首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
You sound like an evil young man: A distributional semantic analysis of systematic form-meaning associations for polarity, gender, and age in fictional characters' names. 你听起来像个邪恶的年轻人:对虚构人物姓名中极性、性别和年龄的系统形式-意义关联的分布语义分析。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001345
Aron Y Joosse, Gökçe Kuscu, Giovanni Cassani

We detail a successful attempt in modeling associations about the age, gender, and polarity of fictional characters based on their names alone. We started by collecting ratings through an online survey on a sample of annotated names from young-adult, children, and fan-fiction stories. We collected ratings over three semantic differentials (gender: male-female; age: old-young; polarity: evil-good) using a slider bar. First, we show that participants tend to agree with authors: names judged to better suit female/young/evil characters tend to be assigned to female/young/evil characters in the original stories. We then show that, in a series of computational studies, we can predict participants' ratings on the three attributes using a distributional semantic model which derives representations for both lexical and sublexical patterns. This attempt was successful for all names, including made-up ones, and using both a supervised and an unsupervised approach. The prediction supported by distributed representations is much better than that afforded by symbolic features such as letters and phonological features, also when accounting for the complexity of the feature spaces. Our results show that people interpret both known and novel names relying on lexical and sublexical patterns, which suggests the availability of systematic form-meaning mappings in everyday language use. This further lends credit to the hypothesis that language internal statistics can support systematic form-meaning associations which apply to both known and novel lexical items. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们详细介绍了一次成功的尝试,即仅根据虚构人物的名字,建立有关其年龄、性别和极性的联想模型。我们首先通过在线调查收集了来自青少年、儿童和同人小说的注释姓名样本的评分。我们使用滑动条收集了三种语义差异(性别:男-女;年龄:老-幼;极性:恶-善)的评分。首先,我们表明参与者倾向于同意作者的观点:被判定为更适合女性/年轻/邪恶角色的名字往往会被分配给原创故事中的女性/年轻/邪恶角色。然后,我们通过一系列计算研究表明,我们可以使用分布式语义模型预测参与者对这三个属性的评价,该模型可以得出词性和次词性模式的表征。这一尝试成功地适用于所有名称,包括编造的名称,并同时使用了监督和非监督方法。考虑到特征空间的复杂性,分布式表征所支持的预测结果要比字母和语音特征等符号特征所提供的预测结果好得多。我们的研究结果表明,人们对已知名称和新名称的解释都依赖于词汇和次词汇模式,这表明在日常语言使用中存在系统的形式-意义映射。这进一步证实了语言内部统计可以支持系统形式-意义关联的假设,这种关联既适用于已知词项,也适用于新词项。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"You sound like an evil young man: A distributional semantic analysis of systematic form-meaning associations for polarity, gender, and age in fictional characters' names.","authors":"Aron Y Joosse, Gökçe Kuscu, Giovanni Cassani","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001345","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001345","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We detail a successful attempt in modeling associations about the age, gender, and polarity of fictional characters based on their names alone. We started by collecting ratings through an online survey on a sample of annotated names from young-adult, children, and fan-fiction stories. We collected ratings over three semantic differentials (gender: male-female; age: old-young; polarity: evil-good) using a slider bar. First, we show that participants tend to agree with authors: names judged to better suit female/young/evil characters tend to be assigned to female/young/evil characters in the original stories. We then show that, in a series of computational studies, we can predict participants' ratings on the three attributes using a distributional semantic model which derives representations for both lexical and sublexical patterns. This attempt was successful for all names, including made-up ones, and using both a supervised and an unsupervised approach. The prediction supported by distributed representations is much better than that afforded by symbolic features such as letters and phonological features, also when accounting for the complexity of the feature spaces. Our results show that people interpret both known and novel names relying on lexical and sublexical patterns, which suggests the availability of systematic form-meaning mappings in everyday language use. This further lends credit to the hypothesis that language internal statistics can support systematic form-meaning associations which apply to both known and novel lexical items. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceiving the "smallest" or "largest" multidigit number: A novel numeric-scale end effect. 感知 "最小 "或 "最大 "的多位数:新颖的数字尺度末端效应
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001386
Mariya Lozin, Michal Pinhas

