o/-单词和正面表情)。我们观察到,与不一致的组合相比,一致的组合反应更快、更准确(简单效应对正向表达有显著影响,但对负向表达则无显著影响)。因此,无关刺激的声音符号一致(与不一致)会促进反应,这与有助于价位声音符号的自动过程相一致,并表明价位声音符号在人类认知系统中根深蒂固。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Valence sound symbolism facilitates classification of vowels and emotional facial expressions.","authors":"David Schmidtke, Anita Körner, Sarah Glim, Ralf Rummer","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001389","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001389","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on iconicity or sound symbolism chronicles associations between aspects of word meaning and word form. In valence sound symbolism, specifically, vowels were demonstrated to be associated with valence, so that the vowel /i/ (as in English sea) is judged to fit positively valenced stimuli, whereas the vowel /o/ (as in German <i>Boot</i>) is judged to fit negatively valenced stimuli. However, because previous findings were based on judgments or name invention tasks, they may reflect deliberate strategies. The present research aims to demonstrate the automaticity of valence sound symbolism in online language processing by means of a speeded response task. A total of 163 participants performed a speeded categorization task in which they were requested to classify one of two simultaneously presented stimuli, pseudowords containing the vowels /i/ or /o/ and pictures with positive or negative emotional facial expressions. Thus, stimulus pairings could be either congruent (/i/-words and positive expressions; /o/-words and negative expressions) or incongruent (/i/-words and negative expressions; /o/-words and positive expressions). We observed faster and more accurate responses for congruent over incongruent combinations (the simple effects were significant for positive but not for negative expressions). Thus, sound symbolic congruency (vs. incongruency) of an irrelevant stimulus facilitated responses, which accords with an automatic process contributing to valence sound symbolism and indicates that valence sound symbolism is deeply engrained in the human cognitive system. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331491","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A neural index reflecting the amount of cognitive resources available during memory encoding: A model-based approach. 反映记忆编码过程中可用认知资源量的神经指数:基于模型的方法
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001364
Si Ma, Vencislav Popov, Qiong Zhang

Humans have a limited amount of cognitive resources to process various cognitive operations at a given moment. The Source of Activation Confusion model of episodic memory proposes that resources are consumed during each processing, and once depleted, they need time to recover gradually. This has been supported by a series of behavioral findings in the past. However, the neural substrate of the resources is not known. In the present study, over an existing electroencephalogram data set of a free recall task (Kahana et al., 2022), we provided a neural index reflecting the amount of cognitive resources available for forming new memory traces. Unique to our approach, we obtained the neural index not through correlating neural patterns with behavior outcomes or experimental conditions, but by demonstrating its alignment with a latent quantity of cognitive resources inferred from the Source of Activation Confusion model. In addition, we showed that the identified neural index can be used to propose novel hypothesis regarding other long-term memory phenomena. Specifically, we found that according to the neural index, neural encoding patterns for subsequently recalled items correspond to greater available cognitive resources compared with those for subsequently unrecalled items. This provides a mechanistic account for the long-established subsequent memory effects (i.e., differential neural encoding patterns between subsequently recalled vs. subsequently unrecalled items), which has been previously associated with attention, fatigue, and properties of the stimuli. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类在特定时刻处理各种认知操作的认知资源是有限的。外显记忆的 "激活源混淆模型 "提出,资源在每次处理过程中都会消耗,一旦耗尽,就需要时间来逐渐恢复。过去的一系列行为学研究结果都支持这一观点。然而,这些资源的神经基质尚不清楚。在本研究中,通过现有的自由回忆任务脑电图数据集(Kahana 等人,2022 年),我们提供了一个神经指数,反映了可用于形成新记忆痕迹的认知资源量。我们的方法与众不同,不是通过将神经模式与行为结果或实验条件相关联,而是通过证明其与从激活源混淆模型中推断出的认知资源的潜在数量相一致,从而获得神经指数。此外,我们还证明了所识别的神经指数可用于提出有关其他长期记忆现象的新假设。具体来说,我们发现根据神经指数,与随后未被回忆的项目相比,随后被回忆项目的神经编码模式对应于更大的可用认知资源。