首页 > 最新文献

Consciousness and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Implicit visuospatial sequence representations are accessible in both the practice and the transfer hand 练习手和转移手均可获得隐性视觉空间序列表征
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103696
Stephan F. Dahm, Markus Martini, Pierre Sachse

A serial reaction time task was used to test whether the representations of a probabilistic second-order sequence structure are (i) stored in an effector-dependent, effector-independent intrinsic or effector-independent visuospatial code and (ii) are inter-manually accessible. Participants were trained either with the dominant or non-dominant hand. Tests were performed with both hands in the practice sequence, a random sequence, and a mirror sequence. Learning did not differ significantly between left and right-hand practice, suggesting symmetric intermanual transfer from the dominant to the non-dominant hand and vice versa. In the posttest, RTs were shorter for the practice sequence than for the random sequence, and longest for the mirror sequence. Participants were unable to freely generate or recognize the practice sequence, indicating implicit knowledge of the probabilistic sequence structure. Because sequence-specific learning did not differ significantly between hands, we conclude that representations of the probabilistic sequence structure are stored in an effector-independent visuospatial code.

我们使用了一项序列反应时间任务来测试概率二阶序列结构的表征是否(i)存储在依赖于效应器的、不依赖于效应器的内在视觉空间代码中,或(ii)可在人与人之间进行存取。参赛者使用惯用手或非惯用手进行训练。测试在练习序列、随机序列和镜像序列中使用双手进行。左手和右手练习的学习效果没有明显差异,这表明惯用手和非惯用手之间的动作迁移是对称的。在后测中,练习序列的反应时间比随机序列短,而镜像序列的反应时间最长。参与者无法自由生成或识别练习序列,这表明他们对概率序列结构有内隐知识。由于序列特异性学习在不同手之间没有显著差异,我们得出结论,概率序列结构的表征存储在与效应器无关的视觉空间代码中。
{"title":"Implicit visuospatial sequence representations are accessible in both the practice and the transfer hand","authors":"Stephan F. Dahm,&nbsp;Markus Martini,&nbsp;Pierre Sachse","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103696","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103696","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A serial reaction time task was used to test whether the representations of a probabilistic second-order sequence structure are (i) stored in an effector-dependent, effector-independent intrinsic or effector-independent visuospatial code and (ii) are inter-manually accessible. Participants were trained either with the dominant or non-dominant hand. Tests were performed with both hands in the practice sequence, a random sequence, and a mirror sequence. Learning did not differ significantly between left and right-hand practice, suggesting symmetric intermanual transfer from the dominant to the non-dominant hand and vice versa. In the posttest, RTs were shorter for the practice sequence than for the random sequence, and longest for the mirror sequence. Participants were unable to freely generate or recognize the practice sequence, indicating implicit knowledge of the probabilistic sequence structure. Because sequence-specific learning did not differ significantly between hands, we conclude that representations of the probabilistic sequence structure are stored in an effector-independent visuospatial code.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000631/pdfft?md5=ff153eb926d2878753bfb13918926259&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000631-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140823613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Slower but more accurate mental rotation performance in aphantasia linked to differences in cognitive strategies 幻觉症患者的思维旋转速度较慢但更准确,这与认知策略的差异有关
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103694
Lachlan Kay , Rebecca Keogh , Joel Pearson

Mental rotation tasks are frequently used as standard measures of mental imagery. However, aphantasia research has brought such use into question. Here, we assessed a large group of individuals who lack visual imagery (aphantasia) on two mental rotation tasks: a three-dimensional block-shape, and a human manikin rotation task. In both tasks, those with aphantasia had slower, but more accurate responses than controls. Both groups demonstrated classic linear increases in response time and error-rate as functions of angular disparity. In the three-dimensional block-shape rotation task, a within-group speed-accuracy trade-off was found in controls, whereas faster individuals in the aphantasia group were also more accurate. Control participants generally favoured using object-based mental rotation strategies, whereas those with aphantasia favoured analytic strategies. These results suggest that visual imagery is not crucial for successful performance in classical mental rotation tasks, as alternative strategies can be effectively utilised in the absence of holistic mental representations.