The ability to recognize the smallest/largest objects or numbers in our environment is an essential cognitive skill, however, little is known about perceiving multidigit numbers as end-values. The present study examined end effects in multidigit numbers and whether such effects are impacted by number length, which captures the numeric scale (e.g., 10's, 100's). Across four experiments (N = 120, 100, 80, and 120, respectively), participants performed three types of numerical comparisons: same-scale comparisons between end-values and nonend-values (e.g., 100 vs. 200), different-scale comparisons between end-values and nonend-values (e.g., 1,000 vs. 200), and same-scale comparisons of nonend-values (e.g., 300 vs. 200). The type of the end-value (i.e., lower/upper) and overall numerical range used in each experiment varied. The results revealed: (a) a novel numeric-scale end effect, characterized by a relatively small end effect for comparisons between nonend-values and end-values from an adjacent numeric scale, and a larger, consistently sized end effect for comparisons between nonend-values and end-values from nonadjacent numeric scales (≥ 2 scales), (b) absent or weak end effects in same-scale multidigit number comparisons, and (c) replication of the lower end effect for single-digit comparisons to the end-value 1. These results reveal differential processing of numbers from adjacent versus nonadjacent numeric scales. We rule out a psychophysical explanation for our findings and instead provide a syntactic explanation based on the perceptual dominance of the numeric-scale component and the way it manifests in the counting process. We conclude that a number's scale plays a crucial role in evaluating multidigit number magnitude. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

识别周围环境中最小/最大物体或数字的能力是一项基本的认知技能,然而,人们对多位数的末端效应知之甚少。本研究考察了多位数的末端效应,以及这种效应是否会受到数字长度的影响,数字长度反映了数字的大小(如 10、100)。在四次实验中(人数分别为 120、100、80 和 120),参与者进行了三种类型的数字比较:尾数和非尾数之间的同尺度比较(如 100 对 200)、尾数和非尾数之间的异尺度比较(如 1,000 对 200)以及非尾数的同尺度比较(如 300 对 200)。每个实验中使用的终值类型(即较低/较高)和总体数值范围各不相同。结果显示:(a) 一种新的数字量表端值效应,其特点是在非端值和相邻数字量表的端值之间进行比较时,端值效应相对较小,而在非端值和非相邻数字量表(≥ 2 个量表)的端值之间进行比较时,端值效应较大且大小一致;(b) 在相同量表的多位数比较中没有端值效应或端值效应较弱;(c) 在与端值 1 进行个位数比较时,低端值效应得到复制。这些结果揭示了相邻和非相邻数字尺度的数字处理过程存在差异。我们排除了对研究结果的心理物理学解释,转而根据数字刻度成分的感知优势及其在计数过程中的表现方式提供了一种句法解释。我们的结论是,数字的尺度在评估多位数的大小时起着至关重要的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Perceiving the \"smallest\" or \"largest\" multidigit number: A novel numeric-scale end effect.","authors":"Mariya Lozin, Michal Pinhas","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001386","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001386","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to recognize the smallest/largest objects or numbers in our environment is an essential cognitive skill, however, little is known about perceiving multidigit numbers as end-values. The present study examined end effects in multidigit numbers and whether such effects are impacted by number length, which captures the numeric scale (e.g., 10's, 100's). Across four experiments (<i>N</i> = 120, 100, 80, and 120, respectively), participants performed three types of numerical comparisons: same-scale comparisons between end-values and nonend-values (e.g., 100 vs. 200), different-scale comparisons between end-values and nonend-values (e.g., 1,000 vs. 200), and same-scale comparisons of nonend-values (e.g., 300 vs. 200). The type of the end-value (i.e., lower/upper) and overall numerical range used in each experiment varied. The results revealed: (a) a novel <i>numeric-scale end effect</i>, characterized by a relatively small end effect for comparisons between nonend-values and end-values from an adjacent numeric scale, and a larger, consistently sized end effect for comparisons between nonend-values and end-values from nonadjacent numeric scales (≥ 2 scales), (b) absent or weak end effects in same-scale multidigit number comparisons, and (c) replication of the lower end effect for single-digit comparisons to the end-value 1. These results reveal differential processing of numbers from adjacent versus nonadjacent numeric scales. We rule out a psychophysical explanation for our findings and instead provide a syntactic explanation based on the perceptual dominance of the numeric-scale component and the way it manifests in the counting process. We conclude that a number's scale plays a crucial role in evaluating multidigit number magnitude. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are selection history effects limited to implicit forms of memory? Evidence from intertrial repetition. 选择史效应仅限于内隐记忆形式吗?试验间重复的证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001387
Ariel M Kershner,Andrew Hollingworth
Selection history effects in visual attention are typically considered implicit memory effects. In three experiments, we investigated if a key selection history effect, intertrial priming, could be based on the incidental application of explicit memory. In the basic search task (Experiment 1), participants searched for real-world objects from different categories. We examined nonpredictive, intertrial repetition at two levels: (1) the repetition of target location from trial N-1 to trial N and (2) the repetition of target location and color within a category. Reliable repetition advantages were observed at both levels. In Experiments 2-4, we examined whether participants had explicit access to the target values driving the selection history effects here. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants could reliably report the properties of the immediately preceding search target. In Experiment 4, participants could reliably report the properties of the last target exemplar they had found in each of the 36 categories. These data indicate that guidance by selection history was based on the nonstrategic application of memory representations that could be explicitly retrieved and reported. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
