首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Mental effort during mindless reading? Pupil fluctuations indicate internal processing during levels of inattention. 无意识阅读时的脑力劳动?瞳孔波动表明注意力不集中时的内部处理过程。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001384
Daniel J Schad,Antje Nuthmann,Frank Rösler,Ralf Engbert
Mind wandering, an experience characterized by a reduced external focus of attention and an increased internal focus, has seen significant theoretical advancement in understanding its underlying cognitive processes. The levels-of-inattention hypothesis posits that in mind wandering, external attention is reduced in a graded fashion, reflecting different levels of weak versus deep attentional decoupling. However, it has remained unclear whether internal processing during mind wandering, and mindless reading in particular, requires effort and, if so, whether it is graded or distinct. To address this, we analyzed pupil size as a measure of cognitive load in the sustained-attention-to-stimulus task during text reading. We examined whether decoupled external attention is linked to an overall reduction in workload and whether internal focus of attention is graded or represents a distinct cognitive process. Overall, overlooking errors in the text was associated with a small pupil size, indicating reduced effortful processing. However, this effect varied with error type: overlooking high- or medium-level errors (weak decoupling) resulted in reduced pupil size, while overlooking low-level errors (deep decoupling) had no effect on pupil size. Moreover, detecting an error (at any processing level) elicited a task-evoked pupillary response, which was absent when it was overlooked. These findings suggest that weak decoupling reduces internal resource-demanding processing and are in line with the hypothesis that large pupils during deep decoupling may be associated with distinct states of effortful internal processing. They further support both the levels-of-inattention hypothesis and the notion that internal focus is a distinct mode of deeply decoupled processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
心智游离是一种以外部注意力降低和内部注意力增加为特征的体验,在理解其基本认知过程方面取得了重大的理论进展。注意水平假说认为,在心智游离中,外部注意以分级的方式减少,反映了不同程度的弱注意与深注意解耦。然而,目前还不清楚在思维游离,特别是无意识阅读时,内部处理是否需要努力,如果需要,它是分级的还是独特的。为了解决这个问题,我们分析了瞳孔大小,以此来衡量文本阅读过程中持续注意刺激任务的认知负荷。我们研究了外部注意力的分离是否与整体工作量的减少有关,以及内部注意力的集中是分级的还是代表一个独特的认知过程。总体而言,忽略文本中的错误与瞳孔缩小有关,这表明费力的处理过程减少了。然而,这种效应随错误类型的不同而变化:忽略高、中级错误(弱解耦)会导致瞳孔缩小,而忽略低级错误(深解耦)则对瞳孔大小没有影响。此外,发现错误(在任何处理水平上)都会引起任务诱发的瞳孔反应,而忽略错误时则没有这种反应。这些研究结果表明,弱解耦减少了内部资源需求处理,并与深度解耦时的大瞳孔可能与不同的费力内部处理状态有关的假设相一致。它们进一步支持了注意力水平假说和内部专注是深度解耦处理的一种独特模式的观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mental effort during mindless reading? Pupil fluctuations indicate internal processing during levels of inattention.","authors":"Daniel J Schad,Antje Nuthmann,Frank Rösler,Ralf Engbert","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001384","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001384","url":null,"abstract":"Mind wandering, an experience characterized by a reduced external focus of attention and an increased internal focus, has seen significant theoretical advancement in understanding its underlying cognitive processes. The levels-of-inattention hypothesis posits that in mind wandering, external attention is reduced in a graded fashion, reflecting different levels of weak versus deep attentional decoupling. However, it has remained unclear whether internal processing during mind wandering, and mindless reading in particular, requires effort and, if so, whether it is graded or distinct. To address this, we analyzed pupil size as a measure of cognitive load in the sustained-attention-to-stimulus task during text reading. We examined whether decoupled external attention is linked to an overall reduction in workload and whether internal focus of attention is graded or represents a distinct cognitive process. Overall, overlooking errors in the text was associated with a small pupil size, indicating reduced effortful processing. However, this effect varied with error type: overlooking high- or medium-level errors (weak decoupling) resulted in reduced pupil size, while overlooking low-level errors (deep decoupling) had no effect on pupil size. Moreover, detecting an error (at any processing level) elicited a task-evoked pupillary response, which was absent when it was overlooked. These findings suggest that weak decoupling reduces internal resource-demanding processing and are in line with the hypothesis that large pupils during deep decoupling may be associated with distinct states of effortful internal processing. They further support both the levels-of-inattention hypothesis and the notion that internal focus is a distinct mode of deeply decoupled processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mouse cursor trajectories capture the flexible adaptivity of predictive sentence processing. 鼠标光标轨迹捕捉了预测句子处理的灵活适应性。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001397
