首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
It is not what you say but how you say it: Evidence from Russian shows robust effects of the structural prior on noisy channel inferences. 关键不在于说什么,而在于怎么说:来自俄罗斯的证据表明,结构先验对有噪声的渠道推断有很强的影响。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001244
Moshe Poliak, Rachel Ryskin, Mika Braginsky, Edward Gibson

Under the noisy-channel framework of language comprehension, comprehenders infer the speaker's intended meaning by integrating the perceived utterance with their knowledge of the language, the world, and the kinds of errors that can occur in communication. Previous research has shown that, when sentences are improbable under the meaning prior (implausible sentences), participants often interpret them nonliterally. The rate of nonliteral interpretation is higher when the errors that could have transformed the intended utterance into the perceived utterance are more likely. However, previous experiments on noisy channel processing mostly relied on implausible sentences, and it is unclear whether participants' nonliteral interpretations were evidence of noisy channel processing or the result of trying to conform to the experimenter's expectations in an experiment with nonsensical sentences. In the current study, we used the unique properties of Russian, an understudied language in the psycholinguistics literature, to test noisy-channel comprehension using only simple plausible sentences. The prior plausibility of sentences was tied only to their word order; subject-verb-object (SVO) sentences were more probable under the structural prior than object-verb-subject (OVS) sentences. In two experiments, we show that participants often interpret OVS sentences nonliterally, and the probability of nonliteral interpretations depended on the Levenshtein distance between the perceived sentence and the (potentially intended) SVO version of the sentence. The results show that the structural prior guides people's final interpretation, independent of the presence of semantic implausibility. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在语言理解的噪声信道框架下,理解者通过将感知到的语句与他们对语言、世界以及交流中可能出现的各种错误的了解结合起来,来推断说话者想要表达的意思。以往的研究表明,当句子在先验意义下是不可能的(不可信句子)时,参与者通常会对其进行非字面解释。当错误更有可能将意图语句转化为感知语句时,非字面解释的比率就会更高。然而,以往的噪音通道加工实验大多依赖于不可信的句子,参与者的非字面解释究竟是噪音通道加工的证据,还是在使用无意义句子的实验中试图符合实验者期望的结果,目前尚不清楚。在当前的研究中,我们利用俄语这种在心理语言学文献中未被充分研究的语言的独特性质,仅使用简单的似是而非的句子来测试噪声通道理解能力。句子的先验可信度只与它们的词序有关;在结构先验下,主谓宾(SVO)句子比宾谓主(OVS)句子更有可能出现。在两个实验中,我们发现被试经常会对 OVS 句子进行非字面解释,而非字面解释的概率取决于感知句子与 SVO 句子(潜在意图)之间的列文斯泰因距离。结果表明,结构先验指导着人们的最终解释,而与语义不可信性的存在无关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"It is not what you say but how you say it: Evidence from Russian shows robust effects of the structural prior on noisy channel inferences.","authors":"Moshe Poliak, Rachel Ryskin, Mika Braginsky, Edward Gibson","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001244","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001244","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Under the noisy-channel framework of language comprehension, comprehenders infer the speaker's intended meaning by integrating the perceived utterance with their knowledge of the language, the world, and the kinds of errors that can occur in communication. Previous research has shown that, when sentences are improbable under the meaning prior (implausible sentences), participants often interpret them nonliterally. The rate of nonliteral interpretation is higher when the errors that could have transformed the intended utterance into the perceived utterance are more likely. However, previous experiments on noisy channel processing mostly relied on implausible sentences, and it is unclear whether participants' nonliteral interpretations were evidence of noisy channel processing or the result of trying to conform to the experimenter's expectations in an experiment with nonsensical sentences. In the current study, we used the unique properties of Russian, an understudied language in the psycholinguistics literature, to test noisy-channel comprehension using only simple plausible sentences. The prior plausibility of sentences was tied only to their word order; subject-verb-object (SVO) sentences were more probable under the structural prior than object-verb-subject (OVS) sentences. In two experiments, we show that participants often interpret OVS sentences nonliterally, and the probability of nonliteral interpretations depended on the Levenshtein distance between the perceived sentence and the (potentially intended) SVO version of the sentence. The results show that the structural prior guides people's final interpretation, independent of the presence of semantic implausibility. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9491629","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic richness effects in isolated spoken word recognition: Evidence from massive auditory lexical decision. 孤立口语单词识别中的语义丰富性效应:来自大规模听觉词汇决策的证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-15 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001208
Filip Nenadić, Ryan G Podlubny, Daniel Schmidtke, Matthew C Kelley, Benjamin V Tucker