视觉注意力中的选择历史效应通常被认为是内隐记忆效应。在三项实验中,我们研究了一种关键的选择历史效应--试验间引物--是否可能基于显性记忆的偶然应用。在基本搜索任务(实验 1)中,参与者搜索真实世界中不同类别的物体。我们从两个层面考察了非预测性的试验间重复:(1)从试验 N-1 到试验 N 的目标位置重复;(2)一个类别中目标位置和颜色的重复。在这两个水平上都观察到了可靠的重复优势。在实验 2-4 中,我们考察了参与者是否能够明确地获得驱动此处选择历史效应的目标值。在实验 2 和 3 中,参与者可以可靠地报告紧接着的搜索目标的属性。在实验 4 中,被试可以可靠地报告他们在 36 个类别中找到的最后一个目标示例的属性。这些数据表明,选择历史的引导是基于记忆表征的非策略性应用,而这些记忆表征是可以明确检索和报告的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Are selection history effects limited to implicit forms of memory? Evidence from intertrial repetition.","authors":"Ariel M Kershner,Andrew Hollingworth","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001387","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001387","url":null,"abstract":"Selection history effects in visual attention are typically considered implicit memory effects. In three experiments, we investigated if a key selection history effect, intertrial priming, could be based on the incidental application of explicit memory. In the basic search task (Experiment 1), participants searched for real-world objects from different categories. We examined nonpredictive, intertrial repetition at two levels: (1) the repetition of target location from trial N-1 to trial N and (2) the repetition of target location and color within a category. Reliable repetition advantages were observed at both levels. In Experiments 2-4, we examined whether participants had explicit access to the target values driving the selection history effects here. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants could reliably report the properties of the immediately preceding search target. In Experiment 4, participants could reliably report the properties of the last target exemplar they had found in each of the 36 categories. These data indicate that guidance by selection history was based on the nonstrategic application of memory representations that could be explicitly retrieved and reported. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"9 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142226229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How do theory of mind and formal language skills impact metaphoric reference comprehension during children's school-age years. 思维理论和正式语言技能如何影响学龄儿童的隐喻参照理解。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001381
Nicolas Petit,Valentina Bambini,Luca Bischetti,Jérôme Prado,Ira Noveck
Prior research has shown that school-aged children's metaphor comprehension becomes adultlike progressively. This has given rise to claims that the development of metaphor comprehension is due to children's evolving abilities with respect to theory of mind (ToM) or to formal language. The present work investigates the extent to which children's growing sophistication with metaphor is attributable to each of these. Experiment 1 validates a newly constructed tablet task-with two groups of children whose mean ages were approximately 7 and 10 (N = 89)-in which participants (a) listen to vignettes that conclude with either a metaphoric or a synonymic (control) reference and then (b) choose pictures (while latencies are recorded) that indicate whether the children understand the reference as intended. The outcomes from Experiment 1 confirm prior results: Accurate responding in the wake of a metaphoric reference increases with age; meanwhile, correct metaphoric responses take longer than synonymic ones. Experiment 2 tests a more expansive range of 6- to 11-year-olds (N = 248) and a wider array of tasks, including two clinical tasks measuring ToM and formal language skills which we use as cognitive predictors of metaphor accuracy and response times. Results show that ToM is a reliable predictor of successful performance on the metaphor task among younger children, before attenuating with age; in contrast, formal language is a predictor of metaphor comprehension that strengthens with age and is maximal in older children. This work underlines the importance of considering developmental perspectives when investigating the cognitive bases of metaphor skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