Anuenue Kukona,Nabil Hasshim
Recent psycholinguistic findings raise fundamental questions about comprehenders' ability to rationally adapt their predictions during sentence processing. Two mouse cursor tracking experiments (each N = 85) assessed this adaptivity by manipulating the reliability of verb-based semantic cues. In Experiment 1, predictive mouse cursor movements to targets (e.g., bike) versus distractors (e.g., kite) were measured while participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive (e.g., "spot … the bike"), predictive (e.g., "ride … the bike"), and antipredictive (e.g., "fly … the bike") sentences. In Experiment 2, participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive and antipredictive sentences. Participants were observed to flexibly adapt their predictions, such that they disengaged prediction in Experiment 1 when verb-based cues were unreliable and as likely to be disconfirmed as confirmed, while they generated adapted predictions in Experiment 2 when verb-based cues were reliably disconfirmed. However, links to individual differences in cognitive control were not observed. These results are interpreted as supporting rational theoretical approaches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
最近的心理语言学研究结果提出了一些基本问题,即理解者在句子处理过程中合理调整其预测的能力。两个鼠标光标跟踪实验(每个实验的人数均为 85 人)通过操纵基于动词的语义线索的可靠性来评估这种适应能力。在实验 1 中,当参与者听到同等比例的非预测性句子(如 "发现......自行车")、预测性句子(如 "骑......自行车")和反预测性句子(如 "飞......自行车")时,对目标(如自行车)和干扰项(如风筝)的预测性鼠标光标移动进行了测量。在实验 2 中,参与者听到的非预测性句子和反预测性句子的比例相同。据观察,在实验 1 中,当基于动词的线索不可靠,被否定和被肯定的可能性一样大时,参与者会灵活地调整他们的预测;而在实验 2 中,当基于动词的线索被可靠地否定时,参与者会产生调整后的预测。然而,并没有观察到认知控制方面的个体差异。这些结果被解释为支持合理的理论方法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mouse cursor trajectories capture the flexible adaptivity of predictive sentence processing.","authors":"Anuenue Kukona,Nabil Hasshim","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001397","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001397","url":null,"abstract":"Recent psycholinguistic findings raise fundamental questions about comprehenders' ability to rationally adapt their predictions during sentence processing. Two mouse cursor tracking experiments (each N = 85) assessed this adaptivity by manipulating the reliability of verb-based semantic cues. In Experiment 1, predictive mouse cursor movements to targets (e.g., bike) versus distractors (e.g., kite) were measured while participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive (e.g., \"spot … the bike\"), predictive (e.g., \"ride … the bike\"), and antipredictive (e.g., \"fly … the bike\") sentences. In Experiment 2, participants heard equal proportions of nonpredictive and antipredictive sentences. Participants were observed to flexibly adapt their predictions, such that they disengaged prediction in Experiment 1 when verb-based cues were unreliable and as likely to be disconfirmed as confirmed, while they generated adapted predictions in Experiment 2 when verb-based cues were reliably disconfirmed. However, links to individual differences in cognitive control were not observed. These results are interpreted as supporting rational theoretical approaches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142205624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task. 词缀不能阻止对模糊词根的语义访问--即使在句子语境中也不能:来自眼动跟踪和迷宫任务的证据。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001378
Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben

How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., bark, with meanings related to both "dog" and "tree") which are disambiguated when affixed by -ing (e.g., barking; related to "dog" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root bark would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as barking (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (dog, tree) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form (barking-tree), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (night) continuation for the sentence He heard loud barking during the … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在句子阅读过程中,语言理解系统是如何识别和解释单词成分或词素的?我们通过使用含有语义模糊的词根(例如,树皮,其含义与 "狗 "和 "树 "都有关)的词语来研究这个问题,这些词根在加上-ing(例如,吠叫;只与 "狗 "有关)后,其含义就变得不明确了。我们的目的是要了解对词根 "bark "意义的高层访问是否会受到低层形态词缀的限制。在实验 1 中,我们使用眼动追踪技术,让被试阅读含有语义模糊词根的句子,如 "吠叫"(质点),同时阅读与词根的两种含义(狗、树)相关的目标句,或者阅读与词根无关的对照句。我们采用的所有五种眼动跟踪测量方法(首次定格持续时间、注视持续时间、经过时间、总阅读时间和向目标回归时间)都显示,与词根相关的两个目标之间没有差异,它们比苜蓿语慢,但比无关目标快。结果表明,即使在词义与完整词形不一致的情况下(barking-tree),模糊词根的两个词义都会被激活。实验 2 采用了一项迷宫任务来支持这些结果,在该任务中,由于出现了与树皮的两种含义相关的干扰物,因此选择 "他在......时听到了响亮的狗叫声 "这一句子的 "掐头去尾(夜晚)"续句的时间被打乱了。我们讨论了这些发现对句子语境中形态解析和词汇歧义解决的本质的影响。