首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task. 词缀不能阻止对模糊词根的语义访问--即使在句子语境中也不能:来自眼动跟踪和迷宫任务的证据。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001378
Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben

How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., bark, with meanings related to both "dog" and "tree") which are disambiguated when affixed by -ing (e.g., barking; related to "dog" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root bark would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as barking (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (dog, tree) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form (barking-tree), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (night) continuation for the sentence He heard loud barking during the … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在句子阅读过程中,语言理解系统是如何识别和解释单词成分或词素的?我们通过使用含有语义模糊的词根(例如,树皮,其含义与 "狗 "和 "树 "都有关)的词语来研究这个问题,这些词根在加上-ing(例如,吠叫;只与 "狗 "有关)后,其含义就变得不明确了。我们的目的是要了解对词根 "bark "意义的高层访问是否会受到低层形态词缀的限制。在实验 1 中,我们使用眼动追踪技术,让被试阅读含有语义模糊词根的句子,如 "吠叫"(质点),同时阅读与词根的两种含义(狗、树)相关的目标句,或者阅读与词根无关的对照句。我们采用的所有五种眼动跟踪测量方法(首次定格持续时间、注视持续时间、经过时间、总阅读时间和向目标回归时间)都显示,与词根相关的两个目标之间没有差异,它们比苜蓿语慢,但比无关目标快。结果表明,即使在词义与完整词形不一致的情况下(barking-tree),模糊词根的两个词义都会被激活。实验 2 采用了一项迷宫任务来支持这些结果,在该任务中,由于出现了与树皮的两种含义相关的干扰物,因此选择 "他在......时听到了响亮的狗叫声 "这一句子的 "掐头去尾(夜晚)"续句的时间被打乱了。我们讨论了这些发现对句子语境中形态解析和词汇歧义解决的本质的影响。我们认为,单词识别和词汇访问过程涉及词根和词缀的分离,从而产生独立和详尽的词根意义访问--即使词缀和上下文排除了词根意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task.","authors":"Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001378","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001378","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., <i>bark</i>, with meanings related to both \"dog\" and \"tree\") which are disambiguated when affixed by -<i>ing</i> (e.g., <i>barking</i>; related to \"dog\" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root <i>bark</i> would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as <i>barking</i> (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (<i>dog</i>, <i>tree</i>) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form <i>(barking-tree</i>), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (<i>night</i>) continuation for the sentence <i>He heard loud barking during the</i> … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The importance of conative factors for individual differences in attention control. 内涵因素对注意力控制个体差异的重要性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001356
Nash Unsworth, Ashley L Miller

Relations between conative factors (task-specific motivation, attention self-efficacy, and self-set goals) and individual differences in attention control (AC) performance were investigated in two latent variable studies. Participants performed AC tasks along with measures of working memory and processing speed. During the AC tasks, participants self-reported their motivation, self-efficacy, and self-set goals for the tasks. Task-unrelated thoughts were also assessed. Confirmatory factor analyses demonstrated that latent factors for the constructs could be formed and the conative factors were each related to the AC factor. Structural equation modeling further suggested that the conative factors tended to account for unique variance in attention, even after accounting for shared variance with working memory and processing speed. These results provide evidence that conative factors are important for individual differences in AC and further suggest that multiple factors likely contribute to variation in performance on AC tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在两项潜变量研究中,研究人员调查了意向因素(特定任务动机、注意力自我效能感和自我设定目标)与注意力控制(AC)表现的个体差异之间的关系。参与者在完成注意力控制任务的同时,还进行了工作记忆和处理速度的测量。在完成注意力控制任务期间,受试者自我报告了他们的动机、自我效能感和自我设定的任务目标。此外,还对与任务无关的想法进行了评估。确认性因素分析表明,可以形成这些建构的潜在因素,并且每一个内涵因素都与交流因素相关。结构方程建模进一步表明,即使考虑了与工作记忆和处理速度之间的共同差异,内省因素仍倾向于解释注意力的独特差异。