首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
The benefits of memory control processes in working memory: Comparing effects of self-reported and instructed strategy use. 工作记忆中记忆控制过程的益处:比较自我报告和指导策略使用的效果。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001370
Lea M Bartsch, Alessandra S Souza, Klaus Oberauer

Working memory performance is often assumed to benefit from different maintenance control strategies such as rehearsal, refreshing, elaboration, and grouping. In studies assessing strategy self-reports, some strategies were indeed associated with better recall. Nevertheless, experimental studies assessing the effect of instructing maintenance strategies compared to a no-instruction baseline lend no evidence for the effectiveness of these strategies for working memory. Explanations for this contradiction could be that instruction implementation engenders dual-task costs or that strategy instructions reduce adaptive strategy switching. Across two experiments, we investigated the frequency and variability of strategy use with trial-wise self-reports in serial recall of word lists. Furthermore, we examined potential instruction costs by comparing performance in trials with self-reported versus instructed use of the same strategies. Self-reported strategy use varied from trial to trial, with elaboration and rehearsal being the most frequent. Self-reported elaboration was correlated with better performance than reading and rehearsal. For the most prevalent strategies-elaboration and rehearsal-there were no costs of instructed strategy implementation. Our results speak against dual-task costs and for the advantage of adaptively choosing one's own strategy from trial to trial. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们通常认为,工作记忆的表现得益于不同的保持控制策略,如复习、刷新、详述和分组。在评估策略自我报告的研究中,有些策略确实与更好的记忆有关。然而,与无指导基线相比,评估指导维持策略效果的实验研究并未证明这些策略对工作记忆的有效性。造成这种矛盾的原因可能是指令的实施产生了双重任务成本,或者是策略指令减少了适应性策略转换。在两个实验中,我们通过在单词表的序列回忆中进行试验性自我报告,调查了策略使用的频率和可变性。此外,我们还通过比较自我报告与指导使用相同策略的试验成绩,考察了潜在的指导成本。自我报告的策略使用在不同的试验中各不相同,其中阐述和预演最为常见。与阅读和排练相比,自我报告的详细阐述与更好的成绩相关。对于最常用的策略--阐述和排练--来说,指导策略的实施没有成本。我们的研究结果表明了双重任务成本的存在,并证明了在不同的试验中适应性地选择自己的策略的优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The benefits of memory control processes in working memory: Comparing effects of self-reported and instructed strategy use.","authors":"Lea M Bartsch, Alessandra S Souza, Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001370","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory performance is often assumed to benefit from different maintenance control strategies such as rehearsal, refreshing, elaboration, and grouping. In studies assessing strategy self-reports, some strategies were indeed associated with better recall. Nevertheless, experimental studies assessing the effect of instructing maintenance strategies compared to a no-instruction baseline lend no evidence for the effectiveness of these strategies for working memory. Explanations for this contradiction could be that instruction implementation engenders dual-task costs or that strategy instructions reduce adaptive strategy switching. Across two experiments, we investigated the frequency and variability of strategy use with trial-wise self-reports in serial recall of word lists. Furthermore, we examined potential instruction costs by comparing performance in trials with self-reported versus instructed use of the same strategies. Self-reported strategy use varied from trial to trial, with elaboration and rehearsal being the most frequent. Self-reported elaboration was correlated with better performance than reading and rehearsal. For the most prevalent strategies-elaboration and rehearsal-there were no costs of instructed strategy implementation. Our results speak against dual-task costs and for the advantage of adaptively choosing one's own strategy from trial to trial. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141460492","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of working memory capacity in the temporal compression of episodic memories: An individual differences approach. 工作记忆容量在外显记忆的时间压缩中的作用:个体差异法
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001350
Nathan Leroy, Steve Majerus, Arnaud D'Argembeau