我们认为,单词识别和词汇访问过程涉及词根和词缀的分离,从而产生独立和详尽的词根意义访问--即使词缀和上下文排除了词根意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Semantic access to ambiguous word roots cannot be stopped by affixation-Not even in sentence contexts: Evidence from eye-tracking and the maze task.","authors":"Roberto G de Almeida, Jordan Gallant, Caitlyn Antal, Gary Libben","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001378","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001378","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does the language comprehension system identify and interpret word constituents-or morphemes-during sentence reading? We investigated this question by employing words containing semantically ambiguous roots (e.g., <i>bark</i>, with meanings related to both \"dog\" and \"tree\") which are disambiguated when affixed by -<i>ing</i> (e.g., <i>barking</i>; related to \"dog\" only). We aimed to understand whether higher-level access to the meaning of the root <i>bark</i> would be constrained by lower-level morphological affixation. In Experiment 1, using eye-tracking, participants read sentences containing words with semantically ambiguous roots, such as <i>barking</i> (a prime), combined with targets that were either related to two meanings of the root (<i>dog</i>, <i>tree</i>) or they were cloze and unrelated controls. All five eye-tracking measures we employed (first fixation duration, gaze duration, go-past time, total reading time, and regressions to target) showed no difference between the two root-related targets, which were slower than cloze, but faster than unrelated. Results show that even in cases where a meaning is inconsistent with the full word form <i>(barking-tree</i>), both meanings of the ambiguous root are activated. These results were supported by Experiment 2, employing a maze task in which the time to select the cloze (<i>night</i>) continuation for the sentence <i>He heard loud barking during the</i> … was disrupted by the presence of distractors related to both meanings of bark. We discuss the implications of these findings for the nature of morphological parsing and lexical ambiguity resolution in sentence contexts. We suggest that word recognition and lexical access processes involve separating roots from affixes, yielding independent and exhaustive access to root meanings-even when they are ruled out by affixation and context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms. 人脸竞赛对元记忆的影响:研究其稳健性和内在机制
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001392
Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata

Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

知觉者对同种族面孔的识别记忆通常优于对跨种族面孔的识别记忆,这种现象被称为跨种族效应(CRE)。尽管跨种族效应无处不在,但人们是否在元认知上意识到跨种族效应尚不清楚。本研究通过五项实验深入调查了感知者对 CRE 的元认知意识。实验 1 和 2 表明,前瞻性(学习判断)和回顾性(信心)元认知判断对人脸的种族类别和原型的变化都很敏感。实验 3 表明,受试者的项目级前瞻性判断受到了关于人脸种族对记忆表现影响的信念的影响。实验 4 显示,在没有直接刺激经验的情况下,全局预测会受到人脸种族的影响,这强调了预先存在的信念的作用。实验 5 扩展了这些发现,在全局预测和项目层面的前瞻性判断中,人脸种族和参与者种族之间都有很大的交叉互动,表明白人和黑人参与者对内群体人脸都有较高的元记忆估计。该实验进一步表明,预先存在的信念会加强人脸种族对元记忆判断的影响,但并不能完全解释这种影响。总之,这些实验提供了有力的证据,证明白人参与者对不同种族类别和原型性的面孔具有良好的元记忆准确性,并证明信念是面孔种族对白人和黑人参与者元记忆判断影响的基础,尽管这可能不是唯一的机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms.","authors":"Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001392","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001392","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142114321","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revealing mental representations of arithmetic word problems through false memories: New insights into semantic congruence. 通过虚假记忆揭示算术文字问题的心理表征:语义一致性的新见解。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001373
Hippolyte Gros, Jean-Pierre Thibaut, Lucas Raynal, Emmanuel Sander

What can false memories tell us about the structure of mental representations of arithmetic word problems? The semantic congruence model describes the central role of world semantics in the encoding, recoding, and solving of these problems. We propose to use memory tasks to evaluate key predictions of the semantic congruence model regarding the representations constructed when solving arithmetic word problems. We designed isomorphic word problems differing only by the world semantics imbued in their problem statement. Half the problems featured quantities (durations, heights, elevator floors) promoting an ordinal encoding, and the other half used quantities (weights, prices, collections) promoting a cardinal encoding. Across three experiments, in French and in English, we used surprise memory tasks to investigate adults' mental representations when solving the problems. After the first solving task, the participants were given an unexpected task: either to recall the problems (Experiments 1 and 2) or to identify, from memory, the experimenter-induced changes in target problem sentences (Experiment 3). Crucially, all problems included a specific mathematical relationship that was not explicit in the problem statement and that could only be inferred from an ordinal encoding. We used the presence or absence of this relationship in the participants' responses to infer the structure of their representations. Converging results from all three experiments bring new evidence of the role of semantic congruence in arithmetic reasoning, new insights into the relevance of the cardinal-ordinal distinction in numerical cognition, and a new perspective on the use of memory tasks to investigate variations in the representations of mathematical word problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于算术文字题的心理表征结构,错误记忆能告诉我们什么?