首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Separate storage mechanisms of absolute depth and relative depth in working memory: Encoding, maintenance, consolidation, and attention engagements.
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-13 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001444
Kaiyue Wang, Wenting Lin, Xiaoyu Wang, Jiehui Qian

Most research on visual working memory (WM) focuses on 2D objects and spatial locations; however, the storage mechanism of depth information-another important dimension in a 3D environment-remains largely unknown. The present study conducted seven experiments to systematically investigate how absolute depth (metric distance) and relative depth (ordinal relations among depth planes) are encoded, maintained, and consolidated in WM. In Experiments 1a and 1b, we found that memory accuracy for relative depth was higher than that for absolute depth, and verbal WM seemed to be automatically involved in encoding relative depth in the form of numerals even though memory stimuli were presented visually, and verbal suppression was applied. Experiments 2a and 2b showed memory for fine absolute depth information gradually declined over time, while memory for coarse depth information and relative depth showed no temporal decay. By manipulating the stimuli-mask onset latency, Experiment 3a revealed that memory for absolute depth remained to be fragile across retention, while Experiment 3b showed that relative depth information could be consolidated into robust memory. By inserting an additional visual search task into the memory task, Experiment 4 showed that attention was required for the maintenance of absolute depth information, while relative depth memory involved minimal attention engagement. These findings indicate separate storage mechanisms for the two types of depth information: Absolute depth, especially fine information, is poorly maintained in a fragile visual store, whereas relative depth, which is encoded coarsely, involves both a robust verbal store and a visual store. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Separate storage mechanisms of absolute depth and relative depth in working memory: Encoding, maintenance, consolidation, and attention engagements.","authors":"Kaiyue Wang, Wenting Lin, Xiaoyu Wang, Jiehui Qian","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001444","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most research on visual working memory (WM) focuses on 2D objects and spatial locations; however, the storage mechanism of depth information-another important dimension in a 3D environment-remains largely unknown. The present study conducted seven experiments to systematically investigate how absolute depth (metric distance) and relative depth (ordinal relations among depth planes) are encoded, maintained, and consolidated in WM. In Experiments 1a and 1b, we found that memory accuracy for relative depth was higher than that for absolute depth, and verbal WM seemed to be automatically involved in encoding relative depth in the form of numerals even though memory stimuli were presented visually, and verbal suppression was applied. Experiments 2a and 2b showed memory for fine absolute depth information gradually declined over time, while memory for coarse depth information and relative depth showed no temporal decay. By manipulating the stimuli-mask onset latency, Experiment 3a revealed that memory for absolute depth remained to be fragile across retention, while Experiment 3b showed that relative depth information could be consolidated into robust memory. By inserting an additional visual search task into the memory task, Experiment 4 showed that attention was required for the maintenance of absolute depth information, while relative depth memory involved minimal attention engagement. These findings indicate separate storage mechanisms for the two types of depth information: Absolute depth, especially fine information, is poorly maintained in a fragile visual store, whereas relative depth, which is encoded coarsely, involves both a robust verbal store and a visual store. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143416017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memorability effects emerge in incidental visual working memory.
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-10 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001447
Joyce Tam, Taryn Green, Ryan E O'Donnell, Brad Wyble

