Pub Date : 2022-12-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317
Cheng Wenwen, Yan Yang, Liyan Cui, Ying Chen, Weiguo Zhang, Xiong Zhang, Shu Zhou
Background: Whether the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by an activation or preparation account is unclear.
Objective: To investigate the account of the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs.
Method: We used a cued task-switching paradigm with three switch probabilities (high, 75%; medium, 50%; and low, 25%) with 19 healthy young adults and recorded the cue- and target-locked event-related potentials (ERPs) and behavioral performance. The task included switch and stay trials under high, medium, and low switch conditions, as well as pure trials.
Results: There was no significant difference in reaction time (RT) in switch and mixing costs between the high and medium switch conditions. The RT in switch and mixing costs in the high and medium switch conditions was significantly less and more than in the low switch condition, respectively. The cue-locked ERPs revealed significant effects on mixing costs (stay - pure) that were cue early frontal positivity (260-300 ms) in the high and medium switch conditions, and on switch costs (switch - stay) that were cue early central positivity (240-260 ms) in the low switch condition. Moreover, the target-locked ERPs of the mixing costs revealed significant effects on mixing costs that were target P3b (440-540 ms) in all three switch conditions, and on switch costs that were target P3b in the medium and low switch conditions.
Conclusion: The effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by the activation account.
{"title":"The Account of the Effect of Switch Probability on Switch and Mixing Costs: An ERP Study in a Cued Task-switching Paradigm.","authors":"Cheng Wenwen, Yan Yang, Liyan Cui, Ying Chen, Weiguo Zhang, Xiong Zhang, Shu Zhou","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Whether the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by an activation or preparation account is unclear.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate the account of the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used a cued task-switching paradigm with three switch probabilities (high, 75%; medium, 50%; and low, 25%) with 19 healthy young adults and recorded the cue- and target-locked event-related potentials (ERPs) and behavioral performance. The task included switch and stay trials under high, medium, and low switch conditions, as well as pure trials.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was no significant difference in reaction time (RT) in switch and mixing costs between the high and medium switch conditions. The RT in switch and mixing costs in the high and medium switch conditions was significantly less and more than in the low switch condition, respectively. The cue-locked ERPs revealed significant effects on mixing costs (stay - pure) that were cue early frontal positivity (260-300 ms) in the high and medium switch conditions, and on switch costs (switch - stay) that were cue early central positivity (240-260 ms) in the low switch condition. Moreover, the target-locked ERPs of the mixing costs revealed significant effects on mixing costs that were target P3b (440-540 ms) in all three switch conditions, and on switch costs that were target P3b in the medium and low switch conditions.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by the activation account.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"230-246"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10633172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-12-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318
Natascia De Lucia, Michele Ragno, Cristina Paci, Gabriella Cacchiò, Anna Rita Caiazzo, Sara Tiberi, Anna De Rosa, Riccardo Navarra, Massimo Caulo, Giuseppe De Michele, Luigi Trojano
Background: Cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a monogenic small-vessel disease that is characterized by a wide range of neurologic and neuropsychological impairments. Constructional impairments have been reported in some cases but have never been assessed systematically.
Objective: To evaluate constructional abilities and their cognitive and neural correlates in nondemented individuals with CADASIL.
Method: Thirty individuals with CADASIL who were not affected by clinically relevant cognitive deterioration and 30 healthy controls (HC) underwent an extensive cognitive assessment and paper-and-pencil visuoconstructional tasks in order to detect constructional impairments. Performance on the visuoconstructional tasks was correlated with the cognitive assessment scores and with quantitative indices of regional gray matter atrophy (obtained via FreeSurfer image analysis) and white matter involvement.
Results: The individuals with CADASIL achieved significantly lower scores on the cognitive assessment compared with the HC. Poor visuoconstructional abilities were observed in seven (23.3%) of the individuals with CADASIL when performing the copy drawing task and in nine (30%) when performing the Rey Complex Figure Test. Logistic regression revealed that visuoconstructional impairments were significantly associated with scores on the Frontal Assessment Battery and the Attentional Matrices Test. Morphometric results revealed that scores on the visuoconstructional tasks were related to gray matter atrophy of the left frontal lobe and right parietal lobe.