语义一致性模型描述了世界语义在这些问题的编码、重新编码和求解中的核心作用。我们建议使用记忆任务来评估语义一致性模型关于解决算术文字问题时所构建表征的关键预测。我们设计了一些同构的单词问题,其区别仅在于问题陈述中包含的世界语义。一半的问题以数量(持续时间、高度、电梯楼层)为特征,提倡序数编码;另一半问题使用数量(重量、价格、集合),提倡心数编码。在用法语和英语进行的三项实验中,我们使用了突击记忆任务来研究成人在解题时的心理表征。在完成第一个解题任务后,参与者会接到一个意外任务:要么回忆问题(实验 1 和 2),要么从记忆中识别实验者引起的目标问题句子的变化(实验 3)。最重要的是,所有问题都包含一种特定的数学关系,这种关系在问题陈述中并不明确,只能通过序数编码来推断。我们利用参与者回答中是否存在这种关系来推断他们的表征结构。这三个实验的结果相辅相成,为语义一致性在算术推理中的作用提供了新的证据,为数字认知中的心数-序数区分的相关性提供了新的见解,也为使用记忆任务研究数学文字问题表征的变化提供了新的视角。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Revealing mental representations of arithmetic word problems through false memories: New insights into semantic congruence.","authors":"Hippolyte Gros, Jean-Pierre Thibaut, Lucas Raynal, Emmanuel Sander","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001373","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001373","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What can false memories tell us about the structure of mental representations of arithmetic word problems? The semantic congruence model describes the central role of world semantics in the encoding, recoding, and solving of these problems. We propose to use memory tasks to evaluate key predictions of the semantic congruence model regarding the representations constructed when solving arithmetic word problems. We designed isomorphic word problems differing only by the world semantics imbued in their problem statement. Half the problems featured quantities (durations, heights, elevator floors) promoting an ordinal encoding, and the other half used quantities (weights, prices, collections) promoting a cardinal encoding. Across three experiments, in French and in English, we used surprise memory tasks to investigate adults' mental representations when solving the problems. After the first solving task, the participants were given an unexpected task: either to recall the problems (Experiments 1 and 2) or to identify, from memory, the experimenter-induced changes in target problem sentences (Experiment 3). Crucially, all problems included a specific mathematical relationship that was not explicit in the problem statement and that could only be inferred from an ordinal encoding. We used the presence or absence of this relationship in the participants' responses to infer the structure of their representations. Converging results from all three experiments bring new evidence of the role of semantic congruence in arithmetic reasoning, new insights into the relevance of the cardinal-ordinal distinction in numerical cognition, and a new perspective on the use of memory tasks to investigate variations in the representations of mathematical word problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142114320","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Control of memory retrieval alters memory-based eye movements. 控制记忆检索会改变基于记忆的眼球运动。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001321
Mrinmayi Kulkarni, Allison E Nickel, Greta N Minor, Deborah E Hannula

Past work has shown that eye movements are affected by long-term memory across different tasks and instructional manipulations. In the current study, we tested whether these memory-based eye movements persist when memory retrieval is under intentional control. Participants encoded multiple scenes with six objects (three faces; three tools). Next, they completed a memory regulation and visual search task, while undergoing eye tracking. Here, scene cues were presented and participants either retrieved the encoded associate, suppressed it, or substituted it with a specific object from the other encoded category. Following a delay, a search display consisting of six dots intermixed with the six encoded objects was presented. Participants' task was to fixate one remaining dot after five had disappeared. Incidental viewing of the objects was of interest. Results revealed that performance in a final recognition phase was impaired for suppressed pairs, but only when the associate was a tool. During the search task, incidental associate viewing was lower when participants attempted to control retrieval, whereas one object from the nonassociate category was most viewed in the substitute condition. Additionally, viewing patterns in the search phase were related to final recognition performance, but the direction of this association differed between conditions. Overall, these results suggest that eye movements are attracted to information retrieved from long-term memory and held active (the associate in the retrieve condition, or an object from the other category in the substitute condition). Furthermore, the level of viewing may index the strength of the representation of retrieved information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