这些结果证明,内省因素对AC的个体差异非常重要,并进一步表明,多种因素可能会导致AC任务中的表现差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The importance of conative factors for individual differences in attention control.","authors":"Nash Unsworth, Ashley L Miller","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001356","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001356","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Relations between conative factors (task-specific motivation, attention self-efficacy, and self-set goals) and individual differences in attention control (AC) performance were investigated in two latent variable studies. Participants performed AC tasks along with measures of working memory and processing speed. During the AC tasks, participants self-reported their motivation, self-efficacy, and self-set goals for the tasks. Task-unrelated thoughts were also assessed. Confirmatory factor analyses demonstrated that latent factors for the constructs could be formed and the conative factors were each related to the AC factor. Structural equation modeling further suggested that the conative factors tended to account for unique variance in attention, even after accounting for shared variance with working memory and processing speed. These results provide evidence that conative factors are important for individual differences in AC and further suggest that multiple factors likely contribute to variation in performance on AC tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447464","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of filler similarity and lineup size on eyewitness identification. 填充物相似性和阵容大小对目击者识别的影响。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001342
Kyros J Shen, Jiaqi Huang, Allan L Lam, John T Wixted

A photo lineup, which is a cross between an old/new and a forced-choice recognition memory test, consists of one suspect, whose face was either seen before or not, and several physically similar fillers. First, the participant/witness must decide whether the person who was previously seen is present (old/new) and then, if present, choose the previously seen target (forced choice). Competing signal-detection models of eyewitness identification performance make different predictions about how certain variables will affect a witness's ability to discriminate previously seen (guilty) suspects from new (innocent) suspects. One key variable is the similarity of the fillers to the suspect in the lineup, and another key variable is the size of the lineup (i.e., the number of fillers). Previous research investigating the role of filler similarity has supported one model, known as the Ensemble model, whereas previous research investigating the role of lineup size has supported a competing model, known as the Independent Observations model. We simultaneously manipulated these two variables (filler similarity and lineup size) and found a pattern that is not predicted by either model. When the fillers were highly similar to the suspect, increasing lineup size reduced discriminability, but when the fillers were dissimilar to the suspect, increasing lineup size enhanced discriminability. The results suggest that each additional filler adds noise to the decision-making process and that this noise factor is minimized by maximizing filler dissimilarity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

列队照相是一种介于 "新旧 "和 "强迫选择 "之间的辨认记忆测试,由一名嫌疑人(其面孔要么以前见过,要么没见过)和几名体貌相似的填充物组成。首先,参与者/证人必须判断之前见过的人是否在场(旧/新),如果在场,则选择之前见过的目标(强迫选择)。对于某些变量会如何影响证人辨别以前见过的(有罪的)嫌疑人和新的(无罪的)嫌疑人的能力,目击者辨认能力的信号检测模型之间存在竞争,并做出了不同的预测。其中一个关键变量是列队中填充物与嫌疑人的相似度,另一个关键变量是列队的大小(即填充物的数量)。以往调查填充物相似性作用的研究支持一个模型,即 "集合模型",而以往调查列队人数作用的研究支持一个竞争模型,即 "独立观察模型"。我们同时操纵了这两个变量(填充物相似度和阵容大小),结果发现了一个两个模型都无法预测的模式。当填充物与嫌疑人高度相似时,增加列队人数会降低辨别能力,但当填充物与嫌疑人不相似时,增加列队人数会提高辨别能力。结果表明,每增加一个填充者都会给决策过程增加噪音,而通过最大限度地提高填充者的不相似性,可以最大限度地降低噪音因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effects of filler similarity and lineup size on eyewitness identification.","authors":"Kyros J Shen, Jiaqi Huang, Allan L Lam, John T Wixted","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001342","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001342","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A photo lineup, which is a cross between an old/new and a forced-choice recognition memory test, consists of one suspect, whose face was either seen before or not, and several physically similar fillers. First, the participant/witness must decide whether the person who was previously seen is present (old/new) and then, if present, choose the previously seen target (forced choice). Competing signal-detection models of eyewitness identification performance make different predictions about how certain variables will affect a witness's ability to discriminate previously seen (guilty) suspects from new (innocent) suspects. One key variable is the similarity of the fillers to the suspect in the lineup, and another key variable is the size of the lineup (i.e., the number of fillers). Previous