Remembering past events usually takes less time than their actual duration-their unfolding is temporally compressed in episodic memory. The rate of temporal compression (i.e., the ratio of the actual duration of an event to the duration of its remembering) is not constant but varies between individuals and as a function of the structure of events (e.g., how they can be divided into shorter subevents). However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying these variations remain poorly understood. Given its role in the encoding and retrieval of information in episodic memory, working memory (WM) capacity could be an important determinant of temporal compression rates. We tested this hypothesis in two experiments in which we asked participants to watch and then mentally replay short videos showing people engaged in daily life activities. We showed that temporal compression rates depend on an interplay between WM and the structure of the remembered events: participants' WM capacity (assessed using complex span tasks) was negatively associated with temporal compression rates, but only when the remembered events contained few event boundaries (i.e., few subevents). This suggests that the temporal compression of events in episodic memory emerges when some of the subevents to be retained are too long to be fully represented in WM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆过去事件所需的时间通常少于事件的实际持续时间--事件的展开在历时记忆中被时间压缩了。时间压缩率(即事件的实际持续时间与记忆持续时间之比)并非恒定不变,而是因人而异,并与事件的结构(如如何将事件划分为更短的子事件)有关。然而,人们对这些变化背后的认知机制仍然知之甚少。鉴于工作记忆(WM)能力在外显记忆信息的编码和检索中的作用,它可能是时间压缩率的一个重要决定因素。我们在两个实验中测试了这一假设。在这两个实验中,我们要求参与者观看并在头脑中回放展示人们从事日常生活活动的短片。实验结果表明,时间压缩率取决于 WM 与记忆事件结构之间的相互作用:参与者的 WM 能力(使用复杂跨度任务进行评估)与时间压缩率呈负相关,但只有当记忆事件包含很少的事件边界(即很少的子事件)时才会出现这种情况。这表明,当需要保留的一些子事件太长而无法在 WM 中完全体现时,就会出现外显记忆中事件的时间压缩。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of working memory capacity in the temporal compression of episodic memories: An individual differences approach.","authors":"Nathan Leroy, Steve Majerus, Arnaud D'Argembeau","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001350","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Remembering past events usually takes less time than their actual duration-their unfolding is temporally compressed in episodic memory. The rate of temporal compression (i.e., the ratio of the actual duration of an event to the duration of its remembering) is not constant but varies between individuals and as a function of the structure of events (e.g., how they can be divided into shorter subevents). However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying these variations remain poorly understood. Given its role in the encoding and retrieval of information in episodic memory, working memory (WM) capacity could be an important determinant of temporal compression rates. We tested this hypothesis in two experiments in which we asked participants to watch and then mentally replay short videos showing people engaged in daily life activities. We showed that temporal compression rates depend on an interplay between WM and the structure of the remembered events: participants' WM capacity (assessed using complex span tasks) was negatively associated with temporal compression rates, but only when the remembered events contained few event boundaries (i.e., few subevents). This suggests that the temporal compression of events in episodic memory emerges when some of the subevents to be retained are too long to be fully represented in WM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phonological networks and systematicity in early lexical acquisition. 早期词汇习得中的语音网络和系统性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001368
Catherine E Laing

Infants' early words tend to be phonologically similar. This may reflect a systematic approach to early production, as they adapt newly acquired forms to fit familiar structures in the output. This "rich-get-richer" approach to phonological acquisition, known as preferential attachment in network science, proposes that new words cluster together with existing phonologically similar words in the lexicon (or network). This contrasts with recent work (e.g., Fourtassi et al., 2020) showing that the learning environment is the key predictor of learning (preferential acquisition). This study expands on previous analyses of vocabulary norm data to analyze naturalistic data, namely phonetic transcriptions of nine infants' word productions, from word onset to age 2;6. Network growth models test whether (a) acquisition is best modeled through preferential attachment or preferential acquisition, (b) the trajectory of network growth changes over time, and (c) there are any differences in network growth of adult target forms versus infants' actual productions. Results show that preferential attachment predicts acquisition of new words more convincingly than preferential acquisition: newly acquired words are phonologically similar to existing words in the network. Furthermore, systematicity becomes increasingly apparent over the course of acquisition, and infants produce their early words more systematically than we would expect from looking at target forms alone. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