Memorability denotes a stimulus-intrinsic property that results in stimuli being more likely to be remembered or forgotten. The effect is generally consistent across observers and can be measured in various stimuli such as faces and scene images. Long-term memory paradigms have been used to measure memorability with studies demonstrating long-term memorability effects via incidental and intentional encoding mechanisms. The present study examines whether memorability modulates the ability to unexpectedly report an image at short intervals. Three sets of experiments (using faces and scenes) with replications used an attribute amnesia paradigm to measure the effect of memorability on incidental visual working memory processes while controlling attentional priorities. When participants had to identify the target image in the immediate trial unexpectedly, we observed that memorable targets yielded a higher accuracy compared to forgettable targets, showing that people incidentally remember more information about memorable stimuli even across very short intervals. This memorability benefit was observed for both stimulus types but was greater for faces compared to scenes. Moreover, follow-up experiments suggested this memorability effect was not due to enhanced attentional capture by the high-memorability item or differences in target-foil contrast. Thus, memorability modulates incidental memory even without the need to retrieve from long-term memory. These results expand our understanding of the mechanisms behind memorability effects and how attentional filters impact memory traces. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Memorability effects emerge in incidental visual working memory.","authors":"Joyce Tam, Taryn Green, Ryan E O'Donnell, Brad Wyble","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001447","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001447","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Memorability denotes a stimulus-intrinsic property that results in stimuli being more likely to be remembered or forgotten. The effect is generally consistent across observers and can be measured in various stimuli such as faces and scene images. Long-term memory paradigms have been used to measure memorability with studies demonstrating long-term memorability effects via incidental and intentional encoding mechanisms. The present study examines whether memorability modulates the ability to unexpectedly report an image at short intervals. Three sets of experiments (using faces and scenes) with replications used an attribute amnesia paradigm to measure the effect of memorability on incidental visual working memory processes while controlling attentional priorities. When participants had to identify the target image in the immediate trial unexpectedly, we observed that memorable targets yielded a higher accuracy compared to forgettable targets, showing that people incidentally remember more information about memorable stimuli even across very short intervals. This memorability benefit was observed for both stimulus types but was greater for faces compared to scenes. Moreover, follow-up experiments suggested this memorability effect was not due to enhanced attentional capture by the high-memorability item or differences in target-foil contrast. Thus, memorability modulates incidental memory even without the need to retrieve from long-term memory. These results expand our understanding of the mechanisms behind memorability effects and how attentional filters impact memory traces. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143391865","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Identifying text-based factors that contribute to the superior reading efficiency of skilled deaf readers: An eye-tracking study of length, frequency, and predictability.
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-10 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001415
Frances G Cooley, Karen Emmorey, Emily Saunders, Grace Sinclair, Casey Stringer, Elizabeth R Schotter

Skilled deaf readers are more efficient than their hearing counterparts-they read faster, skipping more words without a negative impact on comprehension. It is not clear from where deaf readers' efficiency derives, because reading is a complex cognitive process that requires readers to extract meaning from text, incorporating visual, lexical, and contextual information. To assess the contributions of these factors to deaf readers' efficiency, we tracked their eye movements as they read sentences with target words that were manipulated for length, frequency, and predictability, and we assessed the effects of those variables on skipping probability (i.e., whether the reader skipped or fixated the target) and gaze duration (i.e., the amount of time spent fixating the word before leaving it) and compared these patterns to hearing readers with equivalent reading comprehension skill. Deaf readers demonstrated increased skipping rates and shorter gaze durations overall compared to hearing readers and exhibited different patterns of word length and predictability effects, but similar frequency effects. Deaf readers' eye movements reflect visual linguistic processing expertise as they are primarily driven by word length when targeting words for fixation, but frequency and predictability effects on word skipping indicate that they do engage in parafoveal linguistic processing. These results emphasize the qualitative differences in reading strategies between deaf and hearing readers and advance our understanding of the impact of early language and sensory experiences on reading behaviors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

熟练的聋人读者比他们的健听读者更有效率--他们的阅读速度更快,跳读的字数更多,但对理解能力没有负面影响。目前还不清楚聋人读者的效率来源于何处,因为阅读是一个复杂的认知过程,需要读者从文本中提取意义,并结合视觉、词汇和上下文信息。为了评估这些因素对聋人读者阅读效率的影响,我们跟踪了他们在阅读带有目标词的句子时的眼球运动,这些目标词的长度、频率和可预测性都经过了处理,我们还评估了这些变量对跳读概率(即读者是跳过目标词还是固定目标词)和注视持续时间(即在离开目标词之前固定目标词所花费的时间)的影响,并将这些模式与具有同等阅读理解能力的听力读者进行了比较。与听力读者相比,聋人读者的跳读率更高,注视时间更短,表现出不同的单词长度和可预测性效应模式,但频率效应相似。聋人读者的眼球运动反映了他们的视觉语言处理能力,因为他们在瞄准单词进行固定时主要受单词长度的驱动,但单词跳读的频率和可预测性效应表明他们确实参与了视网膜旁的语言处理。这些结果强调了聋人读者和听人读者在阅读策略上的本质区别,并加深了我们对早期语言和感官经验对阅读行为影响的理解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Identifying text-based factors that contribute to the superior reading efficiency of skilled deaf readers: An eye-tracking study of length, frequency, and predictability.","authors":"Frances G Cooley, Karen Emmorey, Emily Saunders, Grace Sinclair, Casey Stringer, Elizabeth R Schotter","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001415","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001415","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Skilled deaf readers are more efficient than their hearing counterparts-they read faster, skipping more words without a negative impact on comprehension. It is not clear from where deaf readers' efficiency derives, because reading is a complex cognitive process that requires readers to extract meaning from text, incorporating visual, lexical, and contextual information. To assess the contributions of these factors to deaf readers' efficiency, we tracked their eye movements as they read sentences with target words that were manipulated for <i>length</i>, <i>frequency</i>, and <i>predictability</i>, and we assessed the effects of those variables on <i>skipping probability</i> (i.e., whether the reader skipped or fixated the target) and <i>gaze duration</i> (i.e., the amount of time spent fixating the word before leaving it) and compared these patterns to hearing readers with equivalent reading comprehension skill. Deaf readers demonstrated increased skipping rates and shorter gaze durations overall compared to hearing readers and exhibited different patterns of word length and predictability effects, but similar frequency effects. Deaf readers' eye movements reflect <i>visual linguistic processing expertise</i> as they are primarily driven by word length when targeting words for fixation, but frequency and predictability effects on word skipping indicate that they do engage in parafoveal linguistic processing. These results emphasize the qualitative differences in reading strategies between deaf and hearing readers and advance our understanding of the impact of early language and sensory experiences on reading behaviors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143391815","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of working memory capacity in the temporal compression of episodic memories: An individual differences approach. 工作记忆容量在外显记忆的时间压缩中的作用:个体差异法
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001350
Nathan Leroy, Steve Majerus, Arnaud D'Argembeau