心理旋转任务经常被用作心理想象的标准测量方法。然而,"象觉失调症 "的研究却对这种使用方式提出了质疑。在这里,我们对一大群缺乏视觉意象(象患)的人进行了两项心理旋转任务的评估:一项是三维块状物体旋转任务,另一项是人体模型旋转任务。在这两项任务中,与对照组相比,患有幻觉症的人反应更慢,但更准确。两组患者的反应时间和错误率都呈现出典型的线性增长,与角度差距成函数关系。在三维块状旋转任务中,对照组发现了组内速度与准确性的权衡,而象患组的反应速度更快,准确性也更高。对照组受试者通常更倾向于使用基于物体的心理旋转策略,而那些患有象障的受试者则更倾向于使用分析策略。这些结果表明,在经典的心智旋转任务中,视觉意象并不是成功的关键,因为在缺乏整体心智表征的情况下,可以有效地利用其他策略。
{"title":"Slower but more accurate mental rotation performance in aphantasia linked to differences in cognitive strategies","authors":"Lachlan Kay ,&nbsp;Rebecca Keogh ,&nbsp;Joel Pearson","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103694","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103694","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mental rotation tasks are frequently used as standard measures of mental imagery. However, aphantasia research has brought such use into question. Here, we assessed a large group of individuals who lack visual imagery (aphantasia) on two mental rotation tasks: a three-dimensional block-shape, and a human manikin rotation task. In both tasks, those with aphantasia had slower, but more accurate responses than controls. Both groups demonstrated classic linear increases in response time and error-rate as functions of angular disparity. In the three-dimensional block-shape rotation task, a within-group speed-accuracy trade-off was found in controls, whereas faster individuals in the aphantasia group were also more accurate. Control participants generally favoured using object-based mental rotation strategies, whereas those with aphantasia favoured analytic strategies. These results suggest that visual imagery is not crucial for successful performance in classical mental rotation tasks, as alternative strategies can be effectively utilised in the absence of holistic mental representations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000618/pdfft?md5=a2a0a0790233865847c767ea2ba2c07e&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000618-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140632589","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prime-induced illusion of control: The influence of unconscious priming on self-initiated actions and the role of regression to the mean 引物诱发的控制幻觉:无意识引物对自发行动的影响以及回归均值的作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103684
Fabian Kiepe, Guido Hesselmann

To what degree human cognition is influenced by subliminal stimuli is a controversial empirical question. One striking example was reported by Linser and Goschke (2007): participants overestimated how much control they had over objectively uncontrollable stimuli when masked congruent primes were presented immediately before the action. Critically, however, unawareness of the masked primes was established by post hoc data selection. In our preregistered study we sought to explore these findings while adjusting prime visibility based on individual thresholds, so that each participant underwent both visible and non-visible conditions. In experiment 1, N = 39 participants engaged in a control judgement task: following the presentation of a semantic prime, they freely selected between two keys, which triggered the appearance of a colored circle. The color of the circles, however, was independent of the key-press. Subsequently, participants assessed their perceived control over the circle’s color, based on their key-presses, via a rating scale that ranged from 0 % (no control) to 100 % (complete control). Contrary to Linser and Goschke (2007)'s findings, this experiment demonstrated that predictive information influenced the experience of agency only when primes were consciously processed. In experiment 2, utilizing symbolic (arrow) primes, N = 35 participants had to rate their feeling of control over the effect-stimulus’ identity during a two-choice identification paradigm (i.e., they were instructed to press a key corresponding to a target stimulus; with a contingency between target and effect stimulus of 75 %/25 %). The results revealed no significant influence of subliminal priming on agency perceptions. In summary, this study implies that unconscious stimuli may not exert a substantial influence on the conscious experience of agency, underscoring the need for careful consideration of methodological aspects and experimental design's impact on observed phenomena.