婴儿的早期词汇往往在语音上相似。这可能反映了早期生产的系统性,因为他们会调整新获得的形式以适应输出中熟悉的结构。这种 "丰富者更丰富 "的语音习得方法在网络科学中被称为 "优先附着"(preferential attachment),它认为新词会与词库(或网络)中现有的语音相似的词聚集在一起。这与最近的研究(如 Fourtassi 等人,2020 年)显示学习环境是预测学习(优先习得)的关键因素形成了鲜明对比。本研究在以往词汇规范数据分析的基础上,进一步分析了自然数据,即九名婴儿从开始学词到两岁六岁学词的语音转录。网络增长模型检验了:(a) 通过优先依附或优先习得是否是最佳习得模型;(b) 网络增长的轨迹是否随时间而变化;(c) 成人目标形式的网络增长与婴儿实际制作的网络增长是否存在差异。结果表明,优先依附比优先习得更能令人信服地预测新词的习得:新习得的词在语音上与网络中现有的词相似。此外,在习得过程中,系统性会变得越来越明显,婴儿早期造词的系统性要比我们单看目标词形所预期的更强。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Phonological networks and systematicity in early lexical acquisition.","authors":"Catherine E Laing","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001368","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001368","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Infants' early words tend to be phonologically similar. This may reflect a systematic approach to early production, as they adapt newly acquired forms to fit familiar structures in the output. This \"rich-get-richer\" approach to phonological acquisition, known as preferential attachment in network science, proposes that new words cluster together with existing phonologically similar words in the lexicon (or network). This contrasts with recent work (e.g., Fourtassi et al., 2020) showing that the learning environment is the key predictor of learning (preferential acquisition). This study expands on previous analyses of vocabulary norm data to analyze naturalistic data, namely phonetic transcriptions of nine infants' word productions, from word onset to age 2;6. Network growth models test whether (a) acquisition is best modeled through preferential attachment or preferential acquisition, (b) the trajectory of network growth changes over time, and (c) there are any differences in network growth of adult target forms versus infants' actual productions. Results show that preferential attachment predicts acquisition of new words more convincingly than preferential acquisition: newly acquired words are phonologically similar to existing words in the network. Furthermore, systematicity becomes increasingly apparent over the course of acquisition, and infants produce their early words more systematically than we would expect from looking at target forms alone. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447462","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of emotional valence of mind wandering on sustained attention performance. 思绪游荡的情绪情感对持续注意力表现的影响
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001369
Matthew S Welhaf, Jonathan B Banks

The construct of mind wandering has notoriously been characterized as heterogenous which may mean that not all types of mind wandering produce the same pattern of results. One operationalization of mind wandering, task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs), can also itself vary in many dimensions, including the emotional valence of TUTs. The current study summarizes several years of work examining the impact that the emotional valence of TUTs has on different aspects of sustained attention. Participants in several studies reported whether their TUTs were negative, neutral, or positive in emotional valence during a sustained attention-to-response task (SART). The first major focus was a meta-analysis where we examined correlations between each TUT valence and SART performance measures. For the second major focus, we tested how different TUT valences changed over the course of the task. The results suggest that negative TUTs typically show stronger associations with SART performance measures, although all TUT valences have numerically similar correlations. Regarding time-on-task effects, across the studies, there was consistent evidence for a linear increase in negative TUTs across blocks. Evidence for this linear increase was not consistent for neutral and positive TUTs. The results of the current study suggest that the relationships between TUTs and performance, and their likelihood of occurring during a task, are not necessarily the same for every type of TUT. These results highlight the importance of continuing to investigate different types of TUTs and different forms of mind wandering, in general, to better understand how this phenomenon occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

众所周知,思绪游走的结构具有异质性,这可能意味着并非所有类型的思绪游走都会产生相同的结果。思维游移的一种操作方式,即与任务无关的想法(TUTs),其本身也会在许多方面发生变化,其中包括 TUTs 的情绪情感。本研究总结了数年来研究 TUTs 的情绪价值对持续注意力不同方面的影响的工作。在几项研究中,受试者报告了在持续注意-反应任务(SART)中,TUTs 的情绪价位是消极的、中性的还是积极的。第一个重点是荟萃分析,我们研究了每种 TUT 情调与 SART 成绩测量之间的相关性。第二个重点是,我们测试了不同的 TUT 情调在任务过程中是如何变化的。结果表明,负性 TUT 通常与 SART 性能指标有更强的相关性,尽管所有 TUT 价位在数量上具有相似的相关性。关于任务时间效应,在所有的研究中,有一致的证据表明负性 TUTs 在各个区块中呈线性增长。中性和阳性 TUT 线性增加的证据并不一致。本研究的结果表明,TUT 与成绩之间的关系以及在任务中发生的可能性并不一定对每种类型的 TUT 都相同。这些结果凸显了继续研究不同类型的TUT和不同形式的思维游离的重要性,以便更好地了解这种现象是如何发生的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Effects of emotional valence of mind wandering on sustained attention performance.","authors":"Matthew S Welhaf, Jonathan B Banks","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001369","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001369","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The construct of mind wandering has notoriously been characterized as heterogenous which may mean that not all types of mind wandering produce the same pattern of results. One operationalization of mind wandering, task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs), can also itself vary in many dimensions, including the emotional valence of TUTs. The current study summarizes several years of work examining the impact that the emotional valence of TUTs has on different aspects of sustained attention. Participants in several studies reported whether their TUTs were negative, neutral, or positive in emotional valence during a sustained attention-to-response task (SART). The first major focus was a meta-analysis where we examined correlations between each TUT valence and SART performance measures. For the second major focus, we tested how different TUT valences changed over the course of the task. The results suggest that negative TUTs typically show stronger associations with SART performance measures, although all TUT valences have numerically similar correlations. Regarding time-on-task effects, across the studies, there was consistent evidence for a linear increase in negative TUTs across blocks. Evidence for this linear increase was not consistent for neutral and positive TUTs. The results of the current study suggest that the relationships between TUTs and performance, and their likelihood of occurring during a task, are not necessarily the same for every type of TUT. These results highlight the importance of continuing to investigate different types of TUTs and different forms of mind wandering, in general, to better understand how this phenomenon occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11666809/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Probing the role of multilingualism and working memory in cross-situational word learning. 探究多语言和工作记忆在跨情景单词学习中的作用。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001361
Ye Li, Viridiana L Benitez