While known to influence visual lexical processing, the semantic information we associate with words has recently been found to influence auditory lexical processing as well. The present work explored the influence of semantic richness in auditory lexical decision. Study 1 recreated an experiment investigating semantic richness effects in concrete nouns (Goh et al., 2016). In Study 2, we expanded the stimulus set from 442 to 8,626 items, exploring the robustness of effects observed in Study 1 against a larger data set with increased diversity in both word class and other characteristics of interest. We also utilized generalized additive mixed models to investigate potential nonlinear effects. Results indicate that semantic richness effects become more nuanced and detectable when a wider set of items belonging to different parts of speech is examined. Findings are discussed in the context of models of spoken word recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

众所周知,视觉会影响词汇的处理,而我们与词汇相关联的语义信息最近也被发现会影响听觉词汇的处理。本研究探讨了语义丰富度对听觉词汇决策的影响。研究 1 重现了一项调查具体名词语义丰富度影响的实验(Goh 等人,2016 年)。在研究 2 中,我们将刺激集从 442 个条目扩展到 8626 个条目,探索研究 1 中观察到的效应在更大的数据集上的稳健性,该数据集在词类和其他感兴趣的特征方面都更加多样化。我们还利用广义加法混合模型来研究潜在的非线性效应。研究结果表明,在研究属于不同语篇的更大范围的项目时,语义丰富度效应会变得更加细微,也更容易察觉。研究结果将结合口语词汇识别模型进行讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Semantic richness effects in isolated spoken word recognition: Evidence from massive auditory lexical decision.","authors":"Filip Nenadić, Ryan G Podlubny, Daniel Schmidtke, Matthew C Kelley, Benjamin V Tucker","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001208","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001208","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While known to influence visual lexical processing, the semantic information we associate with words has recently been found to influence auditory lexical processing as well. The present work explored the influence of <i>semantic richness</i> in auditory lexical decision. Study 1 recreated an experiment investigating semantic richness effects in concrete nouns (Goh et al., 2016). In Study 2, we expanded the stimulus set from 442 to 8,626 items, exploring the robustness of effects observed in Study 1 against a larger data set with increased diversity in both word class and other characteristics of interest. We also utilized generalized additive mixed models to investigate potential nonlinear effects. Results indicate that semantic richness effects become more nuanced and detectable when a wider set of items belonging to different parts of speech is examined. Findings are discussed in the context of models of spoken word recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10356508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive style of field dependence-independence modulates the working memory storage of biological motion. 场依赖-不依赖的认知风格调节生物运动的工作记忆存储。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001256
Kaixuan Wang, Yue Ma, Xiaowei Che, Shouxin Li, Qian Zhang

The biological motion refers to the continuous configuration movement of live agents in space. The perceptual processing of biological motion has the specificity of the dissociation between body form and body motion. However, there is limited evidence for whether such specificity continues when holding biological motion in working memory. We explored this question from the perspective of field dependence (FD) and field independence (FI) cognitive styles in the current study. Three categories of biological motion have been developed: intact movement, motion feature, and form feature. We examined the working memory capacity of motion features, form features, intact movements (Experiments 1-3), and the recognition of three categories of biological motion when remembering intact movements (Experiment 4). The results showed that for the motion features, FI individuals had better memory performance when remembering five items and showed greater working memory capacity and recognition compared with FD individuals, whereas the opposite pattern was observed between FI and FD individuals for the form features. The cognitive style could modulate the working memory storage of biological motion when the task becomes demanding, suggesting that body form and body motion are dissociable in working memory. Our study provided additional evidence for the specificity of biological motion processing in working memory, extending the hierarchical neural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

生物运动是指活体在空间中的连续构型运动。生物运动的知觉处理具有将身体形态和身体运动分离开来的特异性。然而,关于在工作记忆中保持生物运动时这种特异性是否会继续存在的证据却很有限。在本研究中,我们从场依赖(FD)和场独立(FI)认知风格的角度探讨了这一问题。生物运动有三个类别:完整运动、运动特征和形式特征。我们考察了运动特征、形式特征和完整运动的工作记忆能力(实验 1-3),以及记忆完整运动时对三类生物运动的识别能力(实验 4)。结果表明,就运动特征而言,FI个体与FD个体相比,在记忆5个项目时有更好的记忆表现,工作记忆能力和识别能力也更强;而就形式特征而言,FI个体与FD个体的情况正好相反。当任务变得繁重时,认知风格可以调节生物运动的工作记忆存储,这表明身体形态和身体运动在工作记忆中是可以分离的。我们的研究为工作记忆中生物运动处理的特异性提供了更多证据,扩展了分层神经模型。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Cognitive style of field dependence-independence modulates the working memory storage of biological motion.","authors":"Kaixuan Wang, Yue Ma, Xiaowei Che, Shouxin Li, Qian Zhang","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001256","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001256","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The biological motion refers to the continuous configuration movement of live agents in space. The perceptual processing of biological motion has the specificity of the dissociation between body form and body motion. However, there is limited evidence for whether such specificity continues when holding biological motion in working memory. We explored this question from the perspective of field dependence (FD) and field independence (FI) cognitive styles in the current study. Three categories of biological motion have been developed: intact movement, motion feature, and form feature. We examined the working memory capacity of motion features, form features, intact movements (Experiments 1-3), and the recognition of three categories of biological motion when remembering intact movements (Experiment 4). The results showed that for the motion features, FI individuals had better memory performance when remembering five items and showed greater working memory capacity and recognition compared with FD individuals, whereas the opposite pattern was observed between FI and FD individuals for the form features. The cognitive style could modulate the working memory storage of biological motion when the task becomes demanding, suggesting that body form and body motion are dissociable in working memory. Our study provided additional evidence for the specificity of biological motion processing in working memory, extending the hierarchical neural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Manipulations of richness of encoding do not modulate the animacy effect on memory. 操纵编码的丰富性并不能调节记忆中的灵性效应。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-25 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001249
Gesa Fee Komar, Laura Mieth, Axel Buchner, Raoul Bell