这为长期以来形成的后续记忆效应(即后续回忆项目与后续未回忆项目之间的不同神经编码模式)提供了一个机理解释,而后续记忆效应与注意力、疲劳和刺激物的特性有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A neural index reflecting the amount of cognitive resources available during memory encoding: A model-based approach.","authors":"Si Ma, Vencislav Popov, Qiong Zhang","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001364","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001364","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans have a limited amount of cognitive resources to process various cognitive operations at a given moment. The Source of Activation Confusion model of episodic memory proposes that resources are consumed during each processing, and once depleted, they need time to recover gradually. This has been supported by a series of behavioral findings in the past. However, the neural substrate of the resources is not known. In the present study, over an existing electroencephalogram data set of a free recall task (Kahana et al., 2022), we provided a neural index reflecting the amount of cognitive resources available for forming new memory traces. Unique to our approach, we obtained the neural index not through correlating neural patterns with behavior outcomes or experimental conditions, but by demonstrating its alignment with a latent quantity of cognitive resources inferred from the Source of Activation Confusion model. In addition, we showed that the identified neural index can be used to propose novel hypothesis regarding other long-term memory phenomena. Specifically, we found that according to the neural index, neural encoding patterns for subsequently recalled items correspond to greater available cognitive resources compared with those for subsequently unrecalled items. This provides a mechanistic account for the long-established subsequent memory effects (i.e., differential neural encoding patterns between subsequently recalled vs. subsequently unrecalled items), which has been previously associated with attention, fatigue, and properties of the stimuli. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word concreteness modulates bilingual language control during reading comprehension. 词汇的具体性会调节阅读理解过程中的双语语言控制。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001385
Junjie Wu, Hongyu Zhao, Xinye Wu, Qianming Liu, Juan Su, Yannan Ji, Qiping Wang

Controversies persist in the literature regarding the existence of bilingual language control during comprehension, which may be attributed to overlooking the modulating effect of word concreteness. To test this hypothesis, we conducted an experiment using abstract and concrete words, thereby manipulating the activation level of the nontarget language. Sixty Chinese-English bilinguals were instructed to switch between two languages in word reading tasks. We found that abstract words (e.g., [correct], wrong) did not show switching costs, indicating no additional time for switching between languages compared to repeating the same language. In contrast, concrete words (e.g., [sunny], rainy) elicited significant larger switching costs. These findings might suggest greater language control demands on the nontarget language when reading more concrete words. This study offers insights into the modulating effect of word concreteness in language processing on bilingual language control during reading comprehension. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于理解过程中是否存在二语语言控制的文献一直存在争议,这可能是由于忽略了词语具体性的调节作用。为了验证这一假设,我们使用抽象词和具体词进行了一项实验,从而操纵了非目标语言的激活水平。我们指导 60 名汉英双语者在单词阅读任务中切换两种语言。我们发现,抽象词(如[正确]、错误)没有显示出切换成本,这表明与重复同一语言相比,在不同语言之间切换不需要额外的时间。相反,具体词语(如[sunny]、rainy)的转换成本明显更高。这些发现可能表明,在阅读更具体的词语时,对非目标语言的语言控制要求更高。本研究有助于深入了解语言处理中词语的具体性对阅读理解过程中二语语言控制的调节作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Word concreteness modulates bilingual language control during reading comprehension.","authors":"Junjie Wu, Hongyu Zhao, Xinye Wu, Qianming Liu, Juan Su, Yannan Ji, Qiping Wang","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001385","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001385","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Controversies persist in the literature regarding the existence of bilingual language control during comprehension, which may be attributed to overlooking the modulating effect of word concreteness. To test this hypothesis, we conducted an experiment using abstract and concrete words, thereby manipulating the activation level of the nontarget language. Sixty Chinese-English bilinguals were instructed to switch between two languages in word reading tasks. We found that abstract words (e.g., [<i>correct</i>], <i>wrong</i>) did not show switching costs, indicating no additional time for switching between languages compared to repeating the same language. In contrast, concrete words (e.g., [<i>sunny</i>], <i>rainy</i>) elicited significant larger switching costs. These findings might suggest greater language control demands on the nontarget language when reading more concrete words. This study offers insights into the modulating effect of word concreteness in language processing on bilingual language control during reading comprehension. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1