过去的研究表明,在不同的任务和教学操作中,眼动都会受到长期记忆的影响。在本研究中,我们测试了当记忆检索受到有意控制时,这些基于记忆的眼动是否会持续。参与者对包含六个物体(三个人脸;三个工具)的多个场景进行编码。接下来,他们完成了一项记忆调节和视觉搜索任务,同时接受眼动跟踪。在此过程中,会出现场景提示,参与者要么检索已编码的联想对象,要么抑制它,要么用另一个编码类别中的特定对象代替它。延时后,出现一个由六个点组成的搜索显示,其中夹杂着六个编码对象。参与者的任务是在五个点消失后固定住剩下的一个点。无意中看到的物体也会引起兴趣。结果显示,在最后的识别阶段,被抑制的成对对象的识别能力会受到影响,但只有当联想对象是工具时才会如此。在搜索任务中,当受试者试图控制检索时,偶然观看联想对象的次数较少,而在替代条件下,非联想类别中的一个对象被观看的次数最多。此外,搜索阶段的观察模式与最终识别成绩有关,但这种关联的方向在不同条件下有所不同。总之,这些结果表明,眼动会被从长期记忆中检索出来并保持活跃的信息所吸引(检索条件下的关联物,或替代条件下的其他类别的物体)。此外,观看的程度可能会反映出检索信息的表征强度。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Control of memory retrieval alters memory-based eye movements.","authors":"Mrinmayi Kulkarni, Allison E Nickel, Greta N Minor, Deborah E Hannula","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001321","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past work has shown that eye movements are affected by long-term memory across different tasks and instructional manipulations. In the current study, we tested whether these memory-based eye movements persist when memory retrieval is under intentional control. Participants encoded multiple scenes with six objects (three faces; three tools). Next, they completed a memory regulation and visual search task, while undergoing eye tracking. Here, scene cues were presented and participants either retrieved the encoded associate, suppressed it, or substituted it with a specific object from the other encoded category. Following a delay, a search display consisting of six dots intermixed with the six encoded objects was presented. Participants' task was to fixate one remaining dot after five had disappeared. Incidental viewing of the objects was of interest. Results revealed that performance in a final recognition phase was impaired for suppressed pairs, but only when the associate was a tool. During the search task, incidental associate viewing was lower when participants attempted to control retrieval, whereas one object from the nonassociate category was most viewed in the substitute condition. Additionally, viewing patterns in the search phase were related to final recognition performance, but the direction of this association differed between conditions. Overall, these results suggest that eye movements are attracted to information retrieved from long-term memory and held active (the associate in the retrieve condition, or an object from the other category in the substitute condition). Furthermore, the level of viewing may index the strength of the representation of retrieved information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual word and phrase frequency effects in collocational processing: Evidence from typologically different languages, English and Turkish. 搭配加工中的单词和短语频率效应:来自不同类型语言--英语和土耳其语--的证据。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-15 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001324
Doğuş Öksüz, Vaclav Brezina, Padraic Monaghan, Patrick Rebuschat

Collocations are understood to be integral building blocks of language processing, alongside individual words, but thus far evidence for the psychological reality of collocations has tended to be confined to English. In contrast to English, Turkish is an agglutinating language, utilizing productive morphology to convey complex meanings using a single word. Given this, we expected Turkish speakers to be less sensitive to phrasal frequencies than English speakers. In Study 1, we conducted a corpus analysis of translation-equivalent adjective-noun collocations (e.g., front door) and found differences between the two languages in frequency counts. In Study 2, we conducted a reaction time experiment to determine the sensitivity of native speakers of English and Turkish to the frequency of adjectives, nouns, and whole collocations. Turkish speakers were less sensitive to whole-phrase frequencies, as predicted, indicating that collocations are processed less holistically in Turkish than English. Both groups demonstrated that processing collocations involves combining information about individual words and phrases. Taken together, we show that speakers are sensitive to frequency information at multiple grain sizes that are attuned to the typology of different languages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