The animacy effect refers to the memory advantage of words denoting animate beings over words denoting inanimate objects. Remembering animate beings may serve important evolutionary functions, but the cognitive mechanism underlying the animacy effect has remained elusive. According to the richness-of-encoding account, animate words stimulate participants to generate more ideas than inanimate words at encoding. These ideas may later serve as retrieval cues and thus enhance recall. There is as yet only correlational evidence associating rich encoding and the animacy advantage in memory. To experimentally test the assumption that richness of encoding plays a causal role, we examined whether the animacy effect can be modulated by facilitating or suppressing rich encoding. In Experiment 1, richness of encoding was manipulated by requiring participants to write down four ideas or one idea in response to animate and inanimate words. In Experiment 2, the one-idea-generation condition was compared to an unrestricted-idea-generation condition. In Experiment 3, the unrestricted-idea-generation condition was compared to a distractor-task condition in which the idea-generation process was suppressed. In Experiment 4, richness of encoding was manipulated by asking participants to rate the relevance of the words for achieving three survival-related goals or one survival-related goal. Animate words were better remembered than inanimate words. In three of the four experiments, rich encoding led to improved recall. However, none of the manipulations of richness of encoding affected the animacy effect on memory, demonstrating its robustness irrespective of the encoding conditions. These results weaken the richness-of-encoding account of the animacy effect on memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

灵性效应指的是表示有生命的单词比表示无生命物体的单词具有记忆优势。记住有生命的人可能具有重要的进化功能,但灵性效应的认知机制却一直难以捉摸。根据编码丰富性理论,有生命的词在编码时会比无生命的词刺激参与者产生更多的想法。这些想法随后可能会成为检索线索,从而增强记忆。目前只有相关证据表明丰富的编码与记忆中的动画优势有关。为了在实验中检验丰富编码在其中发挥的作用,我们研究了是否可以通过促进或抑制丰富编码来调节动画效应。在实验 1 中,我们要求被试针对有生命和无生命的词语写下四个或一个想法,以此来操纵编码的丰富性。在实验 2 中,一个想法生成条件与不受限制的想法生成条件进行了比较。在实验 3 中,将无限制创意生成条件与抑制创意生成过程的分心任务条件进行了比较。在实验 4 中,通过让被试评定词语与实现三个生存相关目标或一个生存相关目标的相关性,来操纵编码的丰富性。有生命的词比无生命的词记忆效果更好。在四个实验中的三个实验中,丰富的编码提高了记忆效果。然而,对编码丰富性的任何操作都不会影响对记忆的灵性效应,这表明无论编码条件如何,灵性效应都是稳健的。这些结果削弱了编码丰富性对动画记忆效应的解释。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Manipulations of richness of encoding do not modulate the animacy effect on memory.","authors":"Gesa Fee Komar, Laura Mieth, Axel Buchner, Raoul Bell","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001249","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001249","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The animacy effect refers to the memory advantage of words denoting animate beings over words denoting inanimate objects. Remembering animate beings may serve important evolutionary functions, but the cognitive mechanism underlying the animacy effect has remained elusive. According to the richness-of-encoding account, animate words stimulate participants to generate more ideas than inanimate words at encoding. These ideas may later serve as retrieval cues and thus enhance recall. There is as yet only correlational evidence associating rich encoding and the animacy advantage in memory. To experimentally test the assumption that richness of encoding plays a causal role, we examined whether the animacy effect can be modulated by facilitating or suppressing rich encoding. In Experiment 1, richness of encoding was manipulated by requiring participants to write down four ideas or one idea in response to animate and inanimate words. In Experiment 2, the one-idea-generation condition was compared to an unrestricted-idea-generation condition. In Experiment 3, the unrestricted-idea-generation condition was compared to a distractor-task condition in which the idea-generation process was suppressed. In Experiment 4, richness of encoding was manipulated by asking participants to rate the relevance of the words for achieving three survival-related goals or one survival-related goal. Animate words were better remembered than inanimate words. In three of the four experiments, rich encoding led to improved recall. However, none of the manipulations of richness of encoding affected the animacy effect on memory, demonstrating its robustness irrespective of the encoding conditions. These results weaken the richness-of-encoding account of the animacy effect on memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9522378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Repeated naming affects the accessibility of nonselected words: Evidence from picture-word interference experiments. 重复命名会影响非选定词的可及性:图片-词语干扰实验的证据。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-25 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001247
Stefan Wöhner, Andreas Mädebach, Herbert Schriefers, Jörg D Jescheniak