Remembering past events usually takes less time than their actual duration-their unfolding is temporally compressed in episodic memory. The rate of temporal compression (i.e., the ratio of the actual duration of an event to the duration of its remembering) is not constant but varies between individuals and as a function of the structure of events (e.g., how they can be divided into shorter subevents). However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying these variations remain poorly understood. Given its role in the encoding and retrieval of information in episodic memory, working memory (WM) capacity could be an important determinant of temporal compression rates. We tested this hypothesis in two experiments in which we asked participants to watch and then mentally replay short videos showing people engaged in daily life activities. We showed that temporal compression rates depend on an interplay between WM and the structure of the remembered events: participants' WM capacity (assessed using complex span tasks) was negatively associated with temporal compression rates, but only when the remembered events contained few event boundaries (i.e., few subevents). This suggests that the temporal compression of events in episodic memory emerges when some of the subevents to be retained are too long to be fully represented in WM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆过去事件所需的时间通常少于事件的实际持续时间--事件的展开在历时记忆中被时间压缩了。时间压缩率(即事件的实际持续时间与记忆持续时间之比)并非恒定不变,而是因人而异,并与事件的结构(如如何将事件划分为更短的子事件)有关。然而,人们对这些变化背后的认知机制仍然知之甚少。鉴于工作记忆(WM)能力在外显记忆信息的编码和检索中的作用,它可能是时间压缩率的一个重要决定因素。我们在两个实验中测试了这一假设。在这两个实验中,我们要求参与者观看并在头脑中回放展示人们从事日常生活活动的短片。实验结果表明,时间压缩率取决于 WM 与记忆事件结构之间的相互作用:参与者的 WM 能力(使用复杂跨度任务进行评估)与时间压缩率呈负相关,但只有当记忆事件包含很少的事件边界(即很少的子事件)时才会出现这种情况。这表明,当需要保留的一些子事件太长而无法在 WM 中完全体现时,就会出现外显记忆中事件的时间压缩。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of working memory capacity in the temporal compression of episodic memories: An individual differences approach.","authors":"Nathan Leroy, Steve Majerus, Arnaud D'Argembeau","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001350","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Remembering past events usually takes less time than their actual duration-their unfolding is temporally compressed in episodic memory. The rate of temporal compression (i.e., the ratio of the actual duration of an event to the duration of its remembering) is not constant but varies between individuals and as a function of the structure of events (e.g., how they can be divided into shorter subevents). However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying these variations remain poorly understood. Given its role in the encoding and retrieval of information in episodic memory, working memory (WM) capacity could be an important determinant of temporal compression rates. We tested this hypothesis in two experiments in which we asked participants to watch and then mentally replay short videos showing people engaged in daily life activities. We showed that temporal compression rates depend on an interplay between WM and the structure of the remembered events: participants' WM capacity (assessed using complex span tasks) was negatively associated with temporal compression rates, but only when the remembered events contained few event boundaries (i.e., few subevents). This suggests that the temporal compression of events in episodic memory emerges when some of the subevents to be retained are too long to be fully represented in WM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"285-300"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two sources of color-word contingency learning: Episodic retrieval of stimulus-response bindings and propositional knowledge. 颜色-词语应急学习的两个来源:刺激-反应绑定的偶发检索和命题知识。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001353
Matthäus Rudolph, Klaus Rothermund