Conclusion: Impairments on visuoconstructional tasks are quite common in individuals with CADASIL, even in the lack of clinically relevant cognitive deterioration, and are critically related to frontal and parietal atrophy.
{"title":"Constructional Impairments and Their Neural Correlates in Nondemented Adults With Cerebral Autosomal-dominant Arteriopathy With Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy.","authors":"Natascia De Lucia, Michele Ragno, Cristina Paci, Gabriella Cacchiò, Anna Rita Caiazzo, Sara Tiberi, Anna De Rosa, Riccardo Navarra, Massimo Caulo, Giuseppe De Michele, Luigi Trojano","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a monogenic small-vessel disease that is characterized by a wide range of neurologic and neuropsychological impairments. Constructional impairments have been reported in some cases but have never been assessed systematically.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To evaluate constructional abilities and their cognitive and neural correlates in nondemented individuals with CADASIL.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty individuals with CADASIL who were not affected by clinically relevant cognitive deterioration and 30 healthy controls (HC) underwent an extensive cognitive assessment and paper-and-pencil visuoconstructional tasks in order to detect constructional impairments. Performance on the visuoconstructional tasks was correlated with the cognitive assessment scores and with quantitative indices of regional gray matter atrophy (obtained via FreeSurfer image analysis) and white matter involvement.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The individuals with CADASIL achieved significantly lower scores on the cognitive assessment compared with the HC. Poor visuoconstructional abilities were observed in seven (23.3%) of the individuals with CADASIL when performing the copy drawing task and in nine (30%) when performing the Rey Complex Figure Test. Logistic regression revealed that visuoconstructional impairments were significantly associated with scores on the Frontal Assessment Battery and the Attentional Matrices Test. Morphometric results revealed that scores on the visuoconstructional tasks were related to gray matter atrophy of the left frontal lobe and right parietal lobe.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Impairments on visuoconstructional tasks are quite common in individuals with CADASIL, even in the lack of clinically relevant cognitive deterioration, and are critically related to frontal and parietal atrophy.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"247-254"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9199164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-12-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323
Howard S Kirshner
{"title":"Memory Loss, Alzheimer's Disease, and Dementia: A Practical Guide for Clinicians, 3rd ed.","authors":"Howard S Kirshner","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"298-299"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-09-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312
Vidushi Sinha, Frances Lissemore, Alan J Lerner
Background: Semantic category fluency is a widely used task involving language, memory, and executive function. Previous studies of bilingual semantic fluency have shown only small differences between languages. Graph theory analyzes complex relationships in networks, including node and edge number, clustering coefficient, average path length, average number of direct neighbors, and scale-free and small-world properties.
Objective: To shed light on whether the underlying neural processes involved in semantic category fluency testing yield substantially different networks in different languages.
Method: We compared languages and methods using both network analysis and conventional analysis of word production. We administered the animal naming task to 51 Russian-English bilinguals in each language. We constructed network graphs using three methods: (a) simple association of unique co-occurring neighbors, (b) corrected associations between consecutive words occurring beyond chance, and (c) a network community approach using planar maximally filtered graphs. We compared the resultant network analytics as well as their scale-free and small-world properties.
Results: Participants produced more words in Russian than in English. Small-worldness metrics were variable between Russian and English but were consistent across the three graph theory analytical methods.
Conclusion: The networks had similar graph theory properties in both languages. The optimal methodology for creating networks from semantic category fluency remains to be determined.