先前的研究表明,学龄儿童的隐喻理解能力会逐渐变得像成人一样。因此,有人认为,隐喻理解能力的发展是由于儿童在心智理论(ToM)或正式语言方面的能力不断发展所致。本研究调查了儿童隐喻能力的不断提高在多大程度上可归因于上述两种能力。实验 1 验证了一项新构建的平板电脑任务--由平均年龄约为 7 岁和 10 岁的两组儿童完成(N = 89)--在这项任务中,参与者(a)聆听以隐喻或同义词(对照)为结尾的小故事,然后(b)选择图片(同时记录潜伏期),以显示儿童是否理解了所要表达的意思。实验 1 的结果证实了之前的结果:随着年龄的增长,儿童对隐喻参照物的准确反应也会增加;同时,正确的隐喻反应比同义词反应需要更长的时间。实验 2 测试了范围更广的 6 至 11 岁儿童(N = 248)和更多的任务,包括两个测量 ToM 和正式语言技能的临床任务,我们将其作为隐喻准确性和反应时间的认知预测因子。结果表明,在年龄较小的儿童中, ToM 是成功完成隐喻任务的可靠预测因素,但随着年龄的增长,ToM 的作用会减弱;相比之下,正式语言是隐喻理解的预测因素,随着年龄的增长会增强,在年龄较大的儿童中作用最大。这项研究强调,在研究隐喻技能的认知基础时,从发展的角度进行考虑非常重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How do theory of mind and formal language skills impact metaphoric reference comprehension during children's school-age years.","authors":"Nicolas Petit,Valentina Bambini,Luca Bischetti,Jérôme Prado,Ira Noveck","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001381","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001381","url":null,"abstract":"Prior research has shown that school-aged children's metaphor comprehension becomes adultlike progressively. This has given rise to claims that the development of metaphor comprehension is due to children's evolving abilities with respect to theory of mind (ToM) or to formal language. The present work investigates the extent to which children's growing sophistication with metaphor is attributable to each of these. Experiment 1 validates a newly constructed tablet task-with two groups of children whose mean ages were approximately 7 and 10 (N = 89)-in which participants (a) listen to vignettes that conclude with either a metaphoric or a synonymic (control) reference and then (b) choose pictures (while latencies are recorded) that indicate whether the children understand the reference as intended. The outcomes from Experiment 1 confirm prior results: Accurate responding in the wake of a metaphoric reference increases with age; meanwhile, correct metaphoric responses take longer than synonymic ones. Experiment 2 tests a more expansive range of 6- to 11-year-olds (N = 248) and a wider array of tasks, including two clinical tasks measuring ToM and formal language skills which we use as cognitive predictors of metaphor accuracy and response times. Results show that ToM is a reliable predictor of successful performance on the metaphor task among younger children, before attenuating with age; in contrast, formal language is a predictor of metaphor comprehension that strengthens with age and is maximal in older children. This work underlines the importance of considering developmental perspectives when investigating the cognitive bases of metaphor skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"61 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142226230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Categorically distinct subsets allow flexible memory selection in hybrid search. 不同类别的子集允许在混合搜索中灵活选择内存。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001377
Nurit Gronau,Makaela Nartker,Sharon Yakim,Igor Utochkin,Jeremy Wolfe
In many everyday situations, we search our visual surroundings for any one of many memorized items held in memory, a process termed hybrid search. In some cases, only a portion of the memorized mental list is relevant within a specific visual context, thus, restricting memory search to the relevant subset would be desirable. Previous research had shown that participants largely fail to "partition" memory into several distinct subsets, on a trial-by-trial basis. However, given the known role of semantic content in long-term memory organization, we hypothesized that semantically defined subset categories might serve as a more powerful means for flexible memory selection in dynamic hybrid search situations. Experiment 1 revealed that, indeed, semantic characteristics (i.e., object category), but not perceptual features (e.g., arbitrary color), can provide a firm basis for flexible memory partitioning. Experiments 2 and 3 further showed that such memory partitioning is costless and is independent of the nature of the surrounding visual distractors (i.e., a categorically homogeneous or heterogeneous display). These findings demonstrate that confining one's memory search to a currently relevant subset of items is highly effective when the different memory subsets are defined by clear semantic categories. The results underscore the importance of conceptual information in the organization of activated long-term memory and in forming the basis for a flexible trial-by-trial memory selection. Our findings further highlight the relationship between visual search and memory search, and they may shed light on the processes contributing to a successful construction of bounded episodes in long-term memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在许多日常情况下,我们会在视觉环境中搜索记忆中的许多记忆项目中的任何一个,这一过程被称为混合搜索。在某些情况下,记忆中的心理清单只有一部分与特定的视觉环境相关,因此,最好将记忆搜索限制在相关的子集上。以前的研究表明,参与者在逐次试验的基础上,基本上无法将记忆 "分割 "成几个不同的子集。然而,鉴于语义内容在长期记忆组织中的已知作用,我们假设,在动态混合搜索情况下,语义定义的子集类别可能会成为灵活选择记忆的更有力手段。实验 1 显示,语义特征(即对象类别)而非感知特征(如任意颜色)的确可以为灵活的记忆分区提供坚实的基础。实验2和3进一步表明,这种记忆分区是无成本的,而且与周围视觉干扰物的性质(即同类或异类显示)无关。这些研究结果表明,当不同的记忆子集由明确的语义类别定义时,将记忆搜索限制在当前相关的项目子集上是非常有效的。这些结果强调了概念信息在组织激活的长期记忆和形成灵活的逐次试验记忆选择基础方面的重要性。