This study traced different types of distractor effects in the picture-word interference (PWI) task across repeated naming. Starting point was a PWI study by Kurtz et al. (2018). It reported that naming a picture (e.g., of a duck) was slowed down by a distractor word phonologically related to an alternative picture name from a different taxonomic level ("birch" related to "bird") when compared to an unrelated control, indicating that the alternative name was (phonologically) coactivated. Importantly, the effect was stable across repeated naming. The authors argued that this stability challenges adaptive models of word production according to which coactivated but nonselected words become less accessible for future retrieval; such a change in lexical accessibility should have been reflected in reduced interference. Using a similar experimental protocol as Kurtz et al., our study looked at the stability of different distractor effects in PWI. Interference from a distractor word phonologically related to an alternative name was stable across repeated naming, replicating Kurtz et al. In contrast, interference from a distractor word denoting a semantic category coordinate ("stork") was reduced across repeated naming. A similar pattern was found for a distractor word corresponding to an alternative name ("bird"). Facilitation from a distractor word phonologically related to the target name ("dust"), in contrast, was stable across repeated naming. We discuss the implications of these findings with respect to the issue of changes in the accessibility of lexical representations, the use of the PWI task in this context, and the use of this task in general. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究追踪了重复命名中图片-文字干扰(PWI)任务中不同类型的分心效果。起点是 Kurtz 等人(2018 年)的一项 PWI 研究。该研究报告称,与不相关的对照组相比,在命名一幅图片(如鸭子)时,一个与不同分类级别的替代图片名称(与 "鸟 "相关的 "桦树")在语音上相关的分心词会减慢命名速度,这表明替代名称在(语音上)被共同激活。重要的是,这种效应在重复命名时是稳定的。作者认为,这种稳定性挑战了词汇生成的适应性模型,根据该模型,被共同激活但未被选择的词汇在未来检索时的可及性降低;词汇可及性的这种变化本应反映在干扰的减少上。我们的研究采用了与 Kurtz 等人类似的实验方案,考察了 PWI 中不同分心效果的稳定性。与 Kurtz 等人的研究相同,与替代名称语音相关的分心词的干扰在重复命名时是稳定的。相反,表示语义类别坐标的分心词("鹳")的干扰在重复命名时有所减少。与替代名称("鸟")相对应的分心词也发现了类似的模式。相反,与目标名称语音相关的分心词("dust")对重复命名的促进作用则保持稳定。我们讨论了这些发现对词汇表征可及性变化问题的影响、PWI 任务在这种情况下的使用以及该任务的一般使用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Repeated naming affects the accessibility of nonselected words: Evidence from picture-word interference experiments.","authors":"Stefan Wöhner, Andreas Mädebach, Herbert Schriefers, Jörg D Jescheniak","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001247","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001247","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study traced different types of distractor effects in the picture-word interference (PWI) task across repeated naming. Starting point was a PWI study by Kurtz et al. (2018). It reported that naming a picture (e.g., of a duck) was slowed down by a distractor word phonologically related to an alternative picture name from a different taxonomic level (\"birch\" related to \"bird\") when compared to an unrelated control, indicating that the alternative name was (phonologically) coactivated. Importantly, the effect was stable across repeated naming. The authors argued that this stability challenges adaptive models of word production according to which coactivated but nonselected words become less accessible for future retrieval; such a change in lexical accessibility should have been reflected in reduced interference. Using a similar experimental protocol as Kurtz et al., our study looked at the stability of different distractor effects in PWI. Interference from a distractor word phonologically related to an alternative name was stable across repeated naming, replicating Kurtz et al. In contrast, interference from a distractor word denoting a semantic category coordinate (\"stork\") was reduced across repeated naming. A similar pattern was found for a distractor word corresponding to an alternative name (\"bird\"). Facilitation from a distractor word phonologically related to the target name (\"dust\"), in contrast, was stable across repeated naming. We discuss the implications of these findings with respect to the issue of changes in the accessibility of lexical representations, the use of the PWI task in this context, and the use of this task in general. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9522381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attentional control and priming: The distractor set hypothesis. 注意控制与引物:分心集假设
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001252
Hsuan-Fu Chao