Cross-situational word learning (CSWL), the ability to resolve word-referent ambiguity across encounters, is a powerful mechanism found in infants, children, and adults. Yet, we know little about what predicts individual differences in CSWL, especially when learning different mapping structures, such as when referents have a single name (1:1 mapping structure) or two names (2:1 mapping structure). Here, we investigated how multilingual experience and working memory skills (visuo-spatial and phonological) contributed to CSWL of 1:1 and 2:1 structures. Monolingual (n = 78) and multilingual (n = 106) adults completed CSWL tasks of 1:1 and 2:1 structures, a symmetry span task, and a listening span task. Results from path models showed that multilingualism predicted visuo-spatial working memory but not CSWL. Additionally, phonological working memory predicted accuracy on CSWL of 1:1 structure, but not 2:1 structure. Findings highlight the importance of considering language experience and cognitive skills together to better understand the factors that promote individual CSWL skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

跨情境单词学习(Cross-situations word learning,CSWL)是一种在不同情境中解决单词-参照物歧义的能力,它是一种在婴儿、儿童和成人身上都能发现的强大机制。然而,我们对预测 CSWL 个体差异的因素知之甚少,尤其是在学习不同的映射结构时,例如当参照物只有一个名字(1:1 映射结构)或两个名字(2:1 映射结构)时。在此,我们研究了多语言经验和工作记忆技能(视觉空间和语音)如何促进 1:1 和 2:1 结构的 CSWL。单语(78人)和多语(106人)成人完成了1:1和2:1结构的CSWL任务、对称跨度任务和听力跨度任务。路径模型的结果表明,多语言可以预测视觉空间工作记忆,但不能预测CSWL。此外,语音工作记忆能预测 1:1 结构的 CSWL 的准确性,但不能预测 2:1 结构的 CSWL 的准确性。研究结果凸显了将语言经验和认知技能结合起来考虑的重要性,从而更好地理解促进个人CSWL技能的因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Probing the role of multilingualism and working memory in cross-situational word learning.","authors":"Ye Li, Viridiana L Benitez","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001361","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001361","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cross-situational word learning (CSWL), the ability to resolve word-referent ambiguity across encounters, is a powerful mechanism found in infants, children, and adults. Yet, we know little about what predicts individual differences in CSWL, especially when learning different mapping structures, such as when referents have a single name (1:1 mapping structure) or two names (2:1 mapping structure). Here, we investigated how multilingual experience and working memory skills (visuo-spatial and phonological) contributed to CSWL of 1:1 and 2:1 structures. Monolingual (<i>n</i> = 78) and multilingual (<i>n</i> = 106) adults completed CSWL tasks of 1:1 and 2:1 structures, a symmetry span task, and a listening span task. Results from path models showed that multilingualism predicted visuo-spatial working memory but not CSWL. Additionally, phonological working memory predicted accuracy on CSWL of 1:1 structure, but not 2:1 structure. Findings highlight the importance of considering language experience and cognitive skills together to better understand the factors that promote individual CSWL skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attentional mechanisms of the date/delay effect in intertemporal choice: An eye-tracking study. 时际选择中日期/延迟效应的注意机制:眼动追踪研究。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001363
Kristof Keidel, Carsten Murawski, Ulrich Ettinger