There is an ongoing debate about the cognitive mechanisms behind human contingency learning (CL). Although, in some studies, episodic retrieval of previous responses fully explained the observed CL effects (C. G. Giesen et al., 2020; Schmidt et al., 2020), other findings suggest that global contingencies have an additional effect on behavior (Xu & Mordkoff, 2020). In a high-powered (N = 500), preregistered study, we investigated CL effects after controlling for episodic retrieval of distractor-target (S-S) and distractor-response (S-R) bindings. Retrieval explained a large part of the CL effect. However, we still found a reliable residual CL effect even after controlling for retrieval. Notably, the residual CL effect depended on contingency awareness: The residual CL effect only occurred for trials for which participants correctly detected the respective color-word contingency, whereas for trials without contingency awareness, there was no residual CL effect. Collectively, our findings suggest that human CL is driven by two independent sources: (a) episodic retrieval of S-S and S-R bindings and (b) propositional knowledge of the contingencies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

关于人类应急学习(CL)背后的认知机制一直存在争论。尽管在一些研究中,对先前反应的外显检索完全解释了观察到的或然学习效应(C. G. Giesen 等人,2020 年;Schmidt 等人,2020 年),但其他研究结果表明,全局或然效应对行为有额外的影响(Xu & Mordkoff,2020 年)。在一项高功率(N = 500)、预先登记的研究中,我们在控制了分心者-目标(S-S)和分心者-反应(S-R)绑定的外显检索后,调查了CL效应。检索解释了 CL 效应的大部分原因。然而,即使在控制了检索之后,我们仍然发现了可靠的残余 CL 效应。值得注意的是,残余 CL 效应取决于或然意识:残余 CL 效应只出现在参与者正确检测到相应颜色-词语或然性的试验中,而在没有或然性意识的试验中,则没有残余 CL 效应。总之,我们的研究结果表明,人类的CL是由两个独立的来源驱动的:(a) S-S 和 S-R 绑定的偶发检索;(b) 关于或然性的命题知识。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Two sources of color-word contingency learning: Episodic retrieval of stimulus-response bindings and propositional knowledge.","authors":"Matthäus Rudolph, Klaus Rothermund","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001353","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001353","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is an ongoing debate about the cognitive mechanisms behind human contingency learning (CL). Although, in some studies, episodic retrieval of previous responses fully explained the observed CL effects (C. G. Giesen et al., 2020; Schmidt et al., 2020), other findings suggest that global contingencies have an additional effect on behavior (Xu & Mordkoff, 2020). In a high-powered (<i>N</i> = 500), preregistered study, we investigated CL effects after controlling for episodic retrieval of distractor-target (S-S) and distractor-response (S-R) bindings. Retrieval explained a large part of the CL effect. However, we still found a reliable residual CL effect even after controlling for retrieval. Notably, the residual CL effect depended on contingency awareness: The residual CL effect only occurred for trials for which participants correctly detected the respective color-word contingency, whereas for trials without contingency awareness, there was no residual CL effect. Collectively, our findings suggest that human CL is driven by two independent sources: (a) episodic retrieval of S-S and S-R bindings and (b) propositional knowledge of the contingencies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"209-217"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141181378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence for response inhibition as a control process distinct from the common executive function: A two-study factor analysis. 反应抑制是一种不同于普通执行功能的控制过程的证据:两项研究的因子分析
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001352
Grant S Shields, Andrew P Yonelinas