{"title":"Graph Theory Analysis of Semantic Fluency in Russian-English Bilinguals.","authors":"Vidushi Sinha, Frances Lissemore, Alan J Lerner","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Semantic category fluency is a widely used task involving language, memory, and executive function. Previous studies of bilingual semantic fluency have shown only small differences between languages. Graph theory analyzes complex relationships in networks, including node and edge number, clustering coefficient, average path length, average number of direct neighbors, and scale-free and small-world properties.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To shed light on whether the underlying neural processes involved in semantic category fluency testing yield substantially different networks in different languages.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We compared languages and methods using both network analysis and conventional analysis of word production. We administered the animal naming task to 51 Russian-English bilinguals in each language. We constructed network graphs using three methods: (a) simple association of unique co-occurring neighbors, (b) corrected associations between consecutive words occurring beyond chance, and (c) a network community approach using planar maximally filtered graphs. We compared the resultant network analytics as well as their scale-free and small-world properties.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants produced more words in Russian than in English. Small-worldness metrics were variable between Russian and English but were consistent across the three graph theory analytical methods.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The networks had similar graph theory properties in both languages. The optimal methodology for creating networks from semantic category fluency remains to be determined.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"179-187"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10154949/pdf/nihms-1814366.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10130340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-09-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311
August M Price, Rocco Palumbo, Anna Marin, Prayerna Uppal, Cheongmin Suh, Andrew E Budson, Katherine W Turk
Background: Individuals with probable Alzheimer disease (AD) may perform below cutoffs on traditional, memory-based performance validity tests. Previous studies have found success using event-related potentials (ERPs) to detect feigned neurocognitive impairment in younger populations.
Objective: To evaluate the utility of an auditory oddball task in conjunction with the P3b peak amplitude to distinguish probable AD from simulated dementia.
Method: Twenty individuals with probable AD and 20 older healthy controls (HC) underwent an ERP auditory oddball protocol and the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). The HC were asked to perform honestly for one condition and to simulate dementia for the other. The individuals with probable AD were asked to perform honestly. The P3b peak amplitude and button press accuracy were collected from each participant and were analyzed to determine their effectiveness in detecting performance validity.
Results: The P3b peak amplitude remained stable regardless of behavioral condition in the HC group. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude further improved the ability to correctly differentiate individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia with 100% sensitivity and 90% specificity.
Conclusion: The P3b peak amplitude was found to be an effective physiologic measure of cognitive impairment in individuals with probable AD compared with HC simulating dementia. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude served as a promising performance validity measure for differentiating individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia.
{"title":"Distinguishing Between Genuine and Feigned Dementia Using Event-related Potentials.","authors":"August M Price, Rocco Palumbo, Anna Marin, Prayerna Uppal, Cheongmin Suh, Andrew E Budson, Katherine W Turk","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Individuals with probable Alzheimer disease (AD) may perform below cutoffs on traditional, memory-based performance validity tests. Previous studies have found success using event-related potentials (ERPs) to detect feigned neurocognitive impairment in younger populations.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To evaluate the utility of an auditory oddball task in conjunction with the P3b peak amplitude to distinguish probable AD from simulated dementia.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Twenty individuals with probable AD and 20 older healthy controls (HC) underwent an ERP auditory oddball protocol and the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). The HC were asked to perform honestly for one condition and to simulate dementia for the other. The individuals with probable AD were asked to perform honestly. The P3b peak amplitude and button press accuracy were collected from each participant and were analyzed to determine their effectiveness in detecting performance validity.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The P3b peak amplitude remained stable regardless of behavioral condition in the HC group. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude further improved the ability to correctly differentiate individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia with 100% sensitivity and 90% specificity.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The P3b peak amplitude was found to be an effective physiologic measure of cognitive impairment in individuals with probable AD compared with HC simulating dementia. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude served as a promising performance validity measure for differentiating individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"188-197"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9444996/pdf/nihms-1813003.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10505581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-09-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304
Kaitlyn Abeare, Laura Cutler, Kelly Y An, Parveen Razvi, Matthew Holcomb, Laszlo A Erdodi
Background: Abbreviated neurocognitive tests offer a practical alternative to full-length versions but often lack clear interpretive guidelines, thereby limiting their clinical utility.