我们的研究结果进一步强调了视觉搜索与记忆搜索之间的关系,并可能揭示了在长时记忆中成功构建有界情节的过程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Categorically distinct subsets allow flexible memory selection in hybrid search.","authors":"Nurit Gronau,Makaela Nartker,Sharon Yakim,Igor Utochkin,Jeremy Wolfe","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001377","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001377","url":null,"abstract":"In many everyday situations, we search our visual surroundings for any one of many memorized items held in memory, a process termed hybrid search. In some cases, only a portion of the memorized mental list is relevant within a specific visual context, thus, restricting memory search to the relevant subset would be desirable. Previous research had shown that participants largely fail to \"partition\" memory into several distinct subsets, on a trial-by-trial basis. However, given the known role of semantic content in long-term memory organization, we hypothesized that semantically defined subset categories might serve as a more powerful means for flexible memory selection in dynamic hybrid search situations. Experiment 1 revealed that, indeed, semantic characteristics (i.e., object category), but not perceptual features (e.g., arbitrary color), can provide a firm basis for flexible memory partitioning. Experiments 2 and 3 further showed that such memory partitioning is costless and is independent of the nature of the surrounding visual distractors (i.e., a categorically homogeneous or heterogeneous display). These findings demonstrate that confining one's memory search to a currently relevant subset of items is highly effective when the different memory subsets are defined by clear semantic categories. The results underscore the importance of conceptual information in the organization of activated long-term memory and in forming the basis for a flexible trial-by-trial memory selection. Our findings further highlight the relationship between visual search and memory search, and they may shed light on the processes contributing to a successful construction of bounded episodes in long-term memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"53 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142226412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A continuous aggregated accumulation model of recognition judgments. 识别判断的连续聚合积累模型。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001379
Mengxue Kang,Neha Sinha,Melchi M Michel,Rhea Khandelwal,Brian Wang,Muzzammil Ahmed,Arnold L Glass
In recognition, remember-responses are understood to be based on recollection, and know-responses are understood to be based on familiarity. Two kinds of models have been proposed for the process by which recognition decisions are made. In single-process models, familiarity and recollection are integrated, and there is a single criterion for recognition. In dual-process models, familiarity and recollection are segregated, and there are separate criteria for remember and know-judgments. Recent process models can account for the distribution of remember and know-responses (under a range of different assumptions) but do not address the time course of the recognition process. Paradoxical findings, indicating that familiarity is available faster than recollection but remember-responses are on average faster than know-responses, cannot be convincingly explained by any existing dual-process model. We propose a new model that resolves this paradox by analyzing in detail the time course of recollection and familiarity. Know-responses based on the high familiarity of the test item are faster than remember-responses based on recollection. However, low-familiarity, low-recollection responses are slow and are also categorized by participants as know-responses. Hence, the average know-response time is slower than average remember-response time because know-responses include both fast high-familiarity responses and slow low-familiarity, low-recollection responses. A 12-parameter quantitative model that describes the relationship among the effects of confidence, accuracy, and remember and know categorization on accuracy and reaction time provided the best fit between expected reaction time and observed reaction time among the models tested. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在识别过程中,"记得 "反应被理解为基于回忆,而 "知道 "反应被理解为基于熟悉。对于做出识别决定的过程,人们提出了两种模式。在单过程模型中,熟悉和回忆是结合在一起的,而且识别只有一个标准。在双过程模型中,熟悉和回忆是分开的,记忆和认识的判断标准是不同的。最新的过程模型可以解释记住和知道的反应的分布(在一系列不同的假设下),但没有解决识别过程的时间进程问题。现有的任何双过程模型都无法令人信服地解释这种自相矛盾的发现,即熟悉的速度快于回忆的速度,但记住的反应平均快于知道的反应。我们提出了一个新模型,通过详细分析回忆和熟悉的时间过程来解决这一悖论。基于对测试项目高度熟悉的 "知道 "反应比基于回忆的 "记住 "反应更快。然而,低熟悉度、低回忆度的反应速度较慢,参与者也将其归类为 "知道 "反应。因此,平均 "知道-反应 "时间比平均 "记住-反应 "时间慢,因为 "知道-反应 "既包括快速的高熟悉度反应,也包括慢速的低熟悉度、低回忆性反应。在所测试的模型中,有一个 12 参数的定量模型能最好地拟合预期反应时间和观察到的反应时间,该模型描述了信心、准确性、记住和知道分类对准确性和反应时间的影响之间的关系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A continuous aggregated accumulation model of recognition judgments.","authors":"Mengxue Kang,Neha Sinha,Melchi M Michel,Rhea Khandelwal,Brian Wang,Muzzammil Ahmed,Arnold L Glass","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001379","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001379","url":null,"abstract":"In recognition, remember-responses are understood to be based on recollection, and know-responses are understood to be based on familiarity. Two kinds of models have been proposed for the process by which recognition decisions are made. In single-process models, familiarity and recollection are integrated, and there is a single criterion for recognition. In dual-process models, familiarity and recollection are segregated, and there are separate