据了解,搭配与单个单词一样,都是语言处理过程中不可或缺的组成部分,但迄今为止,有关搭配的心理现实的证据往往仅限于英语。与英语不同,土耳其语是一种凝集语言,它利用生产性词形来表达单个词的复杂含义。有鉴于此,我们预计土耳其语使用者对短语频率的敏感度会低于英语使用者。在研究 1 中,我们对翻译等效的形容词-名词搭配(如:前门)进行了语料分析,发现两种语言在频率计数上存在差异。在研究 2 中,我们进行了反应时间实验,以确定英语和土耳其语母语者对形容词、名词和整个搭配频率的敏感度。正如预测的那样,土耳其语者对整个词组频率的敏感度较低,这表明土耳其语对搭配的整体处理不如英语。两组研究都表明,处理搭配时需要结合单个词和短语的信息。总之,我们的研究表明,说话者对多种粒度的频率信息都很敏感,这与不同语言的类型学相适应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Individual word and phrase frequency effects in collocational processing: Evidence from typologically different languages, English and Turkish.","authors":"Doğuş Öksüz, Vaclav Brezina, Padraic Monaghan, Patrick Rebuschat","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001324","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Collocations are understood to be integral building blocks of language processing, alongside individual words, but thus far evidence for the psychological reality of collocations has tended to be confined to English. In contrast to English, Turkish is an agglutinating language, utilizing productive morphology to convey complex meanings using a single word. Given this, we expected Turkish speakers to be less sensitive to phrasal frequencies than English speakers. In Study 1, we conducted a corpus analysis of translation-equivalent adjective-noun collocations (e.g., front door) and found differences between the two languages in frequency counts. In Study 2, we conducted a reaction time experiment to determine the sensitivity of native speakers of English and Turkish to the frequency of adjectives, nouns, and whole collocations. Turkish speakers were less sensitive to whole-phrase frequencies, as predicted, indicating that collocations are processed less holistically in Turkish than English. Both groups demonstrated that processing collocations involves combining information about individual words and phrases. Taken together, we show that speakers are sensitive to frequency information at multiple grain sizes that are attuned to the typology of different languages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139472941","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceiving infinity: An interplay between numerical and physical magnitude. 感知无限:数字幅度与物理幅度之间的相互作用。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001322
Michal Pinhas

Our mental representation of the infinite has received little research attention in cognitive psychology. In countably infinite sets, the infinity symbol (∞) is presumed to be perceived as larger than any finite natural number. The present study sought to explore if the infinity symbol is processed as "larger than" natural numbers, and, if so, whether it is associated with the special status of "the largest." In a series of four experiments (N = 40, 20, 20, and 40, respectively), participants performed numerical and physical comparisons of the infinity symbol against single- and multidigit numbers. Overall, numerical comparisons yielded slower responses for comparisons between infinity and a number than for comparisons between two numbers. Furthermore, distance-like effects were obtained for comparisons to infinity, suggesting the infinity symbol was treated as larger than all numbers presented. Importantly, however, physical comparisons revealed a normal size congruity effect for comparisons of infinity and single digits, but a reversed effect for comparisons of infinity and multidigit numbers, suggesting that the infinity symbol was automatically processed as smaller than multidigit numbers. These novel findings reveal limitations in abstracting the meanings of infinity from its symbol, indicating that the infinity symbol is not perceived as "the largest" and can be misconceived as a "number" mapped onto the numerical magnitude system. More generally, the results seem to reflect a crude, automatic evaluation of numerical magnitude based on the physical magnitude of the stimuli, namely, their overall length and the number of symbols of which they are comprised. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在认知心理学中,我们对无限的心理表征很少受到研究关注。在可数无限集合中,无穷符号 (∞)被假定为大于任何有限自然数。本研究试图探讨无穷符号是否被视为 "大于 "自然数,如果是,它是否与 "最大 "的特殊地位相关联。在四个系列实验(人数分别为 40、20、20 和 40)中,参与者将无穷大符号与个位数和多位数进行数字和物理比较。总体而言,在数字比较中,无穷大和一个数字之间的比较比两个数字之间的比较产生的反应要慢。此外,在与无穷大的比较中也出现了类似距离的效应,这表明无穷大符号被视为大于所有呈现的数字。然而,重要的是,物理比较显示,无穷大和个位数的比较具有正常的大小一致效应,但无穷大和多位数的比较却具有相反的效应,这表明无穷大符号被自动处理为小于多位数。这些新发现揭示了从无穷符号中抽象出无穷含义的局限性,表明无穷符号并不被视为 "最大",而可能被误认为是映射到数字大小系统中的一个 "数字"。更广泛地说,这些结果似乎反映了一种粗略的、基于刺激物物理大小(即刺激物的总长度和由其组成的符号数量)的数字大小自动评估。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceiving infinity: An interplay between numerical and physical magnitude.","authors":"Michal Pinhas","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001322","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our mental representation of the infinite has received little research attention in cognitive psychology. In countably infinite sets, the infinity symbol (∞) is presumed to be perceived as larger than any finite natural number. The present study sought to explore if the infinity symbol is processed as \"larger than\" natural numbers, and, if so, whether it is associated with the special status of \"the largest.