The dominant model of executive functions, which has held for over two decades, contends that various aspects of seemingly disparate forms of inhibitory control-for example, inhibiting a prepotent response, or inhibiting irrelevant thoughts and distractions-are in fact manifestations of a single latent executive function. Recent work, however, has cast doubt on this dominant model, as certain conditions can dissociate performance on tasks thought to index inhibitory control. Moreover, issues related to task reliability and latent estimation of inhibition processes have prompted questions about whether the structure of inhibitory control can even be reliably estimated at a latent level. We addressed these issues in two studies of healthy young adults (Study 1 N = 154, Study 2, N = 279), examining seven then 12 different tasks taken by prior research to assess inhibitory control. Contrary to the dominant model of executive functions, we found that, at a latent level, inhibitory control was best fit by a replicable two-factor solution, with response inhibition as a distinct executive function. Further, our data suggested that prior work on executive functions may not have observed a response inhibition factor due to task selections (i.e., including either one of two specific tasks was critical to identifying a separate response inhibition factor). Therefore, contrary to the current primary theoretical model of executive functions, these results suggest that response inhibition is, in fact, a distinct control process from the control process underpinning other forms of inhibition, which has important implications for designing interventions and assessing outcomes related to inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能的主流模式已经存在了二十多年,该模式认为,看似不同形式的抑制控制的各个方面--例如,抑制先期反应,或抑制无关的想法和分心--实际上是单一潜在执行功能的表现形式。然而,最近的研究对这一主流模型产生了怀疑,因为在某些情况下,被认为是抑制控制指标的任务表现会出现分离。此外,与任务可靠性和抑制过程的潜估计有关的问题也引发了人们的疑问:抑制控制的结构是否能在潜水平上得到可靠的估计。我们在两项针对健康年轻成年人的研究(研究1,人数=154;研究2,人数=279)中探讨了这些问题,分别考察了先前研究中用于评估抑制控制的7项和12项不同任务。与执行功能的主流模型相反,我们发现,在潜意识层面上,抑制控制最符合可复制的双因素解决方案,反应抑制是一种独特的执行功能。此外,我们的数据还表明,由于任务选择的原因,先前关于执行功能的研究可能没有观察到反应抑制因子(也就是说,包含两个特定任务中的一个对于识别单独的反应抑制因子至关重要)。因此,与目前执行功能的主要理论模型相反,这些结果表明,反应抑制实际上是一个不同于其他形式抑制的控制过程,这对设计干预措施和评估与抑制控制有关的结果具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Evidence for response inhibition as a control process distinct from the common executive function: A two-study factor analysis.","authors":"Grant S Shields, Andrew P Yonelinas","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001352","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001352","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The dominant model of executive functions, which has held for over two decades, contends that various aspects of seemingly disparate forms of inhibitory control-for example, inhibiting a prepotent response, or inhibiting irrelevant thoughts and distractions-are in fact manifestations of a single latent executive function. Recent work, however, has cast doubt on this dominant model, as certain conditions can dissociate performance on tasks thought to index inhibitory control. Moreover, issues related to task reliability and latent estimation of inhibition processes have prompted questions about whether the structure of inhibitory control can even be reliably estimated at a latent level. We addressed these issues in two studies of healthy young adults (Study 1 <i>N</i> = 154, Study 2, <i>N</i> = 279), examining seven then 12 different tasks taken by prior research to assess inhibitory control. Contrary to the dominant model of executive functions, we found that, at a latent level, inhibitory control was best fit by a replicable two-factor solution, with response inhibition as a distinct executive function. Further, our data suggested that prior work on executive functions may not have observed a response inhibition factor due to task selections (i.e., including either one of two specific tasks was critical to identifying a separate response inhibition factor). Therefore, contrary to the current primary theoretical model of executive functions, these results suggest that response inhibition is, in fact, a distinct control process from the control process underpinning other forms of inhibition, which has important implications for designing interventions and assessing outcomes related to inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"218-237"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900087","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Driving factors of individual differences in broad retrieval ability: Gr is more than the sum of its parts. 广泛检索能力个体差异的驱动因素:Gr is more than the sum of its parts.
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001336
Benjamin Goecke, Selina Weiss, Oliver Wilhelm

Broad retrieval ability (Gr) posits an essential factor of human cognitive abilities. Previous literature indicates Gr is best modeled as a higher-order factor model with lower-level factors such as ideational fluency (IF), word fluency (WF), expressional fluency (EF), or figural fluency (FF). However, the dimensionality of Gr is not well studied. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether specific retrieval affordances such as differing retrieval time periods (e.g., short vs. long) can be psychometrically separated from more general retrieval affordances. Such a distinction would imply differential associations between specific retrieval, general retrieval, and other cognitive abilities, which, in turn, depict a vital part of explanatory models of individual differences in Gr. To test these assumptions, we conducted a multivariate study (N = 331) and evaluated competing latent variable measurement models for a variety of Gr tests. We then regressed the best measurement model onto working memory capacity, secondary memory, mental speed, and crystallized intelligence in order to evaluate the distinctiveness of Gr. Our results suggest that no specific retrieval affordances with regard to time periods can be distinguished. A higher-order model, with a second-order Gr factor above three first-order factors (IF, WF, EF, and FF) fitted the data best, extending previous literature by increasing construct coverage through the implementation of FF. All covariates show incremental predictive validity, beyond their communality. Summarizing, our results endorse a perspective on Gr as a strong and discriminant factor of cognitive abilities that is not affected by time constraints, and show that Gr is more than a linear combination of its parts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