criteria for remember and know-judgments. Recent process models can account for the distribution of remember and know-responses (under a range of different assumptions) but do not address the time course of the recognition process. Paradoxical findings, indicating that familiarity is available faster than recollection but remember-responses are on average faster than know-responses, cannot be convincingly explained by any existing dual-process model. We propose a new model that resolves this paradox by analyzing in detail the time course of recollection and familiarity. Know-responses based on the high familiarity of the test item are faster than remember-responses based on recollection. However, low-familiarity, low-recollection responses are slow and are also categorized by participants as know-responses. Hence, the average know-response time is slower than average remember-response time because know-responses include both fast high-familiarity responses and slow low-familiarity, low-recollection responses. A 12-parameter quantitative model that describes the relationship among the effects of confidence, accuracy, and remember and know categorization on accuracy and reaction time provided the best fit between expected reaction time and observed reaction time among the models tested. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205625","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gesture size affects numerical estimates in quantifier comprehension. 手势大小会影响量词理解中的数字估计。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001372
Alexandra Lorson,Vinicius Macuch-Silva,Christopher Hart,Bodo Winter
People think and talk about numerical magnitude in terms of space, and co-speech gestures reflect this, with English speakers using expansive gestures when talking about greater quantities. Existing gestural research on the spatial conceptualization of number has largely looked at gesture production, but we do not know whether gestures can influence the interpretation of imprecise or underspecified numerical expressions such as quantifiers. Looking at the quantifier several as a test case, this study investigates the influence of manual inwards-directed (i.e., hands move closer the torso) and outwards-directed (i.e., hands move away from the torso) co-speech gestures on comprehenders' conceptualization of quantities associated with several through three preregistered experiments. Our results suggest that gesture modulates the interpretation of several such that speakers moving their hands outwards and thereby creating space between their hands lead to higher quantity estimates, compared to speakers not gesturing, or moving their hands inwards. We discuss the implications of our findings for future work in numerical cognition, multimodal communication, and pragmatics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
人们从空间的角度思考和谈论数字的大小,共同言语的手势也反映了这一点,讲英语的人在谈论更大的数量时会使用扩张性手势。现有的关于数字空间概念化的手势研究主要着眼于手势的产生,但我们还不知道手势是否会影响对不精确或不明确的数字表达式(如量词)的解释。本研究以数量词 "几 "为测试案例,通过三个预先登记的实验,研究了手动向内(即双手靠近躯干)和向外(即双手远离躯干)的协同语音手势对理解者对与 "几 "相关的数量概念化的影响。我们的结果表明,手势会调节对 "几 "的解释,与不做手势或将手向内移动的人相比,将手向外移动从而在两手之间创造空间的人对 "几 "的数量估计更高。我们将讨论我们的发现对数字认知、多模态交流和语用学等领域未来工作的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Gesture size affects numerical estimates in quantifier comprehension.","authors":"Alexandra Lorson,Vinicius Macuch-Silva,Christopher Hart,Bodo Winter","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001372","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001372","url":null,"abstract":"People think and talk about numerical magnitude in terms of space, and co-speech gestures reflect this, with English speakers using expansive gestures when talking about greater quantities. Existing gestural research on the spatial conceptualization of number has largely looked at gesture production, but we do not know whether gestures can influence the interpretation of imprecise or underspecified numerical expressions such as quantifiers. Looking at the quantifier several as a test case, this study investigates the influence of manual inwards-directed (i.e., hands move closer the torso) and outwards-directed (i.e., hands move away from the torso) co-speech gestures on comprehenders' conceptualization of quantities associated with several through three preregistered experiments. Our results suggest that gesture modulates the interpretation of several such that speakers moving their hands outwards and thereby creating space between their hands lead to higher quantity estimates, compared to speakers not gesturing, or moving their hands inwards. We discuss the implications of our findings for future work in numerical cognition, multimodal communication, and pragmatics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"56 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205628","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mental effort during mindless reading? Pupil fluctuations indicate internal processing during levels of inattention. 无意识阅读时的脑力劳动?瞳孔波动表明注意力不集中时的内部处理过程。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001384
Daniel J Schad,Antje Nuthmann,Frank Rösler,Ralf Engbert