Objective: To replicate validity cutoffs for the Boston Naming Test-Short Form (BNT-15) and to introduce a clinical classification system for the BNT-15 as a measure of object-naming skills.
Method: We collected data from 43 university students and 46 clinical patients. Classification accuracy was computed against psychometrically defined criterion groups. Clinical classification ranges were developed using a z -score transformation.
Results: Previously suggested validity cutoffs (≤11 and ≤12) produced comparable classification accuracy among the university students. However, a more conservative cutoff (≤10) was needed with the clinical patients to contain the false-positive rate (0.20-0.38 sensitivity at 0.92-0.96 specificity). As a measure of cognitive ability, a perfect BNT-15 score suggests above average performance; ≤11 suggests clinically significant deficits. Demographically adjusted prorated BNT-15 T-scores correlated strongly (0.86) with the newly developed z -scores.
Conclusion: Given its brevity (<5 minutes), ease of administration and scoring, the BNT-15 can function as a useful and cost-effective screening measure for both object-naming/English proficiency and performance validity. The proposed clinical classification ranges provide useful guidelines for practitioners.
{"title":"BNT-15: Revised Performance Validity Cutoffs and Proposed Clinical Classification Ranges.","authors":"Kaitlyn Abeare, Laura Cutler, Kelly Y An, Parveen Razvi, Matthew Holcomb, Laszlo A Erdodi","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Abbreviated neurocognitive tests offer a practical alternative to full-length versions but often lack clear interpretive guidelines, thereby limiting their clinical utility.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To replicate validity cutoffs for the Boston Naming Test-Short Form (BNT-15) and to introduce a clinical classification system for the BNT-15 as a measure of object-naming skills.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected data from 43 university students and 46 clinical patients. Classification accuracy was computed against psychometrically defined criterion groups. Clinical classification ranges were developed using a z -score transformation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Previously suggested validity cutoffs (≤11 and ≤12) produced comparable classification accuracy among the university students. However, a more conservative cutoff (≤10) was needed with the clinical patients to contain the false-positive rate (0.20-0.38 sensitivity at 0.92-0.96 specificity). As a measure of cognitive ability, a perfect BNT-15 score suggests above average performance; ≤11 suggests clinically significant deficits. Demographically adjusted prorated BNT-15 T-scores correlated strongly (0.86) with the newly developed z -scores.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Given its brevity (<5 minutes), ease of administration and scoring, the BNT-15 can function as a useful and cost-effective screening measure for both object-naming/English proficiency and performance validity. The proposed clinical classification ranges provide useful guidelines for practitioners.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"155-168"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41118587","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-06-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296
Tuğberk Andaç Topkan, Nesrin Erdoğan, Berrak Barutcu, E. Cindil, E. T. Tali, Y. Karaman
Background: Quantitative MRI assessment methods have limited utility due to a lack of standardized methods and measures for Alzheimer disease (AD) and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Objective: To employ a relatively new and easy-to-use quantitative assessment method to reveal volumetric changes in subcortical gray matter (GM) regions, hippocampus, and global intracranial structures as well as the diagnostic performance and best thresholds of total hippocampal volumetry in individuals with AD and those with aMCI. Method: A total of 74 individuals—37 with mild to moderate AD, 19 with aMCI, and 18 with normal cognition (NC)—underwent a 3T MRI. Fully automated segmentation and volumetric measurements were performed. Results: The AD and aMCI groups had smaller volumes of amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and hippocampus compared with the NC group. These same two groups had significantly smaller total white matter volume than the NC group. The AD group had smaller total GM volume compared with the aMCI and NC groups. The thalamus in the AD group showed a subtle atrophy. There were no significant volumetric differences in the caudate nucleus, putamen, or globus pallidus between the groups. Conclusion: The amygdala and nucleus accumbens showed atrophy comparable to the hippocampal atrophy in both the AD and aMCI groups, which may contribute to cognitive impairment. Hippocampal volumetry is a reliable tool for differentiating between AD and NC groups but has substantially less power in differentiating between AD and aMCI groups. The loss of total GM volume differentiates AD from aMCI and NC.