\" In a series of four experiments (<i>N</i> = 40, 20, 20, and 40, respectively), participants performed numerical and physical comparisons of the infinity symbol against single- and multidigit numbers. Overall, numerical comparisons yielded slower responses for comparisons between infinity and a number than for comparisons between two numbers. Furthermore, distance-like effects were obtained for comparisons to infinity, suggesting the infinity symbol was treated as larger than all numbers presented. Importantly, however, physical comparisons revealed a normal size congruity effect for comparisons of infinity and single digits, but a reversed effect for comparisons of infinity and multidigit numbers, suggesting that the infinity symbol was automatically processed as smaller than multidigit numbers. These novel findings reveal limitations in abstracting the meanings of infinity from its symbol, indicating that the infinity symbol is not perceived as \"the largest\" and can be misconceived as a \"number\" mapped onto the numerical magnitude system. More generally, the results seem to reflect a crude, automatic evaluation of numerical magnitude based on the physical magnitude of the stimuli, namely, their overall length and the number of symbols of which they are comprised. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138812364","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of multisensory distraction on working memory: A role for task relevance? 多感官分心对工作记忆的影响:任务相关性的作用?
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001323
Nora Turoman, Evie Vergauwe

There is growing recognition that working memory and selective attention are highly related. However, a key function of selective attention-ignoring distractors-is much less understood in the domain of working memory. In the attention domain, it is now clear that distractors' task relevance and stimulation of multiple senses at a time (i.e., being multisensory), affect how much such information can distract from the main task, and that load modulates these effects. Here, we examined the effects of the task relevance and multisensory nature of distractors on working memory performance under high and low memory load, aiming to clarify whether distracting information similarly affects selective attention performance and working memory performance. We proposed a multiexperiment research plan involving up to three consecutive experiments, based on an initial online study (Experiment 0) with fully task-irrelevant distractors. There, we found conclusive evidence against a difference in how unisensory and multisensory distractors affected working memory performance. The next study, Experiment 1, replicated these results. However, when distractors were made partly task relevant in the subsequent Experiment 2d, multisensory distractors disrupted working memory performance more than unisensory distractors on average. However, closer nonpreregistered inspection revealed that multisensory distractors were actually only more disruptive than auditory distractors, and similarly as disruptive as visual distractors. Thus, overall, there was no strong evidence for multisensory distractors being more disruptive to working memory performance than unisensory distractors. Taken together, these experiments constitute a novel and detailed investigation of the impact of distracting information on working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

越来越多的人认识到,工作记忆和选择性注意高度相关。然而,在工作记忆领域,人们对选择性注意的一个关键功能--忽略分心物--的理解要少得多。在注意力领域,目前已经明确的是,分心物的任务相关性和对多种感官的刺激(即多感官刺激)会影响这些信息对主要任务的分心程度,而负荷会调节这些影响。在此,我们研究了在高和低记忆负荷下,任务相关性和分心信息的多感官性质对工作记忆能力的影响,旨在弄清分心信息是否同样会影响选择性注意能力和工作记忆能力。我们提出了一个多实验研究计划,包括多达三个连续实验,以完全与任务无关的分心物为基础的初始在线研究(实验 0)为基础。在该实验中,我们发现了确凿的证据,证明单感官和多感官分心物对工作记忆能力的影响存在差异。接下来的实验 1 重复了这些结果。然而,在随后的实验 2d 中,当分散注意力的因素部分与任务相关时,多感官分散注意力因素对工作记忆能力的干扰平均要大于单感官分散注意力因素。然而,仔细观察后发现,多感官干扰实际上只比听觉干扰更具干扰性,与视觉干扰的干扰性相似。因此,总体而言,没有强有力的证据表明多感官干扰比单感官干扰对工作记忆的干扰更大。总之,这些实验是对分心信息对工作记忆能力的影响进行的一次新颖而详细的调查。