Temporal discounting refers to the tendency to discount future rewards as a function of time until receipt of rewards. The discount rate can be reduced by experimentally manipulating time framing, an example being the date/delay effect: Specifically, if time until receipt of the reward is presented as a date (e.g., August 21, 2022) rather than as a delay (e.g., 136 days), temporal discounting is reduced. While this effect has been replicated several times, its underlying cognitive mechanisms are not well understood. Therefore, we used eye tracking to examine the role of attention in the date/delay effect. Participants completed both a delay and date condition of the Monetary Choice Questionnaire, while eye movements were recorded (N = 54). Results revealed a successful replication of the date/delay effect (p < .001, gav = 0.48). Eye tracking showed that participants compared time attributes (relative to reward attributes) more and fixated them longer in the date compared to the delay condition. Moreover, the absolute difference in reward values of choice options was more predictive of choosing the delayed reward in the date compared to the delay condition. Finally, explorative correlations revealed a stronger date/delay effect in participants who paid more attention to time than reward attributes in the delay condition and who used a more integrative search strategy. Our findings suggest that the date manipulation causes participants to weight rewards more strongly in their decision process than in the delay condition, ultimately reducing temporal discounting. Computation of time intervals in the date condition could possibly reflect an adaptation lowering the date/delay effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

时间折扣指的是在获得奖励之前,未来奖励会随着时间的推移而打折扣。折现率可以通过实验操纵时间框架来降低,例如日期/延迟效应:具体来说,如果将获得奖励前的时间作为一个日期(如 2022 年 8 月 21 日)而不是延迟时间(如 136 天)来呈现,时间折扣就会降低。虽然这种效应已被多次复制,但其背后的认知机制却不甚明了。因此,我们使用眼动跟踪来研究注意力在日期/延迟效应中的作用。受试者同时完成了货币选择问卷的延迟和日期条件,同时眼动也被记录下来(54 人)。结果显示,日期/延迟效应被成功复制(p < .001, gav = 0.48)。眼动跟踪显示,与延迟条件相比,参与者在日期条件下更多地比较了时间属性(相对于奖励属性),并且固定时间更长。此外,与延迟条件相比,在日期条件下,选择选项奖励值的绝对差异更能预测选择延迟奖励的可能性。最后,探索性相关分析表明,在延迟条件下,那些更关注时间而非奖励属性的参与者,以及那些使用更综合搜索策略的参与者,会受到更强的日期/延迟效应的影响。我们的研究结果表明,与延迟条件相比,日期操作会使参与者在决策过程中更重视奖励,从而最终减少时间折扣。在日期条件下计算时间间隔可能反映了一种降低日期/延迟效应的适应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Attentional mechanisms of the date/delay effect in intertemporal choice: An eye-tracking study.","authors":"Kristof Keidel, Carsten Murawski, Ulrich Ettinger","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001363","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001363","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Temporal discounting refers to the tendency to discount future rewards as a function of time until receipt of rewards. The discount rate can be reduced by experimentally manipulating time framing, an example being the date/delay effect: Specifically, if time until receipt of the reward is presented as a date (e.g., August 21, 2022) rather than as a delay (e.g., 136 days), temporal discounting is reduced. While this effect has been replicated several times, its underlying cognitive mechanisms are not well understood. Therefore, we used eye tracking to examine the role of attention in the date/delay effect. Participants completed both a delay and date condition of the Monetary Choice Questionnaire, while eye movements were recorded (<i>N</i> = 54). Results revealed a successful replication of the date/delay effect (<i>p</i> < .001, <i>g</i><sub>av</sub> = 0.48). Eye tracking showed that participants compared time attributes (relative to reward attributes) more and fixated them longer in the date compared to the delay condition. Moreover, the absolute difference in reward values of choice options was more predictive of choosing the delayed reward in the date compared to the delay condition. Finally, explorative correlations revealed a stronger date/delay effect in participants who paid more attention to time than reward attributes in the delay condition and who used a more integrative search strategy. Our findings suggest that the date manipulation causes participants to weight rewards more strongly in their decision process than in the delay condition, ultimately reducing temporal discounting. Computation of time intervals in the date condition could possibly reflect an adaptation lowering the date/delay effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141428153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Wait, how did you call this?": Speaker-specific word choices are stored and generalized. "等等,你怎么称呼这个?特定说话人的词汇选择会被储存和概括。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001348
Nitzan Trainin, Einat Shetreet