{"title":"Volumetric Assessment of Hippocampus and Subcortical Gray Matter Regions in Alzheimer Disease and Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment","authors":"Tuğberk Andaç Topkan, Nesrin Erdoğan, Berrak Barutcu, E. Cindil, E. T. Tali, Y. Karaman","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296","url":null,"abstract":"Background: Quantitative MRI assessment methods have limited utility due to a lack of standardized methods and measures for Alzheimer disease (AD) and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Objective: To employ a relatively new and easy-to-use quantitative assessment method to reveal volumetric changes in subcortical gray matter (GM) regions, hippocampus, and global intracranial structures as well as the diagnostic performance and best thresholds of total hippocampal volumetry in individuals with AD and those with aMCI. Method: A total of 74 individuals—37 with mild to moderate AD, 19 with aMCI, and 18 with normal cognition (NC)—underwent a 3T MRI. Fully automated segmentation and volumetric measurements were performed. Results: The AD and aMCI groups had smaller volumes of amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and hippocampus compared with the NC group. These same two groups had significantly smaller total white matter volume than the NC group. The AD group had smaller total GM volume compared with the aMCI and NC groups. The thalamus in the AD group showed a subtle atrophy. There were no significant volumetric differences in the caudate nucleus, putamen, or globus pallidus between the groups. Conclusion: The amygdala and nucleus accumbens showed atrophy comparable to the hippocampal atrophy in both the AD and aMCI groups, which may contribute to cognitive impairment. Hippocampal volumetry is a reliable tool for differentiating between AD and NC groups but has substantially less power in differentiating between AD and aMCI groups. The loss of total GM volume differentiates AD from aMCI and NC.","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 1","pages":"95 - 103"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41437948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-06-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000299
P. Caroppo, C. Villa, A. Del Sole, G. Bernardi, S. Carradori, P. Tiraboschi, G. Giaccone, Sara Prioni
We present the case of a man exhibiting a clinical phenotype of behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). The man had developed psychiatric disturbances with verbal aggressiveness over a few months, followed by cognitive and frontal behavioral disorders, fulfilling the clinical criteria for bvFTD. Atrophy and hypometabolism in frontotemporal regions were consistent with the diagnosis. However, serum-screening exams for syphilis infection were positive, and CSF analysis, despite a negative Venereal Disease Research Laboratory Test, suggested the diagnosis of neurosyphilis. After specific antibiotic therapy, the man’s behavioral abnormalities and cognitive deficits notably improved, confirming neurosyphilis as the cause of the clinical phenotype. The cognitive deficits completely recovered 1 year post therapy and remained stable for 2 years. After ∼2½ years from the first treatment, the man’s behavioral disorders mildly worsened, at which time we re-evaluated him. His cognition was stable, and a positive Venereal Disease Research Laboratory Test confirmed the diagnosis of neurosyphilis. With this case, we demonstrated that in some instances, neurosyphilis can mimic frontotemporal dementia. As a cause of treatable dementia, it should be considered in the differential diagnosis of bvFTD, particularly when psychiatric symptoms and a rapid cognitive decline are noted, even in the presence of brain atrophy and/or hypometabolism.