research investigating the role of filler similarity has supported one model, known as the Ensemble model, whereas previous research investigating the role of lineup size has supported a competing model, known as the Independent Observations model. We simultaneously manipulated these two variables (filler similarity and lineup size) and found a pattern that is not predicted by either model. When the fillers were highly similar to the suspect, increasing lineup size reduced discriminability, but when the fillers were dissimilar to the suspect, increasing lineup size enhanced discriminability. The results suggest that each additional filler adds noise to the decision-making process and that this noise factor is minimized by maximizing filler dissimilarity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Extending a rational process model of causal reasoning: Assessing Markov violations and explaining away with inhibitory causal relations. 扩展因果推理的理性过程模型:评估马尔可夫违规行为并用抑制性因果关系进行解释。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001395
Bob Rehder

Causes sometimes decrease rather increase the probability of an effect, as when drinking coffee lowers the probability of sleep or an aspirin eliminates a headache. This research tests how two causal reasoning errors that have influenced the development of theories of human causal reasoning manifest themselves in the presence of inhibitory causal relations. Past research with generative causal relations (a cause makes its effect more probable) has shown that people violate the Markov condition, the pattern of independence that should obtain among causally related variables. And it has shown that they explain away-the phenomenon in which one should lower likelihood of one event when another is discovered to have occurred (e.g., exonerating one murder suspect when evidence against another is found)-too little or not at all. The new empirical findings reported here reveal that both sorts of errors manifest themselves when inhibitory causal relations are present although, unexpectedly, the direction of those errors sometimes reverses. Only the mutation sampler, a rational process model of human causal reasoning, correctly predicted these novel empirical findings. These results support the view that causal reasoning errors can be understood as arising from rational inference constrained by limited cognitive resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

原因有时会降低而不是增加结果的概率,例如喝咖啡会降低睡眠的概率,或阿司匹林会消除头痛。本研究测试了影响人类因果推理理论发展的两个因果推理错误在存在抑制性因果关系时的表现形式。过去对生成性因果关系(原因使其结果更有可能发生)的研究表明,人们违反了马尔可夫条件,即因果相关变量之间应具有的独立性模式。研究还表明,人们对这一现象的解释太少或根本没有解释--当发现另一事件发生时,人们本应降低某一事件发生的可能性(例如,当发现不利于另一谋杀嫌疑人的证据时,却为其开脱罪责)。本文报告的新的实证研究结果表明,当存在抑制性因果关系时,这两种错误都会表现出来,但出乎意料的是,这些错误的方向有时会相反。只有突变采样器--人类因果推理的理性过程模型--能正确预测这些新的经验发现。这些结果支持这样一种观点,即因果推理错误可以被理解为受限于有限认知资源的理性推理所致。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Extending a rational process model of causal reasoning: Assessing Markov violations and explaining away with inhibitory causal relations.","authors":"Bob Rehder","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001395","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001395","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Causes sometimes decrease rather increase the probability of an effect, as when drinking coffee lowers the probability of sleep or an aspirin eliminates a headache. This research tests how two causal reasoning errors that have influenced the development of theories of human causal reasoning manifest themselves in the presence of inhibitory causal relations. Past research with generative causal relations (a cause makes its effect more probable) has shown that people violate the Markov condition, the pattern of independence that should obtain among causally related variables. And it has shown that they explain away-the phenomenon in which one should lower likelihood of one event when another is discovered to have occurred (e.g., exonerating one murder suspect when evidence against another is found)-too little or not at all. The new empirical findings reported here reveal that both sorts of errors manifest themselves when inhibitory causal relations are present although, unexpectedly, the direction of those errors sometimes reverses. Only the mutation sampler, a rational process model of human causal reasoning, correctly predicted these novel empirical findings. These results support the view that causal reasoning errors can be understood as arising from rational inference constrained by limited cognitive resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using diffusion models for symbolic numeracy tasks to examine aging effects. 利用符号运算任务的扩散模型研究老化效应。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001400