Mind wandering, an experience characterized by a reduced external focus of attention and an increased internal focus, has seen significant theoretical advancement in understanding its underlying cognitive processes. The levels-of-inattention hypothesis posits that in mind wandering, external attention is reduced in a graded fashion, reflecting different levels of weak versus deep attentional decoupling. However, it has remained unclear whether internal processing during mind wandering, and mindless reading in particular, requires effort and, if so, whether it is graded or distinct. To address this, we analyzed pupil size as a measure of cognitive load in the sustained-attention-to-stimulus task during text reading. We examined whether decoupled external attention is linked to an overall reduction in workload and whether internal focus of attention is graded or represents a distinct cognitive process. Overall, overlooking errors in the text was associated with a small pupil size, indicating reduced effortful processing. However, this effect varied with error type: overlooking high- or medium-level errors (weak decoupling) resulted in reduced pupil size, while overlooking low-level errors (deep decoupling) had no effect on pupil size. Moreover, detecting an error (at any processing level) elicited a task-evoked pupillary response, which was absent when it was overlooked. These findings suggest that weak decoupling reduces internal resource-demanding processing and are in line with the hypothesis that large pupils during deep decoupling may be associated with distinct states of effortful internal processing. They further support both the levels-of-inattention hypothesis and the notion that internal focus is a distinct mode of deeply decoupled processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
心智游离是一种以外部注意力降低和内部注意力增加为特征的体验,在理解其基本认知过程方面取得了重大的理论进展。注意水平假说认为,在心智游离中,外部注意以分级的方式减少,反映了不同程度的弱注意与深注意解耦。然而,目前还不清楚在思维游离,特别是无意识阅读时,内部处理是否需要努力,如果需要,它是分级的还是独特的。为了解决这个问题,我们分析了瞳孔大小,以此来衡量文本阅读过程中持续注意刺激任务的认知负荷。我们研究了外部注意力的分离是否与整体工作量的减少有关,以及内部注意力的集中是分级的还是代表一个独特的认知过程。总体而言,忽略文本中的错误与瞳孔缩小有关,这表明费力的处理过程减少了。然而,这种效应随错误类型的不同而变化:忽略高、中级错误(弱解耦)会导致瞳孔缩小,而忽略低级错误(深解耦)则对瞳孔大小没有影响。此外,发现错误(在任何处理水平上)都会引起任务诱发的瞳孔反应,而忽略错误时则没有这种反应。这些研究结果表明,弱解耦减少了内部资源需求处理,并与深度解耦时的大瞳孔可能与不同的费力内部处理状态有关的假设相一致。它们进一步支持了注意力水平假说和内部专注是深度解耦处理的一种独特模式的观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mental effort during mindless reading? Pupil fluctuations indicate internal processing during levels of inattention.","authors":"Daniel J Schad,Antje Nuthmann,Frank Rösler,Ralf Engbert","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001384","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001384","url":null,"abstract":"Mind wandering, an experience characterized by a reduced external focus of attention and an increased internal focus, has seen significant theoretical advancement in understanding its underlying cognitive processes. The levels-of-inattention hypothesis posits that in mind wandering, external attention is reduced in a graded fashion, reflecting different levels of weak versus deep attentional decoupling. However, it has remained unclear whether internal processing during mind wandering, and mindless reading in particular, requires effort and, if so, whether it is graded or distinct. To address this, we analyzed pupil size as a measure of cognitive load in the sustained-attention-to-stimulus task during text reading. We examined whether decoupled external attention is linked to an overall reduction in workload and whether internal focus of attention is graded or represents a distinct cognitive process. Overall, overlooking errors in the text was associated with a small pupil size, indicating reduced effortful processing. However, this effect varied with error type: overlooking high- or medium-level errors (weak decoupling) resulted in reduced pupil size, while overlooking low-level errors (deep decoupling) had no effect on pupil size. Moreover, detecting an error (at any processing level) elicited a task-evoked pupillary response, which was absent when it was overlooked. These findings suggest that weak decoupling reduces internal resource-demanding processing and are in line with the hypothesis that large pupils during deep decoupling may be associated with distinct states of effortful internal processing. They further support both the levels-of-inattention hypothesis and the notion that internal focus is a distinct mode of deeply decoupled processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"154 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mouse cursor trajectories capture the flexible adaptivity of predictive sentence processing. 鼠标光标轨迹捕捉了预测句子处理的灵活适应性。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001397
Anuenue Kukona,Nabil Hasshim