{"title":"Neurosyphilis Mimicking Behavioral Variant of Frontotemporal Dementia in a 59-Year-Old Man","authors":"P. Caroppo, C. Villa, A. Del Sole, G. Bernardi, S. Carradori, P. Tiraboschi, G. Giaccone, Sara Prioni","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000299","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000299","url":null,"abstract":"We present the case of a man exhibiting a clinical phenotype of behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). The man had developed psychiatric disturbances with verbal aggressiveness over a few months, followed by cognitive and frontal behavioral disorders, fulfilling the clinical criteria for bvFTD. Atrophy and hypometabolism in frontotemporal regions were consistent with the diagnosis. However, serum-screening exams for syphilis infection were positive, and CSF analysis, despite a negative Venereal Disease Research Laboratory Test, suggested the diagnosis of neurosyphilis. After specific antibiotic therapy, the man’s behavioral abnormalities and cognitive deficits notably improved, confirming neurosyphilis as the cause of the clinical phenotype. The cognitive deficits completely recovered 1 year post therapy and remained stable for 2 years. After ∼2½ years from the first treatment, the man’s behavioral disorders mildly worsened, at which time we re-evaluated him. His cognition was stable, and a positive Venereal Disease Research Laboratory Test confirmed the diagnosis of neurosyphilis. With this case, we demonstrated that in some instances, neurosyphilis can mimic frontotemporal dementia. As a cause of treatable dementia, it should be considered in the differential diagnosis of bvFTD, particularly when psychiatric symptoms and a rapid cognitive decline are noted, even in the presence of brain atrophy and/or hypometabolism.","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 1","pages":"140 - 146"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42177080","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-06-01DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000297
M. Mendez, Youssef I. Khattab, Oleg Yerstein
Background: Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a progressive neurologic syndrome that presents with complex visual deficits. Although PCA is most commonly a form of Alzheimer disease (AD), its early diagnosis is usually delayed due to a lack of understanding for how best to clinically screen for the syndrome. Objective: To identify neurobehavioral screening tasks for PCA—beyond simple visual constructions—that can be administered in clinic or at bedside. Method: We compared the performance of 12 individuals who met neuroimaging-supported consensus criteria for PCA with that of 12 matched individuals with typical AD (tAD) and 24 healthy controls (HC) on clinic/bedside tasks measuring (a) complex figure copying, (b) Balint syndrome, (c) visual object agnosia, (d) color identification, (e) figure–ground discrimination, (f) global–local processing, (g) dressing apraxia, (h) ideomotor apraxia, and (i) Gerstmann syndrome. Results: All of the individuals with PCA were impaired on the figure–ground discrimination task compared with half of the tAD group and no HC. Approximately half of the PCA group had Balint syndrome, dressing apraxia, and ideomotor apraxia compared with none in the tAD group. Difficulty copying a complex figure, global–local processing impairment, and Gerstmann syndrome did not distinguish between the two dementia groups. Conclusion: The figure–ground discrimination task can be used successfully as an overall screening measure for PCA, followed by specific tasks for Balint syndrome and dressing and limb apraxia. Findings reinforce PCA as a predominant occipitoparietal disorder with dorsal visual stream involvement and parietal signs with spatiomotor impairments.
{"title":"Clinical Screening for Posterior Cortical Atrophy","authors":"M. Mendez, Youssef I. Khattab, Oleg Yerstein","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000297","url":null,"abstract":"Background: Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a progressive neurologic syndrome that presents with complex visual deficits. Although PCA is most commonly a form of Alzheimer disease (AD), its early diagnosis is usually delayed due to a lack of understanding for how best to clinically screen for the syndrome. Objective: To identify neurobehavioral screening tasks for PCA—beyond simple visual constructions—that can be administered in clinic or at bedside. Method: We compared the performance of 12 individuals who met neuroimaging-supported consensus criteria for PCA with that of 12 matched individuals with typical AD (tAD) and 24 healthy controls (HC) on clinic/bedside tasks measuring (a) complex figure copying, (b) Balint syndrome, (c) visual object agnosia, (d) color identification, (e) figure–ground discrimination, (f) global–local processing, (g) dressing apraxia, (h) ideomotor apraxia, and (i) Gerstmann syndrome. Results: All of the individuals with PCA were impaired on the figure–ground discrimination task compared with half of the tAD group and no HC. Approximately half of the PCA group had Balint syndrome, dressing