Repeating a single-prime stimulus as a target to respond to usually facilitates responses. However, sometimes, prime repetition slows the responses and produces the single-prime negative priming effect. In this study, the distractor set hypothesis was proposed as a mechanism of attentional control that can contribute toward single-prime negative priming. In Experiments 1a-1d, an integrated Stroop task was used. The results showed that the prime produced negative priming only when it matched the form of the competing distractors. A separate Stroop task was used in Experiments 2 and 3 and a flanker task was used in Experiments 4a and 4b. In both tasks, the results suggested that a prime produced negative priming when the location of the prime matched that of the distractors. In Experiment 5, alternative explanations including the effects of the prime-to-distractor similarity and the target set were examined. The results revealed that the distractor set, rather than the target set and the similarity between the prime and distractor, could better account for the negative priming effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

重复单个素色刺激作为反应目标通常会促进反应。然而,有时素材重复会减慢反应速度,产生单素材负引物效应。在本研究中,我们提出了分心集假说,认为它是一种能促成单引物负引物的注意控制机制。实验 1a-1d 采用的是综合 Stroop 任务。结果表明,只有当素材与竞争分心物的形式相匹配时,才会产生消极引物。实验 2 和 3 采用了单独的 Stroop 任务,实验 4a 和 4b 则采用了侧面任务。在这两项任务中,结果表明,当质点的位置与分心物的位置相匹配时,质点会产生负引物。在实验 5 中,我们研究了其他解释,包括素色与分心物相似性和目标集的影响。结果表明,干扰物集合,而不是目标集合以及素色和干扰物之间的相似性,能够更好地解释负引物效应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Attentional control and priming: The distractor set hypothesis.","authors":"Hsuan-Fu Chao","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001252","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001252","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Repeating a single-prime stimulus as a target to respond to usually facilitates responses. However, sometimes, prime repetition slows the responses and produces the single-prime negative priming effect. In this study, the distractor set hypothesis was proposed as a mechanism of attentional control that can contribute toward single-prime negative priming. In Experiments 1a-1d, an integrated Stroop task was used. The results showed that the prime produced negative priming only when it matched the form of the competing distractors. A separate Stroop task was used in Experiments 2 and 3 and a flanker task was used in Experiments 4a and 4b. In both tasks, the results suggested that a prime produced negative priming when the location of the prime matched that of the distractors. In Experiment 5, alternative explanations including the effects of the prime-to-distractor similarity and the target set were examined. The results revealed that the distractor set, rather than the target set and the similarity between the prime and distractor, could better account for the negative priming effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9619137","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Explaining dual-action benefits: Inhibitory control and redundancy gains as complementary mechanisms. 解释双重作用的益处:作为互补机制的抑制控制和冗余收益
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-20 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001231
Tim Raettig, Lynn Huestegge

Performing two actions at the same time usually results in performance costs. However, recent studies have also reported dual-action benefits: performing only one of two possible actions may necessitate the inhibition of the initially activated, but unwarranted second action, leading to single-action costs. Presumably, two preconditions determine the occurrence and strength of such inhibition-based dual-action benefits: (a) response set reductivity and (b) action prepotency. A nonreductive response set (given when all possible responses have to be kept in working memory) creates inhibitory action control demands in single-, but not in dual-action trials, and the ensuing inhibitory costs are proportional to the level of action prepotency (i.e., an action that is easy to initiate is hard to inhibit). Here, we set out to test this hypothesis by varying representational characteristics in working memory (namely response set reductivity and action prepotency) across four experiments. In Experiments 1 to 3, we compared (a) a randomized mode of trial presentation to (b) intermixed, but predictable fixed sequences of trial types and (c) a completely blocked mode of presentation. As expected, dual-action benefits were strongly present in Experiment 1, significantly reduced in Experiment 2, and absent in Experiment 3. This pattern of results matches our predictions derived from the assumption that differential inhibitory costs in single-action trials are the root cause of dual-action benefits. Crucially, however, the results of Experiment 4 (in which response conditions were only partially blocked) pointed to a secondary source of dual-action benefits that was inseparable from inhibition-based effects in previous experimental designs: semantic redundancy gains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