广义检索能力(Gr)是人类认知能力的一个基本要素。以往的文献表明,广义检索能力最好与表意流畅性(IF)、词语流畅性(WF)、表达流畅性(EF)或表象流畅性(FF)等低层次因素一起作为一个高阶因素模型。然而,对 Gr 的维度并没有进行深入研究。此外,特定的检索能力(如不同的检索时间段(如短检索时间段与长检索时间段))是否能从心理统计学上与更一般的检索能力区分开来,目前仍不清楚。这种区分将意味着特定检索、一般检索和其他认知能力之间存在不同的关联,而这反过来又是Gr个体差异解释模型的重要组成部分。为了验证这些假设,我们进行了一项多元研究(N = 331),并对各种Gr测试的竞争潜变量测量模型进行了评估。然后,我们将最佳测量模型与工作记忆能力、二级记忆、心智速度和智能结晶进行回归,以评估 Gr 的独特性。我们的研究结果表明,在时间段方面没有特定的检索能力可以区分。在三个一阶因子(IF、WF、EF 和 FF)之上再加上一个二阶 Gr 因子的高阶模型最符合数据,通过实施 FF 增加了构念的覆盖范围,从而扩展了以前的文献。所有协变量都显示出超越其共性的增量预测有效性。综上所述,我们的研究结果支持将 Gr 视为不受时间限制影响的认知能力的强判别因素的观点,并表明 Gr 不仅仅是各部分的线性组合。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Driving factors of individual differences in broad retrieval ability: Gr is more than the sum of its parts.","authors":"Benjamin Goecke, Selina Weiss, Oliver Wilhelm","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001336","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001336","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Broad retrieval ability (Gr) posits an essential factor of human cognitive abilities. Previous literature indicates Gr is best modeled as a higher-order factor model with lower-level factors such as ideational fluency (IF), word fluency (WF), expressional fluency (EF), or figural fluency (FF). However, the dimensionality of Gr is not well studied. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether specific retrieval affordances such as differing retrieval time periods (e.g., short vs. long) can be psychometrically separated from more general retrieval affordances. Such a distinction would imply differential associations between specific retrieval, general retrieval, and other cognitive abilities, which, in turn, depict a vital part of explanatory models of individual differences in Gr. To test these assumptions, we conducted a multivariate study (<i>N</i> = 331) and evaluated competing latent variable measurement models for a variety of Gr tests. We then regressed the best measurement model onto working memory capacity, secondary memory, mental speed, and crystallized intelligence in order to evaluate the distinctiveness of Gr. Our results suggest that no specific retrieval affordances with regard to time periods can be distinguished. A higher-order model, with a second-order Gr factor above three first-order factors (IF, WF, EF, and FF) fitted the data best, extending previous literature by increasing construct coverage through the implementation of FF. All covariates show incremental predictive validity, beyond their communality. Summarizing, our results endorse a perspective on Gr as a strong and discriminant factor of cognitive abilities that is not affected by time constraints, and show that Gr is more than a linear combination of its parts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"301-319"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Both congruent and incongruent trials drive the congruency sequence effect: Novel support for an episodic retrieval view of adaptive control in the prime-probe task. 相同和不相同的试验都会产生相同序列效应:为素色探究任务中适应性控制的外显检索观点提供了新的支持。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001349
Matthew G Dunaway, Daniel H Weissman

The congruency effect in Stroop-like tasks-a popular measure of distraction-is smaller after incongruent relative to congruent trials. However, it is unclear whether this congruency sequence effect (CSE)-a popular index of coping with distraction-reflects adjustments of control after congruent trials, incongruent trials, or both. The episodic retrieval account of the CSE posits adjustments of control after both congruent and incongruent trials. In this account, retrieving a memory of the previous trial's congruency (i.e., congruent or incongruent) biases control processes to prepare for an upcoming trial with the same congruency (i.e., congruent or incongruent). In contrast, the default setting account posits adjustments of control after a single trial type. For example, control processes might increase inhibition of the response cued by the distractor after incongruent trials but make no adjustments after congruent trials. To distinguish between these accounts for the first time while (a) using long distractor-target intervals and (b) excluding prevalent feature integration and contingency learning confounds, we employed a confound-minimized prime-probe task with neutral trials. We usually observed adjustments of control after both trial types. Furthermore, whether the reduction of the congruency effect after incongruent trials indexed (a) inhibition of the distractor-congruent response or (b) activation of the distractor-incongruent response depended on whether the distractor and target were same-sized or different-sized, respectively. These findings favor the episodic retrieval account of the CSE over the default setting account. They also indicate that "low-level" stimulus properties may influence the nature of "high-level" control adjustments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