It has been repeatedly shown that individuals track speaker-specific language use during interaction. Most studies focused on how this facilitates meaning inference when interspeaker variation differentiates between two or more alternatives, or how it allows for successful lexical alignment. However, it has been unclear whether mapping interspeaker variation is stored actively, and if so, what purposes this storage serves. In a pseudointeractive experiment, we created interspeaker variation in naming preferences, such that one speaker (the common speaker) consistently produced favored words, and the other speaker consistently produced less-favored/disfavored words (the uncommon speaker), across two conditions-one where both speakers were relatively common, and one where one of the speakers was highly uncommon. Participants engaged in a picture selection task, at first as matchers (where they were instructed by one of the speakers-each in his/her turn-which image to choose), and then as directors (where they were the instructors). They were then tested on how well they mapped interspeaker variation and how they generalized it linguistically and socially. Participants were successful at directly mapping interspeaker variation in naming preferences. Furthermore, they used this information in (a) lexically aligning with their interlocutors, (b) hypothesizing about unexposed word choices by these speakers, and (c) creating social representations of the speakers as individuals. In line with surprisal-driven learning accounts, these effects were larger for a speaker that used highly uncommon words. Our results suggest that individuals store interspeaker variation explicitly, which in turn helps them to predict their interlocutors' future linguistic and social behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究一再表明,个体在互动过程中会跟踪特定说话者的语言使用情况。大多数研究都集中在当说话者之间的差异将两个或更多选择区分开来时,这种差异是如何促进意义推断的,或者是如何使词汇对齐成功的。然而,目前还不清楚说话者之间的差异映射是否会被主动存储,如果是,这种存储的目的是什么。在一项假互动实验中,我们创造了说话者之间在命名偏好方面的差异,这样,在两个条件下,一个条件是两个说话者都相对常见,另一个条件是其中一个说话者非常不常见。受试者参与了一项图片选择任务,他们先是作为配对者(受其中一位说话者的指导,轮流选择图片),然后又作为指导者(他们是指导者)。然后测试他们对说话者之间的差异进行映射的能力,以及他们如何在语言和社交方面进行概括。学员们成功地直接映射出了命名偏好中的说话者之间的差异。此外,他们还利用这些信息:(a)在词汇上与对话者保持一致;(b)对这些说话者的未曝光词汇选择进行假设;以及(c)将说话者作为个体建立社会表征。与惊奇驱动学习的观点一致的是,这些效应在说话者使用非常不常见的词时更大。我们的研究结果表明,个体会明确存储说话者之间的差异,这反过来又有助于他们预测对话者未来的语言和社会行为。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"\"Wait, how did you call this?\": Speaker-specific word choices are stored and generalized.","authors":"Nitzan Trainin, Einat Shetreet","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001348","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001348","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been repeatedly shown that individuals track speaker-specific language use during interaction. Most studies focused on how this facilitates meaning inference when interspeaker variation differentiates between two or more alternatives, or how it allows for successful lexical alignment. However, it has been unclear whether mapping interspeaker variation is stored actively, and if so, what purposes this storage serves. In a pseudointeractive experiment, we created interspeaker variation in naming preferences, such that one speaker (the common speaker) consistently produced favored words, and the other speaker consistently produced less-favored/disfavored words (the uncommon speaker), across two conditions-one where both speakers were relatively common, and one where one of the speakers was highly uncommon. Participants engaged in a picture selection task, at first as matchers (where they were instructed by one of the speakers-each in his/her turn-which image to choose), and then as directors (where they were the instructors). They were then tested on how well they mapped interspeaker variation and how they generalized it linguistically and socially. Participants were successful at directly mapping interspeaker variation in naming preferences. Furthermore, they used this information in (a) lexically aligning with their interlocutors, (b) hypothesizing about unexposed word choices by these speakers, and (c) creating social representations of the speakers as individuals. In line with surprisal-driven learning accounts, these effects were larger for a speaker that used highly uncommon words. Our results suggest that individuals store interspeaker variation explicitly, which in turn helps them to predict their interlocutors' future linguistic and social behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141428152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the response suppression mechanism of working memory. 测试工作记忆的反应抑制机制
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001359
Benjamin Kowialiewski, Klaus Oberauer