Roger Ratcliff, Gail McKoon

We present a model-based analysis of aging effects in three symbolic numeracy tasks using three groups of subjects (young adults, 60- to 69-year-olds, and 70- to 90-year-olds). The tasks are number discrimination (is this number greater or less than 50), number memory (was this number in the list of numbers just displayed), and number line (point to where this number is on this number line). The first two tasks were fit by the standard two-choice diffusion model and the last one by the spatially continuous diffusion model (Ratcliff, 2018). Results showed good fits of the models to accuracy (choices) and response time distributions. In the tasks, nondecision time (the time to encode a stimulus and make a response) increased with age, but the amount of evidence needed for a decision (boundary settings) increased in the number discrimination and number memory tasks, but not the number line task. The number discrimination task produced conflicting accuracy and response time results as a function of age, but the model-based analyses resolved these differences. In the number memory task, drift rates (evidence used to drive the decision process) were lower for the older adults than for young adults, but for the other two (easier) tasks, there was no change in drift rate with age. The analyses extracted differences among individuals in model components, some of which were systematic across tasks. In particular, drift rates were correlated across tasks, which shows consistent individual differences across tasks, results that could not have been obtained without model-based analyses. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们利用三组受试者(年轻成年人、60 至 69 岁的老年人和 70 至 90 岁的老年人)对三种符号运算任务中的衰老效应进行了基于模型的分析。这些任务分别是数字辨别(这个数字大于或小于 50)、数字记忆(这个数字是否在刚才显示的数字列表中)和数列(指出这个数字在这条数列上的位置)。前两项任务采用标准双选扩散模型,最后一项任务采用空间连续扩散模型(Ratcliff,2018 年)。结果显示,模型与准确率(选择)和反应时间分布的拟合效果良好。在这些任务中,非决策时间(对刺激进行编码并做出反应的时间)随着年龄的增长而增加,但在数字辨别和数字记忆任务中,做出决策所需的证据量(边界设置)会增加,而在数字线任务中则不会。在数字辨别任务中,准确率和反应时间与年龄的函数关系产生了矛盾,但基于模型的分析解决了这些差异。在数字记忆任务中,老年人的漂移率(用于驱动决策过程的证据)低于年轻人,但在其他两项(较简单的)任务中,漂移率没有随年龄变化而变化。分析提取了不同个体在模型组成方面的差异,其中一些差异在不同任务中是系统性的。特别是,漂移率在不同任务间存在相关性,这表明不同任务间的个体差异是一致的,如果没有基于模型的分析,是无法得到这样的结果的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Using diffusion models for symbolic numeracy tasks to examine aging effects.","authors":"Roger Ratcliff, Gail McKoon","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001400","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001400","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present a model-based analysis of aging effects in three symbolic numeracy tasks using three groups of subjects (young adults, 60- to 69-year-olds, and 70- to 90-year-olds). The tasks are number discrimination (is this number greater or less than 50), number memory (was this number in the list of numbers just displayed), and number line (point to where this number is on this number line). The first two tasks were fit by the standard two-choice diffusion model and the last one by the spatially continuous diffusion model (Ratcliff, 2018). Results showed good fits of the models to accuracy (choices) and response time distributions. In the tasks, nondecision time (the time to encode a stimulus and make a response) increased with age, but the amount of evidence needed for a decision (boundary settings) increased in the number discrimination and number memory tasks, but not the number line task. The number discrimination task produced conflicting accuracy and response time results as a function of age, but the model-based analyses resolved these differences. In the number memory task, drift rates (evidence used to drive the decision process) were lower for the older adults than for young adults, but for the other two (easier) tasks, there was no change in drift rate with age. The analyses extracted differences among individuals in model components, some of which were systematic across tasks. In particular, drift rates were correlated across tasks, which shows consistent individual differences across tasks, results that could not have been obtained without model-based analyses. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reassessing the role of language dominance in n-2 language repetition costs as a marker of inhibition in multilingual language switching. 重新评估语言优势在 n-2 语言重复成本中的作用,将其作为多语言转换中的抑制标记。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001333