Stroop类似任务中的一致性效应--一种常用的注意力分散测量指标--在不一致试验后相对于一致试验后更小。然而,目前还不清楚这种一致性序列效应(CSE)--一种应对分心的流行指标--反映的是在一致性试验、不一致性试验或两者之后的控制调整。CSE的外显检索理论认为,在相同和不相同的试验后都会调整控制。在这种解释中,对上一次试验的一致性(即一致或不一致)记忆的检索会使控制过程偏向于为即将到来的具有相同一致性(即一致或不一致)的试验做准备。与此相反,默认设置理论认为控制过程会在单一试验类型之后进行调整。例如,在不一致的试验后,控制过程可能会增加对分心物所提示反应的抑制,但在一致的试验后则不会做出任何调整。为了首次区分这些说法,同时(a)使用较长的分心物-目标间隔和(b)排除普遍存在的特征整合和权变学习混淆,我们采用了中性试验的混淆最小化素色探究任务。在这两种试验类型之后,我们通常都能观察到控制的调整。此外,不一致试验后一致性效应的降低是指(a)对分心物-一致反应的抑制还是(b)对分心物-不一致反应的激活,这取决于分心物和目标物的大小是相同还是不同。与默认设置的观点相比,这些研究结果更倾向于 CSE 的记忆性检索观点。它们还表明,"低层次 "刺激属性可能会影响 "高层次 "控制调整的性质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Both congruent and incongruent trials drive the congruency sequence effect: Novel support for an episodic retrieval view of adaptive control in the prime-probe task.","authors":"Matthew G Dunaway, Daniel H Weissman","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001349","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001349","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The congruency effect in Stroop-like tasks-a popular measure of distraction-is smaller after incongruent relative to congruent trials. However, it is unclear whether this congruency sequence effect (CSE)-a popular index of coping with distraction-reflects adjustments of control after congruent trials, incongruent trials, or both. The episodic retrieval account of the CSE posits adjustments of control after both congruent and incongruent trials. In this account, retrieving a memory of the previous trial's congruency (i.e., congruent or incongruent) biases control processes to prepare for an upcoming trial with the same congruency (i.e., congruent or incongruent). In contrast, the default setting account posits adjustments of control after a single trial type. For example, control processes might increase inhibition of the response cued by the distractor after incongruent trials but make no adjustments after congruent trials. To distinguish between these accounts for the first time while (a) using long distractor-target intervals and (b) excluding prevalent feature integration and contingency learning confounds, we employed a confound-minimized prime-probe task with neutral trials. We usually observed adjustments of control after both trial types. Furthermore, whether the reduction of the congruency effect after incongruent trials indexed (a) inhibition of the distractor-congruent response or (b) activation of the distractor-incongruent response depended on whether the distractor and target were same-sized or different-sized, respectively. These findings favor the episodic retrieval account of the CSE over the default setting account. They also indicate that \"low-level\" stimulus properties may influence the nature of \"high-level\" control adjustments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"171-189"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of emotional valence of mind wandering on sustained attention performance. 思绪游荡的情绪情感对持续注意力表现的影响
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001369
Matthew S Welhaf, Jonathan B Banks

The construct of mind wandering has notoriously been characterized as heterogenous which may mean that not all types of mind wandering produce the same pattern of results. One operationalization of mind wandering, task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs), can also itself vary in many dimensions, including the emotional valence of TUTs. The current study summarizes several years of work examining the impact that the emotional valence of TUTs has on different aspects of sustained attention. Participants in several studies reported whether their TUTs were negative, neutral, or positive in emotional valence during a sustained attention-to-response task (SART). The first major focus was a meta-analysis where we examined correlations between each TUT valence and SART performance measures. For the second major focus, we tested how different TUT valences changed over the course of the task. The results suggest that negative TUTs typically show stronger associations with SART performance measures, although all TUT valences have numerically similar correlations. Regarding time-on-task effects, across the studies, there was consistent evidence for a linear increase in negative TUTs across blocks. Evidence for this linear increase was not consistent for neutral and positive TUTs. The results of the current study suggest that the relationships between TUTs and performance, and their likelihood of occurring during a task, are not necessarily the same for every type of TUT. These results highlight the importance of continuing to investigate different types of TUTs and different forms of mind wandering, in general, to better understand how this phenomenon occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