Recent psycholinguistic findings raise fundamental questions about comprehenders' ability to rationally adapt their predictions during sentence processing. Two mouse cursor tracking experiments (each N = 85) assessed this adaptivity by manipulating the reliability of verb-based semantic cues. In Experiment 1, predictive mouse cursor movements to targets (e.g., bike) versus distractors (e.g., kite) were measured while participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive (e.g., "spot … the bike"), predictive (e.g., "ride … the bike"), and antipredictive (e.g., "fly … the bike") sentences. In Experiment 2, participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive and antipredictive sentences. Participants were observed to flexibly adapt their predictions, such that they disengaged prediction in Experiment 1 when verb-based cues were unreliable and as likely to be disconfirmed as confirmed, while they generated adapted predictions in Experiment 2 when verb-based cues were reliably disconfirmed. However, links to individual differences in cognitive control were not observed. These results are interpreted as supporting rational theoretical approaches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
最近的心理语言学研究结果提出了一些基本问题,即理解者在句子处理过程中合理调整其预测的能力。两个鼠标光标跟踪实验(每个实验的人数均为 85 人)通过操纵基于动词的语义线索的可靠性来评估这种适应能力。在实验 1 中,当参与者听到同等比例的非预测性句子(如 "发现......自行车")、预测性句子(如 "骑......自行车")和反预测性句子(如 "飞......自行车")时,对目标(如自行车)和干扰项(如风筝)的预测性鼠标光标移动进行了测量。在实验 2 中,参与者听到的非预测性句子和反预测性句子的比例相同。据观察,在实验 1 中,当基于动词的线索不可靠,被否定和被肯定的可能性一样大时,参与者会灵活地调整他们的预测;而在实验 2 中,当基于动词的线索被可靠地否定时,参与者会产生调整后的预测。然而,并没有观察到认知控制方面的个体差异。这些结果被解释为支持合理的理论方法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mouse cursor trajectories capture the flexible adaptivity of predictive sentence processing.","authors":"Anuenue Kukona,Nabil Hasshim","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001397","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001397","url":null,"abstract":"Recent psycholinguistic findings raise fundamental questions about comprehenders' ability to rationally adapt their predictions during sentence processing. Two mouse cursor tracking experiments (each N = 85) assessed this adaptivity by manipulating the reliability of verb-based semantic cues. In Experiment 1, predictive mouse cursor movements to targets (e.g., bike) versus distractors (e.g., kite) were measured while participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive (e.g., \"spot … the bike\"), predictive (e.g., \"ride … the bike\"), and antipredictive (e.g., \"fly … the bike\") sentences. In Experiment 2, participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive and antipredictive sentences. Participants were observed to flexibly adapt their predictions, such that they disengaged prediction in Experiment 1 when verb-based cues were unreliable and as likely to be disconfirmed as confirmed, while they generated adapted predictions in Experiment 2 when verb-based cues were reliably disconfirmed. However, links to individual differences in cognitive control were not observed. These results are interpreted as supporting rational theoretical approaches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task. 词缀不能阻止对模糊词根的语义访问--即使在句子语境中也不能:来自眼动跟踪和迷宫任务的证据。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001378
Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben

How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., bark, with meanings related to both "dog" and "tree") which are disambiguated when affixed by -ing (e.g., barking; related to "dog" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root bark would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as barking (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (dog, tree) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form (barking-tree), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (night) continuation for the sentence He heard loud barking during the … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在句子阅读过程中,语言理解系统是如何识别和解释单词成分或词素的?我们通过使用含有语义模糊的词根(例如,树皮,其含义与 "狗 "和 "树 "都有关)的词语来研究这个问题,这些词根在加上-ing(例如,吠叫;只与 "狗 "有关)后,其含义就变得不明确了。我们的目的是要了解对词根 "bark "意义的高层访问是否会受到低层形态词缀的限制。在实验 1 中,我们使用眼动追踪技术,让被试阅读含有语义模糊词根的句子,如 "吠叫"(质点),同时阅读与词根的两种含义(狗、树)相关的目标句,或者阅读与词根无关的对照句。我们采用的所有五种眼动跟踪测量方法(首次定格持续时间、注视持续时间、经过时间、总阅读时间和向目标回归时间)都显示,与词根相关的两个目标之间没有差异,它们比苜蓿语慢,但比无关目标快。结果表明,即使在词义与完整词形不一致的情况下(barking-tree),模糊词根的两个词义都会被激活。实验 2 采用了一项迷宫任务来支持这些结果,在该任务中,由于出现了与树皮的两种含义相关的干扰物,因此选择 "他在......时听到了响亮的狗叫声 "这一句子的 "掐头去尾(夜晚)"续句的时间被打乱了。我们讨论了这些发现对句子语境中形态解析和词汇歧义解决的本质的影响。我们认为,单词识别和词汇访问过程涉及词根和词缀的分离,从而产生独立和详尽的词根意义访问--即使词缀和上下文排除了词根意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task.","authors":"Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001378","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001378","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., <i>bark</i>, with meanings related to both \"dog\" and \"tree\") which are disambiguated when affixed by -<i>ing</i> (e.g., <i>barking</i>; related to \"dog\" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root <i>bark</i> would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as <i>barking</i> (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (<i>dog</i>, <i>tree</i>) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form <i>(barking-tree</i>), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (<i>night</i>) continuation for the sentence <i>He heard loud barking during the</i> … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1