Iring Koch, Mathieu Declerck, Greta Petersen, Daniel Rister, Wolfgang Scharke, Andrea M Philipp

Speaking two or more languages shows bilingual flexibility, but flexible switching requires language control and often incurs performance costs. We examined inhibitory control assessing n-2 repetition costs when switching three languages (L1 [German], L2 [English], L3 [French]). These costs denote worse performance in n-2 repetitions (e.g., L2-L3-L2) than in n-2 nonrepetitions (e.g., L1-L3-L2), indicating persisting inhibition. In two experiments (n = 28 in Experiment 1; n = 44 in Experiment 2), n-2 repetition costs were observed, but only for L2. Looking into L2 trials specifically, we found n-2 repetition costs when switching back to L2 from the still weaker L3 but not when returning from the stronger L1, suggesting that L2 is a strong competitor for L3 (requiring L2 inhibition) but less so for L1. Finding n-2 repetition costs supports an inhibitory account of language control in general, but our study shows only partial evidence for the theoretically assumed more specific relation between language dominance and language inhibition (i.e., only for dominance relations with respect to L1 and L3 when switching back to L2). Taken together, the findings thus suggest the need for further refinement of the concept of language dominance and its relation to inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

会说两种或两种以上语言显示了双语的灵活性,但灵活切换需要语言控制,而且往往会产生表现成本。我们研究了抑制控制,评估了转换三种语言(L1 [德语]、L2 [英语]、L3 [法语])时的 n-2 重复成本。这些代价表明,n-2 次重复(如 L2-L3-L2)的成绩比 n-2 次不重复(如 L1-L3-L2)的成绩差,这表明抑制作用持续存在。在两个实验中(实验 1,n = 28;实验 2,n = 44),我们观察到了 n-2 重复成本,但仅针对 L2。具体到 L2 试验,我们发现从仍然较弱的 L3 切换回 L2 时有 n-2 的重复成本,而从较强的 L1 切换回 L2 时则没有,这表明 L2 是 L3 的强竞争者(需要 L2 抑制),而 L1 的竞争者则较少。n-2重复成本的发现支持了语言控制的一般抑制性解释,但我们的研究只部分证明了理论上假设的语言优势和语言抑制之间更具体的关系(即只证明了切换回L2时L1和L3的优势关系)。综上所述,研究结果表明,有必要进一步完善语言优势的概念及其与语言抑制的关系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reassessing the role of language dominance in n-2 language repetition costs as a marker of inhibition in multilingual language switching.","authors":"Iring Koch, Mathieu Declerck, Greta Petersen, Daniel Rister, Wolfgang Scharke, Andrea M Philipp","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001333","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001333","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Speaking two or more languages shows bilingual flexibility, but flexible switching requires language control and often incurs performance costs. We examined inhibitory control assessing <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs when switching three languages (L1 [German], L2 [English], L3 [French]). These costs denote worse performance in <i>n</i>-2 repetitions (e.g., L2-L3-L2) than in <i>n</i>-2 nonrepetitions (e.g., L1-L3-L2), indicating persisting inhibition. In two experiments (<i>n</i> = 28 in Experiment 1; <i>n</i> = 44 in Experiment 2), <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs were observed, but only for L2. Looking into L2 trials specifically, we found <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs when switching back to L2 from the still weaker L3 but not when returning from the stronger L1, suggesting that L2 is a strong competitor for L3 (requiring L2 inhibition) but less so for L1. Finding <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs supports an inhibitory account of language control in general, but our study shows only partial evidence for the theoretically assumed more specific relation between language dominance and language inhibition (i.e., only for dominance relations with respect to L1 and L3 when switching back to L2). Taken together, the findings thus suggest the need for further refinement of the concept of language dominance and its relation to inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Penn Electrophysiology of Encoding and Retrieval Study. 宾夕法尼亚编码和检索电生理学研究
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001319
Michael J Kahana, Lynn J Lohnas, M Karl Healey, Ada Aka, Adam W Broitman, Patrick Crutchley, Elizabeth Crutchley, Kylie H Alm, Brandon S Katerman, Nicole E Miller, Joel R Kuhn, Yuxuan Li, Nicole M Long, Jonathan Miller, Madison D Paron, Jesse K Pazdera, Isaac Pedisich, Joseph H Rudoler, Christoph T Weidemann