Many working memory (WM) paradigms involve recalling multiple items from the same memory set. Participants rarely repeat items they have already recalled, avoiding repetition errors. To prevent these errors, WM models incorporate a response suppression mechanism that removes recalled items from the set of response options. Despite its importance for our understanding of WM, response suppression has received limited direct testing. To address this gap, we used computational models implementing two hypothetical mechanisms of response suppression to derive predictions and tested these predictions experimentally. Participants were asked to recall the same items multiple times during a single trial. If already recalled items are removed from the response set to prevent repetition errors, memory performance should be impaired when the same item is tested again. Contrary to this, we found that memory performance was unimpaired when the same item was tested a second time, and even displayed a recall advantage. Therefore, this study demonstrates the implausibility of response suppression to account for how people avoid repetition errors. We discuss alternative explanations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多工作记忆(WM)范式都涉及从同一记忆集中回忆多个项目。为了避免重复错误,被试很少重复他们已经回忆过的项目。为了避免这些错误,工作记忆模型中包含了一种反应抑制机制,它可以从反应选项集中删除已回忆起的项目。尽管反应抑制机制对我们理解 WM 非常重要,但对它的直接测试却很有限。为了弥补这一不足,我们使用计算模型实现了两种假设的反应抑制机制,从而得出了预测结果,并通过实验对这些预测结果进行了测试。我们要求参与者在一次试验中多次回忆相同的项目。如果为了防止重复错误而将已经回忆起的项目从反应集中删除,那么当再次测试同一项目时,记忆表现应该会受到影响。与此相反,我们发现,当同一项目被第二次测试时,记忆表现并没有受到影响,甚至还表现出了回忆优势。因此,这项研究表明,用反应抑制来解释人们如何避免重复错误是不靠谱的。我们将讨论其他的解释。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Testing the response suppression mechanism of working memory.","authors":"Benjamin Kowialiewski, Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001359","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001359","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many working memory (WM) paradigms involve recalling multiple items from the same memory set. Participants rarely repeat items they have already recalled, avoiding repetition errors. To prevent these errors, WM models incorporate a response suppression mechanism that removes recalled items from the set of response options. Despite its importance for our understanding of WM, response suppression has received limited direct testing. To address this gap, we used computational models implementing two hypothetical mechanisms of response suppression to derive predictions and tested these predictions experimentally. Participants were asked to recall the same items multiple times during a single trial. If already recalled items are removed from the response set to prevent repetition errors, memory performance should be impaired when the same item is tested again. Contrary to this, we found that memory performance was unimpaired when the same item was tested a second time, and even displayed a recall advantage. Therefore, this study demonstrates the implausibility of response suppression to account for how people avoid repetition errors. We discuss alternative explanations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141200477","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The number of different digits determines solution and verification of multiplication problems. 不同数字的个数决定了乘法问题的解和验证。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001310
Smadar Sapir-Yogev, Gitit Kavé, Sarit Ashkenazi

The solution and verification of single-digit multiplication problems vary in speed and accuracy. The current study examines whether the number of different digits in a problem accounts for this variance. In Experiment 1, 41 participants solved all 2-9 multiplication problems. In Experiment 2, 43 participants verified these problems. In Experiment 3, 26 participants solved 10 problems that differed in shared-digit network (SDN) size and matched in problem size. In Experiment 4, 24 participants verified these matched sets. Results show faster and more accurate responses to problems that include fewer different digits relative to problems with more different digits, and faster and more accurate responses to problems whose SDN is small relative to problems whose SDN is large. We thus show that the number of different digits in a problem, including the operands and the solution, determines the speed and accuracy of its solution and verification. This parsimonious account also explains why responses to five and tie problems, which include fewer different digits relative to nonfive and nontie problems, are faster and more accurate than responses to other problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