人类认知在多大程度上受潜意识刺激的影响是一个有争议的经验问题。Linser 和 Goschke(2007 年)报告了一个令人震惊的例子:当被试者在行动前立即出现被遮蔽的同义预示时,他们高估了自己对客观上无法控制的刺激的控制程度。然而,关键的一点是,通过事后数据选择,受试者并没有意识到被遮蔽的原素。在预先登记的研究中,我们试图探索这些发现,同时根据个人阈值调整素材的可见性,让每个参与者都经历可见和不可见两种条件。在实验 1 中,N = 39 名参与者参与了一项控制判断任务:在呈现语义质点后,他们在两个按键之间自由选择,这两个按键会触发一个彩色圆圈的出现。然而,圆圈的颜色与按键无关。随后,受试者根据自己的按键情况,通过从0%(无控制)到100%(完全控制)的评分表,评估自己对圆圈颜色的感知控制能力。与 Linser 和 Goschke(2007 年)的研究结果相反,该实验表明,只有在有意识地处理预设信息时,预设信息才会影响代理体验。在实验 2 中,利用符号(箭头)预设,N=35 名参与者必须在二选一的识别范式(即他们被指示按下与目标刺激相对应的按键;目标刺激和效应刺激之间的或然率为 75%/25%)中评定他们对效应刺激身份的控制感。结果显示,潜意识引物对代理认知没有明显影响。总之,这项研究表明,无意识刺激可能不会对有意识的代理体验产生实质性影响,这强调了需要仔细考虑方法论方面和实验设计对观察到的现象的影响。
{"title":"Prime-induced illusion of control: The influence of unconscious priming on self-initiated actions and the role of regression to the mean","authors":"Fabian Kiepe,&nbsp;Guido Hesselmann","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103684","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103684","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To what degree human cognition is influenced by subliminal stimuli is a controversial empirical question. One striking example was reported by Linser and Goschke (2007): participants overestimated how much control they had over objectively uncontrollable stimuli when masked congruent primes were presented immediately before the action. Critically, however, unawareness of the masked primes was established by post hoc data selection. In our preregistered study we sought to explore these findings while adjusting prime visibility based on individual thresholds, so that each participant underwent both visible and non-visible conditions. In experiment 1, N = 39 participants engaged in a control judgement task: following the presentation of a semantic prime, they freely selected between two keys, which triggered the appearance of a colored circle. The color of the circles, however, was independent of the key-press. Subsequently, participants assessed their perceived control over the circle’s color, based on their key-presses, via a rating scale that ranged from 0 % (no control) to 100 % (complete control). Contrary to Linser and Goschke (2007)'s findings, this experiment demonstrated that predictive information influenced the experience of agency only when primes were consciously processed. In experiment 2, utilizing symbolic (arrow) primes, N = 35 participants had to rate their feeling of control over the effect-stimulus’ identity during a two-choice identification paradigm (i.e., they were instructed to press a key corresponding to a target stimulus; with a contingency between target and effect stimulus of 75 %/25 %). The results revealed no significant influence of subliminal priming on agency perceptions. In summary, this study implies that unconscious stimuli may not exert a substantial influence on the conscious experience of agency, underscoring the need for careful consideration of methodological aspects and experimental design's impact on observed phenomena.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140549782","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The involvement of decomposition and composition processes in restructuring during problem solving 在解决问题的过程中,分解和组成过程参与结构调整
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103685
Zhonglu Zhang , Yizhu Li , Yuxin Zeng , Jiamin Deng , Qiang Xing , Jing Luo

Decomposition of chunks has been widely accepted as a critical proxy of restructuring, but the role of composition in forming new representations has been largely neglected. This study aims to investigate the roles of both decomposition and composition processes in chunk restructuring, as well as their relationships with “aha” experiences during problem-solving. Participants were asked to move a part of a character to another character to create two new characters. Across three experiments, the characters to be decomposed or composed were varied in terms of tight or loose chunks. The results showed that decomposition or composition of tight chunks led to lower success rates, longer response times, and significantly stronger “Aha!” emotional experiences (mainly in terms of surprise and suddenness). This study provides evidence for the contribution of both decomposition and composition processes to restructuring in creative insight.

人们普遍认为语块的分解是重组的关键代表,但组成在形成新表征中的作用却在很大程度上被忽视了。本研究旨在探讨分解和组合过程在语块重组中的作用,以及它们与解决问题过程中的 "啊哈 "体验之间的关系。参与者被要求将一个字符的一部分移到另一个字符上,从而创建两个新字符。在三次实验中,被分解或组成的字符在紧块或松块方面各不相同。结果表明,分解或组成紧密块的成功率较低,反应时间较长,"啊哈!"情绪体验(主要表现在惊喜和突然性方面)明显较强。这项研究为分解和组合过程对创造性洞察力重组的贡献提供了证据。
{"title":"The involvement of decomposition and composition processes in restructuring during problem solving","authors":"Zhonglu Zhang ,&nbsp;Yizhu Li ,&nbsp;Yuxin Zeng ,&nbsp;Jiamin Deng ,&nbsp;Qiang Xing ,&nbsp;Jing Luo","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103685","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103685","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Decomposition of chunks has been widely accepted as a critical proxy of restructuring, but the role of composition in forming new representations has been largely neglected. This study aims to investigate the roles of both decomposition and composition processes in chunk restructuring, as well as their relationships with “aha” experiences during problem-solving. Participants were asked to move a part of a character to another character to create two new characters. Across three experiments, the characters to be decomposed or composed were varied in terms of tight or loose chunks. The results showed that decomposition or composition of tight chunks led to lower success rates, longer response times, and significantly stronger “Aha!” emotional experiences (mainly in terms of surprise and suddenness). This study provides evidence for the contribution of both decomposition and composition processes to restructuring in creative insight.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140540747","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Aphantasia and involuntary imagery 幻觉和不自主意象
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103679
Raquel Krempel , Merlin Monzel