The Penn Electrophysiology of Encoding and Retrieval Study (PEERS) aimed to characterize the behavioral and electrophysiological (EEG) correlates of memory encoding and retrieval in highly practiced individuals. Across five PEERS experiments, 300+ subjects contributed more than 7,000 memory testing sessions with recorded EEG data. Here we tell the story of PEERS: its genesis, evolution, major findings, and the lessons it taught us about taking a big scientific approach in studying memory and the human brain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

宾夕法尼亚大学编码和检索电生理学研究(PEERS)旨在描述高度练习者记忆编码和检索的行为和电生理学(EEG)相关性。在五项 PEERS 实验中,300 多名受试者进行了 7,000 多次记忆测试,并记录了脑电图数据。在这里,我们将讲述 PEERS 的故事:它的起源、演变、主要发现,以及它在采用大科学方法研究记忆和人脑方面给我们的启示。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The Penn Electrophysiology of Encoding and Retrieval Study.","authors":"Michael J Kahana, Lynn J Lohnas, M Karl Healey, Ada Aka, Adam W Broitman, Patrick Crutchley, Elizabeth Crutchley, Kylie H Alm, Brandon S Katerman, Nicole E Miller, Joel R Kuhn, Yuxuan Li, Nicole M Long, Jonathan Miller, Madison D Paron, Jesse K Pazdera, Isaac Pedisich, Joseph H Rudoler, Christoph T Weidemann","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001319","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001319","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Penn Electrophysiology of Encoding and Retrieval Study (PEERS) aimed to characterize the behavioral and electrophysiological (EEG) correlates of memory encoding and retrieval in highly practiced individuals. Across five PEERS experiments, 300+ subjects contributed more than 7,000 memory testing sessions with recorded EEG data. Here we tell the story of PEERS: its genesis, evolution, major findings, and the lessons it taught us about taking a big scientific approach in studying memory and the human brain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural correlates of memory in a naturalistic spatiotemporal context. 自然时空背景下记忆的神经相关性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001341
Matthew R Dougherty, Woohyeuk Chang, Joseph H Rudoler, Brandon S Katerman, David J Halpern, James P Bruska, Nicholas B Diamond, Michael J Kahana

We investigated memory encoding and retrieval during a quasinaturalistic spatial-episodic memory task in which subjects delivered items to landmarks in a desktop virtual environment and later recalled the delivered items. Transition probabilities and latencies revealed the spatial and temporal organization of memory. As subjects gained experience with the town, their improved spatial knowledge led to more efficient navigation and increased spatial organization during recall. Subjects who exhibited stronger spatial organization exhibited weaker temporal organization. Scalp-recorded electroencephalographic signals revealed spectral correlates of successful encoding and retrieval. Increased theta power (T+) and decreased alpha/beta power (A-) accompanied successful encoding, with the addition of increased gamma (G+) accompanying successful retrieval. Logistic regression classifiers trained on spectral features reliably predicted mnemonic success in held-out sessions. Univariate and multivariate electroencephalographic analyses revealed a similar spectral T+A-G+ of successful memory. These findings extend behavioral and neural signatures of successful encoding and retrieval to a naturalistic task in which learning occurs within a spatiotemporal context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了受试者在准自然主义空间外显记忆任务中进行记忆编码和检索的过程,在该任务中,受试者将物品送到桌面虚拟环境中的地标,随后再回忆已送达的物品。转换概率和延迟揭示了记忆的空间和时间组织。随着受试者在城镇中积累经验,他们的空间知识得到了提高,从而提高了导航效率,并在回忆过程中增强了空间组织能力。空间组织能力较强的受试者的时间组织能力较弱。头皮记录的脑电信号显示了成功编码和检索的频谱相关性。θ功率(T+)增加和α/β功率(A-)降低伴随着编码的成功,另外伽马(G+)增加也伴随着检索的成功。根据频谱特征训练的逻辑回归分类器能可靠地预测保持记忆过程中的记忆成功率。单变量和多变量脑电图分析显示,成功记忆的频谱T+A-G+相似。