同时执行两个动作通常会导致执行成本。然而,最近的研究也报告了双行动益处:在两个可能的行动中只执行其中一个,可能需要抑制最初激活的、但不需要的第二个行动,从而导致单行动成本。据推测,有两个先决条件决定了这种基于抑制的双重行动效益的发生和强度:(a) 反应集的还原性和 (b) 行动的先行性。非还原性反应集(当所有可能的反应都必须保留在工作记忆中时)会在单行动试验中产生抑制性行动控制需求,但在双行动试验中则不会,随之而来的抑制性成本与行动先行性水平成正比(即容易启动的行动很难抑制)。在这里,我们通过在四个实验中改变工作记忆中的表征特征(即反应集还原性和动作前置性)来验证这一假设。在实验 1 至 3 中,我们比较了(a)随机试验呈现模式和(b)混合但可预测的固定试验类型序列,以及(c)完全阻断的呈现模式。不出所料,双重行动益处在实验 1 中非常明显,在实验 2 中明显减少,而在实验 3 中则没有。这种结果模式符合我们的预测,即单一行动试验中不同的抑制成本是双重行动益处的根本原因。然而,至关重要的是,实验 4(其中反应条件只被部分阻断)的结果表明,在之前的实验设计中,双重行动益处的第二种来源与基于抑制的效应是不可分割的:语义冗余收益。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Explaining dual-action benefits: Inhibitory control and redundancy gains as complementary mechanisms.","authors":"Tim Raettig, Lynn Huestegge","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001231","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Performing two actions at the same time usually results in performance costs. However, recent studies have also reported dual-action benefits: performing only one of two possible actions may necessitate the inhibition of the initially activated, but unwarranted second action, leading to single-action costs. Presumably, two preconditions determine the occurrence and strength of such inhibition-based dual-action benefits: (a) response set reductivity and (b) action prepotency. A nonreductive response set (given when all possible responses have to be kept in working memory) creates inhibitory action control demands in single-, but not in dual-action trials, and the ensuing inhibitory costs are proportional to the level of action prepotency (i.e., an action that is easy to initiate is hard to inhibit). Here, we set out to test this hypothesis by varying representational characteristics in working memory (namely response set reductivity and action prepotency) across four experiments. In Experiments 1 to 3, we compared (a) a randomized mode of trial presentation to (b) intermixed, but predictable fixed sequences of trial types and (c) a completely blocked mode of presentation. As expected, dual-action benefits were strongly present in Experiment 1, significantly reduced in Experiment 2, and absent in Experiment 3. This pattern of results matches our predictions derived from the assumption that differential inhibitory costs in single-action trials are the root cause of dual-action benefits. Crucially, however, the results of Experiment 4 (in which response conditions were only partially blocked) pointed to a secondary source of dual-action benefits that was inseparable from inhibition-based effects in previous experimental designs: semantic redundancy gains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9476230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Structure shapes the representation of a novel category. 结构塑造了新类别的表征。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-15 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001257
Sarah H Solomon, Anna C Schapiro

Concepts contain rich structures that support flexible semantic cognition. These structures can be characterized by patterns of feature covariation: Certain features tend to cluster in the same items (e.g., feathers, wings, can fly). Existing computational models demonstrate how this kind of structure can be leveraged to slowly learn the distinctions between categories, on developmental timescales. However, it is not clear whether and how we leverage feature structure to quickly learn a novel category. We thus investigated how the internal structure of a new category is first extracted from experience, with the prediction that feature-based structure would have a rapid and broad influence on the learned category representation. Across three experiments, novel categories were designed with patterns of feature associations determined by carefully constructed graph structures, with Modular graphs-exhibiting strong clusters of feature covariation-compared against Random and Lattice graphs. In Experiment 1, a feature inference task using verbal stimuli revealed that Modular structure broadly facilitated category learning. Experiment 2 replicated this effect in visual categories. In Experiment 3, a statistical learning paradigm revealed that this Modular benefit relates to high-level structure rather than pairwise feature associations and persists even when category structure is incidental to the task. A neural network model was readily able to account for these effects, suggesting that correlational feature structure may be encoded within rapidly learned, distributed category representations. These findings constrain theories of category representation and link theories of category learning with structure learning more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