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of multisensory distraction on working memory: A role for task relevance?","authors":"Nora Turoman, Evie Vergauwe","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001323","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001323","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is growing recognition that working memory and selective attention are highly related. However, a key function of selective attention-ignoring distractors-is much less understood in the domain of working memory. In the attention domain, it is now clear that distractors' task relevance and stimulation of multiple senses at a time (i.e., being multisensory), affect how much such information can distract from the main task, and that load modulates these effects. Here, we examined the effects of the task relevance and multisensory nature of distractors on working memory performance under high and low memory load, aiming to clarify whether distracting information similarly affects selective attention performance and working memory performance. We proposed a multiexperiment research plan involving up to three consecutive experiments, based on an initial online study (Experiment 0) with fully task-irrelevant distractors. There, we found conclusive evidence against a difference in how unisensory and multisensory distractors affected working memory performance. The next study, Experiment 1, replicated these results. However, when distractors were made partly task relevant in the subsequent Experiment 2d, multisensory distractors disrupted working memory performance more than unisensory distractors on average. However, closer nonpreregistered inspection revealed that multisensory distractors were actually only more disruptive than auditory distractors, and similarly as disruptive as visual distractors. Thus, overall, there was no strong evidence for multisensory distractors being more disruptive to working memory performance than unisensory distractors. Taken together, these experiments constitute a novel and detailed investigation of the impact of distracting information on working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832723","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reading proficiency predicts spatial eye-movement control in the first and second language. 阅读能力可预测第一语言和第二语言的空间眼动控制。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001325
Daniil Gnetov, Victor Kuperman

Research on first language (L1) reading has long since established the link between the proficiency of the reader and their efficiency in oculomotor control. More proficient readers make longer saccades and land closer to the word's center, which is a word's optimal viewing position, and make fewer refixations. Eye-tracking studies of second language (L2) reading have so far provided little evidence in this regard. This study analyzes spatial oculomotor measures in the Multilingual Eye-movement Corpus, which contains data on English text reading and its component skills from 543 participants representing 12 different L1s. Our analyses establish a strong role of proficiency in English, both for L1 and L2 readers of English. While most effects replicated ones observed in L1 reading, we also found that more proficient readers of English were less accurate in targeting optimal viewing positions. We link this finding to Fitts' law of motor control for aimed movements. This article discusses the theoretical implications of the novel findings for reading research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有关第一语言(L1)阅读的研究早已证实,阅读者的熟练程度与其眼球运动控制的效率之间存在联系。阅读能力较强的读者的眼球移动时间较长,并且更靠近单词的中心位置,这是单词的最佳视线位置,同时也减少了混淆。对第二语言(L2)阅读的眼动跟踪研究迄今为止还没有提供这方面的证据。本研究分析了多语言眼动语料库(Multilingual Eye-movement Corpus)中的空间眼动测量数据,该语料库包含来自 543 名代表 12 种不同 L1 语言的参与者的英语文本阅读数据及其组成技能。我们的分析表明,英语熟练程度对第一语言和第二语言的英语读者都有很大作用。虽然大多数效应都与在 L1 阅读中观察到的效应相同,但我们还发现,英语水平较高的读者在瞄准最佳观看位置时准确性较低。我们将这一发现与菲茨的瞄准运动控制法则联系起来。本文讨论了这些新发现对阅读研究的理论意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reading proficiency predicts spatial eye-movement control in the first and second language.","authors":"Daniil Gnetov, Victor Kuperman","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001325","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on first language (L1) reading has long since established the link between the proficiency of the reader and their efficiency in oculomotor control. More proficient readers make longer saccades and land closer to the word's center, which is a word's optimal viewing position, and make fewer refixations. Eye-tracking studies of second language (L2) reading have so far provided little evidence in this regard. This study analyzes spatial oculomotor measures in the Multilingual Eye-movement Corpus, which contains data on English text reading and its component skills from 543 participants representing 12 different L1s. Our analyses establish a strong role of proficiency in English, both for L1 and L2 readers of English. While most effects replicated ones observed in L1 reading, we also found that more proficient readers of English were less accurate in targeting optimal viewing positions. We link this finding to Fitts' law of motor control for aimed movements. This article discusses the theoretical implications of the novel findings for reading research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708338","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1