这些发现将成功编码和检索的行为和神经特征扩展到了在时空背景下进行学习的自然任务中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Neural correlates of memory in a naturalistic spatiotemporal context.","authors":"Matthew R Dougherty, Woohyeuk Chang, Joseph H Rudoler, Brandon S Katerman, David J Halpern, James P Bruska, Nicholas B Diamond, Michael J Kahana","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001341","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001341","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated memory encoding and retrieval during a quasinaturalistic spatial-episodic memory task in which subjects delivered items to landmarks in a desktop virtual environment and later recalled the delivered items. Transition probabilities and latencies revealed the spatial and temporal organization of memory. As subjects gained experience with the town, their improved spatial knowledge led to more efficient navigation and increased spatial organization during recall. Subjects who exhibited stronger spatial organization exhibited weaker temporal organization. Scalp-recorded electroencephalographic signals revealed spectral correlates of successful encoding and retrieval. Increased theta power (T+) and decreased alpha/beta power (A-) accompanied successful encoding, with the addition of increased gamma (G+) accompanying successful retrieval. Logistic regression classifiers trained on spectral features reliably predicted mnemonic success in held-out sessions. Univariate and multivariate electroencephalographic analyses revealed a similar spectral T+A-G+ of successful memory. These findings extend behavioral and neural signatures of successful encoding and retrieval to a naturalistic task in which learning occurs within a spatiotemporal context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479512","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning traps and change blindness in dynamic environments. 动态环境中的学习陷阱和变化盲点。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001390
Won Jae Lee, Amy X Li, Jaimie E Lee, Brett K Hayes

Learning traps arise when early experience leads to a false belief about the reward structure of the environment which, in turn, leads to avoidance of rewarding options. Previous work on the negative effects of such traps has focused on static learning environments. The current work examines an additional negative effect of learning traps in dynamic environments-blindness to change in the features that predict decision outcomes. In two experiments (N = 416), participants had to decide whether to approach members of two different categories, respectively associated with either gains or losses. Early in learning, a category rule involving two feature dimensions predicted category membership. Subsequently, there was a change in the feature composition of this rule. When outcome feedback was only provided when an item was approached, a substantial proportion of participants fell into the trap of using a simple one-dimensional rule to guide approach decisions. Most of these participants did not notice the subsequent rule change and never learned the new rule. Signaling the possibility of rule change (Experiment 2) had no effect on change blindness for those in the learning trap but did improve learning of the new rule for those who initially avoided the trap. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当早期经验导致人们对环境中的奖励结构产生错误认识,进而回避有奖励的选择时,就会出现学习陷阱。以前关于这种陷阱的负面影响的研究主要集中在静态学习环境上。目前的研究则是探讨动态环境中学习陷阱的另一种负面影响--对预测决策结果的特征变化的盲目性。在两个实验中(N = 416),参与者必须决定是否接近两个不同类别的成员,这两个类别分别与收益或损失相关。在学习初期,涉及两个特征维度的类别规则可以预测类别成员。随后,这一规则的特征构成发生了变化。当只有在接近一个项目时才提供结果反馈时,相当一部分参与者会陷入使用简单的一维规则来指导接近决策的陷阱。这些参与者中的大多数并没有注意到随后的规则变化,也从未学习过新规则。提示规则改变的可能性(实验 2)对那些陷入学习陷阱的人来说没有影响,但对那些最初避开了陷阱的人来说,却提高了他们对新规则的学习能力。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Learning traps and change blindness in dynamic environments.","authors":"Won Jae Lee, Amy X Li, Jaimie E Lee, Brett K Hayes","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001390","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning traps arise when early experience leads to a false belief about the reward structure of the environment which, in turn, leads to avoidance of rewarding options. Previous work on the negative effects of such traps has focused on static learning environments. The current work examines an additional negative effect of learning traps in dynamic environments-blindness to change in the features that predict decision outcomes. In two experiments (N = 416), participants had to decide whether to approach members of two different categories, respectively associated with either gains or losses. Early in learning, a category rule involving two feature dimensions predicted category membership. Subsequently, there was a change in the feature composition of this rule. When outcome feedback was only provided when an item was approached, a substantial proportion of participants fell into the trap of using a simple one-dimensional rule to guide approach decisions. Most of these participants did not notice the subsequent rule change and never learned the new rule. Signaling the possibility of rule change (Experiment 2) had no effect on change blindness for those in the learning trap but did improve learning of the new rule for those who initially avoided the trap. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neighborhood in Chinese lexicon: A megastudy analysis of lexical decision and naming of two-character Chinese words. 汉语词汇中的邻里关系:对双字汉语词的词汇决定和命名的大型研究分析。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001357