众所周知,思绪游走的结构具有异质性,这可能意味着并非所有类型的思绪游走都会产生相同的结果。思维游移的一种操作方式,即与任务无关的想法(TUTs),其本身也会在许多方面发生变化,其中包括 TUTs 的情绪情感。本研究总结了数年来研究 TUTs 的情绪价值对持续注意力不同方面的影响的工作。在几项研究中,受试者报告了在持续注意-反应任务(SART)中,TUTs 的情绪价位是消极的、中性的还是积极的。第一个重点是荟萃分析,我们研究了每种 TUT 情调与 SART 成绩测量之间的相关性。第二个重点是,我们测试了不同的 TUT 情调在任务过程中是如何变化的。结果表明,负性 TUT 通常与 SART 性能指标有更强的相关性,尽管所有 TUT 价位在数量上具有相似的相关性。关于任务时间效应,在所有的研究中,有一致的证据表明负性 TUTs 在各个区块中呈线性增长。中性和阳性 TUT 线性增加的证据并不一致。本研究的结果表明,TUT 与成绩之间的关系以及在任务中发生的可能性并不一定对每种类型的 TUT 都相同。这些结果凸显了继续研究不同类型的TUT和不同形式的思维游离的重要性,以便更好地了解这种现象是如何发生的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Effects of emotional valence of mind wandering on sustained attention performance.","authors":"Matthew S Welhaf, Jonathan B Banks","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001369","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001369","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The construct of mind wandering has notoriously been characterized as heterogenous which may mean that not all types of mind wandering produce the same pattern of results. One operationalization of mind wandering, task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs), can also itself vary in many dimensions, including the emotional valence of TUTs. The current study summarizes several years of work examining the impact that the emotional valence of TUTs has on different aspects of sustained attention. Participants in several studies reported whether their TUTs were negative, neutral, or positive in emotional valence during a sustained attention-to-response task (SART). The first major focus was a meta-analysis where we examined correlations between each TUT valence and SART performance measures. For the second major focus, we tested how different TUT valences changed over the course of the task. The results suggest that negative TUTs typically show stronger associations with SART performance measures, although all TUT valences have numerically similar correlations. Regarding time-on-task effects, across the studies, there was consistent evidence for a linear increase in negative TUTs across blocks. Evidence for this linear increase was not consistent for neutral and positive TUTs. The results of the current study suggest that the relationships between TUTs and performance, and their likelihood of occurring during a task, are not necessarily the same for every type of TUT. These results highlight the importance of continuing to investigate different types of TUTs and different forms of mind wandering, in general, to better understand how this phenomenon occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"238-254"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11666809/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory modeling of counterfactual generation. 反事实生成的记忆建模。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001335
Feiyi Wang, Ada Aka, Lisheng He, Sudeep Bhatia

We use a computational model of memory search to study how people generate counterfactual outcomes in response to an established target outcome. Hierarchical Bayesian model fitting to data from six experiments reveals that counterfactual outcomes that are perceived as more desirable and more likely to occur are also more likely to come to mind and are generated earlier than other outcomes. Additionally, core memory mechanisms such as semantic clustering and word frequency biases have a strong influence on retrieval dynamics in counterfactual thinking. Finally, we find that the set of counterfactuals that come to mind can be manipulated by modifying the total number of counterfactuals that participants are prompted to generate, and our model can predict these effects. Overall, our findings demonstrate how computational memory search models can be integrated with current theories of counterfactual thinking to provide novel insights into the process of generating counterfactual thoughts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

我们利用记忆搜索的计算模型来研究人们如何针对既定的目标结果产生反事实结果。对来自六个实验的数据进行分层贝叶斯模型拟合后发现,与其他结果相比,人们认为更理想、更有可能发生的反事实结果也更有可能出现在脑海中,并且更早产生。此外,语义聚类和词频偏差等核心记忆机制对反事实思维的检索动态有很大影响。最后,我们发现,可以通过改变参与者被提示生成的反事实的总数来操纵所想到的反事实的集合,而我们的模型可以预测这些效果。总之,我们的研究结果表明,计算记忆搜索模型可以与当前的反事实思维理论相结合,为反事实思维的产生过程提供新的见解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Memory modeling of counterfactual generation.","authors":"Feiyi Wang, Ada Aka, Lisheng He, Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001335","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001335","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We use a computational model of memory search to study how people generate counterfactual outcomes in response to an established target outcome. Hierarchical Bayesian model fitting to data from six experiments reveals that counterfactual outcomes that are perceived as more desirable and more likely to occur are also more likely to come to mind and are generated earlier than other outcomes. Additionally, core memory mechanisms such as semantic clustering and word frequency biases have a strong influence on retrieval dynamics in counterfactual thinking. Finally, we find that the set of counterfactuals that come to mind can be manipulated by modifying the total number of counterfactuals that participants are prompted to generate, and our model can predict these effects. Overall, our findings demonstrate how computational memory search models can be integrated with current theories of counterfactual thinking to provide novel insights into the process of generating counterfactual thoughts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"255-284"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140865703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1