概念包含丰富的结构,可支持灵活的语义认知。这些结构可以用特征共变模式来描述:某些特征往往聚集在相同的项目中(如羽毛、翅膀、会飞)。现有的计算模型展示了如何利用这种结构,在发展的时间尺度上慢慢学习类别之间的区别。然而,我们是否以及如何利用特征结构来快速学习一个新类别,目前还不清楚。因此,我们研究了如何首先从经验中提取新类别的内部结构,并预测基于特征的结构将对学习到的类别表征产生快速而广泛的影响。在三个实验中,我们设计了新类别,其特征关联模式由精心构建的图结构决定,其中模块图(Modular graphs)与随机图(Random graphs)和格状图(Lattice graphs)相比,表现出强烈的特征共变群。在实验 1 中,使用语言刺激进行的特征推理任务显示,模块化结构广泛促进了类别学习。实验 2 在视觉类别中复制了这一效果。在实验 3 中,统计学习范式揭示了这种模块化优势与高层次结构有关,而不是与成对的特征关联有关,而且即使在类别结构与任务无关的情况下,这种优势也会持续存在。神经网络模型很容易解释这些效应,表明相关特征结构可能被编码在快速学习的分布式类别表征中。这些发现制约了类别表征理论,并将类别学习理论与更广泛的结构学习理论联系起来。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Structure shapes the representation of a novel category.","authors":"Sarah H Solomon, Anna C Schapiro","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001257","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001257","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Concepts contain rich structures that support flexible semantic cognition. These structures can be characterized by patterns of feature covariation: Certain features tend to cluster in the same items (e.g., <i>feathers, wings, can fly</i>). Existing computational models demonstrate how this kind of structure can be leveraged to slowly learn the distinctions between categories, on developmental timescales. However, it is not clear whether and how we leverage feature structure to quickly learn a novel category. We thus investigated how the internal structure of a new category is first extracted from experience, with the prediction that feature-based structure would have a rapid and broad influence on the learned category representation. Across three experiments, novel categories were designed with patterns of feature associations determined by carefully constructed graph structures, with Modular graphs-exhibiting strong clusters of feature covariation-compared against Random and Lattice graphs. In Experiment 1, a feature inference task using verbal stimuli revealed that Modular structure broadly facilitated category learning. Experiment 2 replicated this effect in visual categories. In Experiment 3, a statistical learning paradigm revealed that this Modular benefit relates to high-level structure rather than pairwise feature associations and persists even when category structure is incidental to the task. A neural network model was readily able to account for these effects, suggesting that correlational feature structure may be encoded within rapidly learned, distributed category representations. These findings constrain theories of category representation and link theories of category learning with structure learning more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9637163","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning basic arithmetic: A comparison between rote and procedural learning based on an artificial sequence. 学习基本算术:基于人工序列的背诵式学习和程序式学习的比较。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-06 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001241
Stéphanie Chouteau, Benoît Lemaire, Catherine Thevenot, Jasinta Dewi, Karine Mazens

It is commonly accepted that repeatedly using mental procedures results in a transition to memory retrieval, but the determinant of this process is still unclear. In a 3-week experiment, we compared two different learning situations involving basic additions, one based on counting and the other based on arithmetic fact memorization. Two groups of participants learned to verify additions such as "G + 2 = Q?" built on an artificial sequence (e.g., "XGRQD…"). The first group learned the sequence beforehand and could therefore count to solve the problems, whereas the second group was not aware of the sequence and had to learn the equations by rote. With practice, solution times of both groups reached a plateau, indicating a certain level of automatization. However, a more fine-grained comparison indicated that participants relied on fundamentally different learning mechanisms. In the counting condition, most participants showed a persistent linear effect of the numerical operand on solution times, suggesting that fluency was reached through an acceleration of counting procedures. However, some participants began memorizing the problems involving the largest addends: Their solution times were very similar to those of participants in the rote learning group, suggesting that they resulted from a memory retrieval process. These findings show that repeated mental procedures do not systematically lead to memory retrieval but that fluency can also be reached through the acceleration of these procedures. Moreover, these results challenge associationist models, which cannot currently predict that the process of memorization begins with problems involving the largest addends. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们普遍认为,反复使用心理程序会导致向记忆检索过渡,但这一过程的决定因素仍不清楚。在一项为期 3 周的实验中,我们比较了涉及基本加法的两种不同学习情境,一种是基于计数的学习情境,另一种是基于算术事实记忆的学习情境。两组参与者学习验证建立在人工序列(如 "XGRQD......")上的加法,如 "G + 2 = Q?第一组事先学习了序列,因此可以通过计算来解决问题,而第二组则不知道序列,只能通过死记硬背来学习等式。随着练习的进行,两组学生的解题时间都达到了一个平稳点,这表明他们已经达到了一定的自动化水平。然而,更细致的比较表明,学员们所依赖的学习机制有着本质的不同。在计数条件下,大多数参与者的数字操作数对解题时间有持续的线性影响,这表明他们是通过加速计数程序来达到流畅解题的。然而,一些参与者开始记忆涉及最大加数的问题:他们的解题时间与死记硬背组的学员非常相似,这表明他们的解题时间是记忆检索过程的结果。这些研究结果表明,重复的思维过程并不会系统地导致记忆检索,但通过加速这些过程也可以达到流畅的记忆。此外,这些结果对联想模型提出了挑战,因为联想模型目前无法预测记忆过程是从涉及最大加数的问题开始的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Learning basic arithmetic: A comparison between rote and procedural learning based on an artificial sequence.","authors":"Stéphanie Chouteau, Benoît Lemaire, Catherine Thevenot, Jasinta Dewi, Karine Mazens","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001241","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001241","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is commonly accepted that repeatedly using mental procedures results in a transition to memory retrieval, but the determinant of this process is still unclear. In a 3-week experiment, we compared two different learning situations involving basic additions, one based on counting and the other based on arithmetic fact memorization. Two groups of participants learned to verify additions such as \"G + 2 = Q?\" built on an artificial sequence (e.g., \"XGRQD…\"). The first group learned the sequence beforehand and could therefore count to solve the problems, whereas the second group was not aware of the sequence and had to learn the equations by rote. With practice, solution times of both groups reached a plateau, indicating a certain level of automatization. However, a more fine-grained comparison indicated that participants relied on fundamentally different learning mechanisms. In the counting condition, most participants showed a persistent linear effect of the numerical operand on solution times, suggesting that fluency was reached through an acceleration of counting procedures. However, some participants began memorizing the problems involving the largest addends: Their solution times were very similar to those of participants in the rote learning group, suggesting that they resulted from a memory retrieval process. These findings show that repeated mental procedures do not systematically lead to memory retrieval but that fluency can also be reached through the acceleration of these procedures. Moreover, these results challenge associationist models, which cannot currently predict that the process of memorization begins with problems involving the largest addends. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9257687","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nothing else matters: Stimulus-response binding and retrieval is independent of affective consequences. 其他的都不重要:刺激反应的绑定和检索与情感后果无关。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001288
Juhi Parmar, Klaus Rothermund