Chi-Shing Tse, Melvin J Yap, Yuen-Lai Chan

The present study examines the impact of neighborhood size (number of other two-character words sharing the same character at the same position) on Chinese lexical processing, along with its joint effects with variables such as character frequency, word frequency, and semantic transparency. Previous factorial experiments have yielded conflicting results that are difficult to reconcile with existing models (Li et al., 2015, 2017). To provide high-powered tests for these theoretically important effects on visual word recognition, we leveraged the megastudy approach and used linear mixed-effect analyses to investigate lexical decision and naming responses to a large pool of two-character Chinese words (N > 17,000) sourced from Tse et al.'s (2017, 2023) database. In all analyses we controlled for extraneous orthographic (e.g., stroke count), phonological (e.g., consistency), and semantic (e.g., transparency) variables. In addition to evaluating Li et al.'s (2015, 2017) models, we also investigated whether the parallel dual-route mechanism, which entails lexical access via whole-word or character decomposition-then-composition, could account for neighborhood size effect and its interactions in lexical decision and naming. Finally, we discuss the implications of our findings on the specificity of lexical effects with regard to character position and lexical processing task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究考察了邻域大小(在同一位置共享相同字符的其他双字符词的数量)对汉语词汇加工的影响,以及它与字符频率、词频和语义透明度等变量的联合效应。以往的因子实验得出的结果相互矛盾,难以与现有模型相协调(Li 等人,2015 年,2017 年)。为了对这些理论上对视觉词语识别的重要影响进行高能测试,我们采用了巨型研究方法,并使用线性混合效应分析来研究来自 Tse 等人(2017 年,2023 年)数据库的大量双字汉语词语(N > 17,000 个)的词汇决策和命名反应。在所有分析中,我们都控制了无关的正字法(如笔画数)、语音学(如一致性)和语义学(如透明度)变量。除了评估 Li 等人(2015、2017 年)的模型外,我们还研究了并行双路径机制(即通过全词或字符分解--然后再组合的方式访问词汇)能否解释邻域大小效应及其在词汇决策和命名中的交互作用。最后,我们讨论了我们的研究结果对词汇效应在字符位置和词汇处理任务方面的特异性的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Neighborhood in Chinese lexicon: A megastudy analysis of lexical decision and naming of two-character Chinese words.","authors":"Chi-Shing Tse, Melvin J Yap, Yuen-Lai Chan","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001357","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001357","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examines the impact of neighborhood size (number of other two-character words sharing the same character at the same position) on Chinese lexical processing, along with its joint effects with variables such as character frequency, word frequency, and semantic transparency. Previous factorial experiments have yielded conflicting results that are difficult to reconcile with existing models (Li et al., 2015, 2017). To provide high-powered tests for these theoretically important effects on visual word recognition, we leveraged the megastudy approach and used linear mixed-effect analyses to investigate lexical decision and naming responses to a large pool of two-character Chinese words (<i>N</i> > 17,000) sourced from Tse et al.'s (2017, 2023) database. In all analyses we controlled for extraneous orthographic (e.g., stroke count), phonological (e.g., consistency), and semantic (e.g., transparency) variables. In addition to evaluating Li et al.'s (2015, 2017) models, we also investigated whether the parallel dual-route mechanism, which entails lexical access via whole-word or character decomposition-then-composition, could account for neighborhood size effect and its interactions in lexical decision and naming. Finally, we discuss the implications of our findings on the specificity of lexical effects with regard to character position and lexical processing task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762244","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1