apraxia, and ideomotor apraxia compared with none in the tAD group. Difficulty copying a complex figure, global–local processing impairment, and Gerstmann syndrome did not distinguish between the two dementia groups. Conclusion: The figure–ground discrimination task can be used successfully as an overall screening measure for PCA, followed by specific tasks for Balint syndrome and dressing and limb apraxia. Findings reinforce PCA as a predominant occipitoparietal disorder with dorsal visual stream involvement and parietal signs with spatiomotor impairments.","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 1","pages":"104 - 109"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41891837","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-04-27DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000307
Kasra Hendi, Mohamad Rahmani, Amirhossein Larijani, Hamideh Ajam Zibadi, S. Raminfard, R. Shariat Moharari, V. Gerganov, Maysam Alimohamadi
Background: Dominant-hemisphere tumors, especially gliomas, as infiltrative tumors, frequently affect cognitive functioning. Establishing a balance between extensive resection, which is proven to result in longer survival, and less extensive resection, in order to maintain more cognitive abilities, is challenging. Objective: To evaluate changes in cognitive functioning before and after surgical resection of language-related, eloquent-area, high-grade gliomas under awake craniotomy. Method: We provided individuals with newly diagnosed high-grade gliomas of the language-related eloquent areas with the same standard of care, including surgical resection of the glioma using intraoperative sensory-motor and cognitive mapping under awake craniotomy, and the same protocol for chemoradiotherapy. Cognitive functioning was assessed using Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised (ACE–R) at four time points (preoperatively, early after surgery, and 3 and 6 months postoperatively). Results: The preoperative evaluation revealed a range of cognitive impairments in 70.7% of the individuals, affecting all of the cognitive subdomains (mostly attention and visuospatial abilities). Overall cognitive functioning (ie, ACE–R score) dropped by 13.5% (P = 0.169) early postoperatively. At the 3-month evaluation, an average of 15.3% (P = 0.182) recovery in cognitive functioning was observed (mostly in verbal fluency: 39.1%). This recovery improved further, reaching 29% (P < 0.001) at the 6-month evaluation. The greatest improvement occurred in verbal fluency: 68.8%, P = 0.001. Conclusion: Extensive resection of eloquent-area gliomas with the aid of modern neuroimaging and neuromonitoring techniques under awake craniotomy is possible without significant long-term cognitive sequela.
{"title":"Changes in Cognitive Functioning After Surgical Resection of Language-related, Eloquent-area, High-grade Gliomas Under Awake Craniotomy","authors":"Kasra Hendi, Mohamad Rahmani, Amirhossein Larijani, Hamideh Ajam Zibadi, S. Raminfard, R. Shariat Moharari, V. Gerganov, Maysam Alimohamadi","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000307","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000307","url":null,"abstract":"Background: Dominant-hemisphere tumors, especially gliomas, as infiltrative tumors, frequently affect cognitive functioning. Establishing a balance between extensive resection, which is proven to result in longer survival, and less extensive resection, in order to maintain more cognitive abilities, is challenging. Objective: To evaluate changes in cognitive functioning before and after surgical resection of language-related, eloquent-area, high-grade gliomas under awake craniotomy. Method: We provided individuals with newly diagnosed high-grade gliomas of the language-related eloquent areas with the same standard of care, including surgical resection of the glioma using intraoperative sensory-motor and cognitive mapping under awake craniotomy, and the same protocol for chemoradiotherapy. Cognitive functioning was assessed using Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised (ACE–R) at four time points (preoperatively, early after surgery, and 3 and 6 months postoperatively). Results: The preoperative evaluation revealed a range of cognitive impairments in 70.7% of the individuals, affecting all of the cognitive subdomains (mostly attention and visuospatial abilities). Overall cognitive functioning (ie, ACE–R score) dropped by 13.5% (P = 0.169) early postoperatively. At the 3-month evaluation, an average of 15.3% (P = 0.182) recovery in cognitive functioning was observed (mostly in verbal fluency: 39.1%). This recovery improved further, reaching 29% (P < 0.001) at the 6-month evaluation. The greatest improvement occurred in verbal fluency: 68.8%, P = 0.001. Conclusion: Extensive resection of eloquent-area gliomas with the aid of modern neuroimaging and neuromonitoring techniques under awake craniotomy is possible without significant long-term cognitive sequela.","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 1","pages":"130 - 139"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48462467","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}