解决和验证个位数乘法问题的速度和准确性各不相同。目前的研究考察了问题中不同数字的数量是否能解释这种差异。在实验1中,41名参与者解决了所有2-9乘法问题。在实验2中,43名参与者验证了这些问题。在实验3中,26名被试解决了10个共享数字网络(shared-digit network, SDN)大小不同但问题大小匹配的问题。在实验4中,24名参与者验证了这些匹配集。结果表明,对于不同数字较少的问题,响应速度比不同数字较多的问题更快、更准确;对于SDN较小的问题,响应速度比SDN较大的问题更快、更准确。因此,我们表明,一个问题中不同数字的数量,包括操作数和解,决定了其解和验证的速度和准确性。这种简洁的解释也解释了为什么对五和八问题的反应比对其他问题的反应更快、更准确,因为与非五和八问题相比,五和八问题包含的不同数字更少。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The number of different digits determines solution and verification of multiplication problems.","authors":"Smadar Sapir-Yogev, Gitit Kavé, Sarit Ashkenazi","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001310","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001310","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The solution and verification of single-digit multiplication problems vary in speed and accuracy. The current study examines whether the number of different digits in a problem accounts for this variance. In Experiment 1, 41 participants solved all 2-9 multiplication problems. In Experiment 2, 43 participants verified these problems. In Experiment 3, 26 participants solved 10 problems that differed in shared-digit network (SDN) size and matched in problem size. In Experiment 4, 24 participants verified these matched sets. Results show faster and more accurate responses to problems that include fewer different digits relative to problems with more different digits, and faster and more accurate responses to problems whose SDN is small relative to problems whose SDN is large. We thus show that the number of different digits in a problem, including the operands and the solution, determines the speed and accuracy of its solution and verification. This parsimonious account also explains why responses to five and tie problems, which include fewer different digits relative to nonfive and nontie problems, are faster and more accurate than responses to other problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"944-956"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136400081","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The function/content word distinction and eye movements in reading. 阅读中的功能/内容单词区分和眼动。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001301
Adrian Staub

A substantial quantity of research has explored whether readers' eye movements are sensitive to the distinction between function and content words. No clear answer has emerged, in part due to the difficulty of accounting for differences in length, frequency, and predictability between the words in the two classes. Based on evidence that readers differentially overlook function word errors, we hypothesized that function words may be more frequently skipped or may receive shorter fixations. We present two very large-scale eyetracking experiments using selected sentences from a corpus of natural text, with each sentence containing a target function or content word. The target words in the two classes were carefully matched on length, frequency, and predictability, with the latter variable operationalized in terms of next-word probability obtained from the large language model GPT-2. While the experiments replicated a range of expected effects, word class did not have any clear influence on target word skipping probability, and there was some evidence for a content word advantage in fixation duration measures. These results indicate that readers' tendency to overlook function word errors is not due to reduced time spent encoding these words. The results also broadly support the implicit assumption in prominent models of eye movement control in reading that a word's syntactic category does not play an important role in decisions about when and where to move the eyes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大量研究探讨了读者的眼球运动是否对功能词和内容词之间的区别敏感。目前还没有明确的答案,部分原因是很难解释这两个类别中单词在长度、频率和可预测性方面的差异。基于读者不同地忽略虚词错误的证据,我们假设虚词可能更频繁地被跳过,或者可能受到更短的注视。我们提出了两个非常大规模的眼动追踪实验,使用从自然文本语料库中选择的句子,每个句子都包含一个目标函数或内容词。两类中的目标词在长度、频率和可预测性方面进行了仔细匹配,后一个变量根据从大型语言模型GPT-2中获得的下一个单词概率进行了操作。虽然实验复制了一系列预期效果,但单词类别对目标单词跳过概率没有任何明显的影响,并且有一些证据表明内容单词在固定持续时间测量方面具有优势。这些结果表明,读者忽视虚词错误的倾向并不是因为编码这些单词所花费的时间减少。研究结果还广泛支持了阅读中眼动控制突出模型中的隐含假设,即单词的句法类别在决定何时何地移动眼睛方面并不起重要作用。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The function/content word distinction and eye movements in reading.","authors":"Adrian Staub","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001301","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001301","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A substantial quantity of research has explored whether readers' eye movements are sensitive to the distinction between function and content words. No clear answer has emerged, in part due to the difficulty of accounting for differences in length, frequency, and predictability between the words in the two classes. Based on evidence that readers differentially overlook function word errors, we hypothesized that function words may be more frequently skipped or may receive shorter fixations. We present two very large-scale eyetracking experiments using selected sentences from a corpus of natural text, with each sentence containing a target function or content word. The target words in the two classes were carefully matched on length, frequency, and predictability, with the latter variable operationalized in terms of next-word probability obtained from the large language model GPT-2. While the experiments replicated a range of expected effects, word class did not have any clear influence on target word skipping probability, and there was some evidence for a <i>content</i> word advantage in fixation duration measures. These results indicate that readers' tendency to overlook function word errors is not due to reduced time spent encoding these words. The results also broadly support the implicit assumption in prominent models of eye movement control in reading that a word's syntactic category does not play an important role in decisions about when and where to move the eyes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"967-984"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50163470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1