Stimulus-response binding and retrieval (SRBR) is a fundamental mechanism driving behavior automatization. In five experiments, we investigated the modulatory role of affective consequences (AC) on SRBR effects to test whether binding/retrieval can explain instrumental learning (i.e., the "law of effect"). SRBR effects were assessed in a sequential prime-probe design, with an orthogonal variation of response relation (response repetition vs. change) by distractor relation (word repetition vs. change). Binding/retrieval effects are measured by an interaction of the two factors, with distractor repetitions inducing a retrieval of the prime episode and a tendency to re-enact the previous response, which leads to facilitation in conditions where the response must be repeated, but leads to interference when the required response changes from prime to probe. Positive, neutral, or negative events signaling changes in points that were linked to monetary gains or losses were delivered after every trial to investigate whether AC modulate the binding/retrieval effect. Consistently across all five experiments (total N= 466), robust SRBR effects were obtained, but we did not find any evidence for an affective modulation of these binding/retrieval effects, indicating that these effects are automatic and independent of AC. In particular, Experiment 5 demonstrated a dissociation between instrumental learning via AC (reflected in higher frequencies of rewarded responses) and SRBR, which was not influenced by AC following a response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

刺激反应绑定和检索(SRBR)是驱动行为自动化的基本机制。在五个实验中,我们研究了情感后果(AC)对SRBR效应的调节作用,以测试绑定/检索是否可以解释工具学习(即“效应定律”)。SRBR效应是在顺序主探针设计中评估的,反应关系(反应重复与变化)与干扰物关系(单词重复与改变)的正交变化。结合/检索效果是通过两个因素的相互作用来衡量的,干扰物的重复会导致对主要事件的检索,并有重新产生先前反应的趋势,这在必须重复反应的情况下会导致便利,但在所需反应从主要变为探测时会导致干扰。在每次试验后,都会发生阳性、中性或阴性事件,表明与货币收益或损失相关的点发生变化,以调查AC是否调节结合/回收效应。在所有五个实验中(总共N=466),一致地获得了强大的SRBR效应,但我们没有发现任何证据表明这些结合/检索效应的情感调节,表明这些效应是自动的,并且独立于AC。特别是,实验5证明了通过AC的工具学习(反映在较高频率的奖励反应中)和SRBR之间的分离,SRBR在反应后不受AC的影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Nothing else matters: Stimulus-response binding and retrieval is independent of affective consequences.","authors":"Juhi Parmar, Klaus Rothermund","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001288","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001288","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Stimulus-response binding and retrieval (SRBR) is a fundamental mechanism driving behavior automatization. In five experiments, we investigated the modulatory role of affective consequences (AC) on SRBR effects to test whether binding/retrieval can explain instrumental learning (i.e., the \"law of effect\"). SRBR effects were assessed in a sequential prime-probe design, with an orthogonal variation of response relation (response repetition vs. change) by distractor relation (word repetition vs. change). Binding/retrieval effects are measured by an interaction of the two factors, with distractor repetitions inducing a retrieval of the prime episode and a tendency to re-enact the previous response, which leads to facilitation in conditions where the response must be repeated, but leads to interference when the required response changes from prime to probe. Positive, neutral, or negative events signaling changes in points that were linked to monetary gains or losses were delivered after every trial to investigate whether AC modulate the binding/retrieval effect. Consistently across all five experiments (total N= 466), robust SRBR effects were obtained, but we did not find any evidence for an affective modulation of these binding/retrieval effects, indicating that these effects are automatic and independent of AC. In particular, Experiment 5 demonstrated a dissociation between instrumental learning via AC (reflected in higher frequencies of rewarded responses) and SRBR, which was not influenced by AC following a response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41179142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1