Aphantasia is a condition that is often characterized as the impaired ability to create voluntary mental images. Aphantasia is assumed to selectively affect voluntary imagery mainly because even though aphantasics report being unable to visualize something at will, many report having visual dreams. We argue that this common characterization of aphantasia is incorrect. Studies on aphantasia are often not clear about whether they are assessing voluntary or involuntary imagery, but some studies show that several forms of involuntary imagery are also affected in aphantasia (including imagery in dreams). We also raise problems for two attempts to show that involuntary images are preserved in aphantasia. In addition, we report the results of a study about afterimages in aphantasia, which suggest that these tend to be less intense in aphantasics than in controls. Involuntary imagery is often treated as a unitary kind that is either present or absent in aphantasia. We suggest that this approach is mistaken and that we should look at different types of involuntary imagery case by case. Doing so reveals no evidence of preserved involuntary imagery in aphantasia. We suggest that a broader characterization of aphantasia, as a deficit in forming mental imagery, whether voluntary or not, is more appropriate. Characterizing aphantasia as a volitional deficit is likely to lead researchers to give incorrect explanations for aphantasia, and to look for the wrong mechanisms underlying it.

幻觉症是一种通常被描述为自主创造心理图像能力受损的疾病。人们之所以认为幻觉症会选择性地影响自主意象,主要是因为尽管幻觉症患者表示无法随意想象某些事物,但许多人却表示会做视觉梦。我们认为,这种对象质症的常见描述是不正确的。关于象觉失调症的研究通常并不清楚他们评估的是自主意象还是非自主意象,但一些研究表明,象觉失调症患者的几种非自主意象(包括梦中的意象)也会受到影响。我们还提出了两个试图证明非自主意象在象患中得以保留的问题。此外,我们还报告了一项关于象觉失调症患者余像的研究结果,结果表明象觉失调症患者的余像强度往往低于对照组。非自主意象通常被视为一种单一的意象,在象患中要么存在,要么不存在。我们认为这种观点是错误的,我们应该逐一研究不同类型的非自主意象。这样做没有发现任何证据表明非自主意象在失象症中得到了保留。我们认为,更恰当的做法是将象失症定性为心理意象(无论是否自愿)的形成障碍。将象觉障碍定性为一种意志缺陷很可能会导致研究人员对象觉障碍做出错误的解释,并寻找其背后的错误机制。
{"title":"Aphantasia and involuntary imagery","authors":"Raquel Krempel ,&nbsp;Merlin Monzel","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103679","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103679","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Aphantasia is a condition that is often characterized as the impaired ability to create <em>voluntary</em> mental images. Aphantasia is assumed to selectively affect voluntary imagery mainly because even though aphantasics report being unable to visualize something at will, many report having visual dreams. We argue that this common characterization of aphantasia is incorrect. Studies on aphantasia are often not clear about whether they are assessing voluntary or involuntary imagery, but some studies show that several forms of involuntary imagery are also affected in aphantasia (including imagery in dreams). We also raise problems for two attempts to show that involuntary images are preserved in aphantasia. In addition, we report the results of a study about afterimages in aphantasia, which suggest that these tend to be less intense in aphantasics than in controls. Involuntary imagery is often treated as a unitary kind that is either present or absent in aphantasia. We suggest that this approach is mistaken and that we should look at different types of involuntary imagery case by case. Doing so reveals no evidence of preserved involuntary imagery in aphantasia. We suggest that a broader characterization of aphantasia, as a deficit in forming mental imagery, whether voluntary or not, is more appropriate. Characterizing aphantasia as a volitional deficit is likely to lead researchers to give incorrect explanations for aphantasia, and to look for the wrong mechanisms underlying it.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140339884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Task relevance alters the effect of emotion on congruency judgments during action understanding 任务相关性会改变情绪对行动理解过程中一致性判断的影响
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103682
Yiheng Chen , Qiwei Zhao , Yueyi Ding , Yingzhi Lu

The congruency judgments in action understanding helps individuals make timely adjustments to unexpected occurrence, and this process may be influenced by emotion. Previous research has showed contradictory effect of emotion on conflict processing, possibly due to the degree of relevance between emotion and task. However, to date, no study has systematically manipulated the relevance to explore how emotion affects congruency judgments in action understanding. We employed a cue-target paradigm and controlled the way emotional stimuli were presented on the target interface, setting up three experiments: emotion served as task-irrelevant distractor, task-irrelevant target and task-relevant target. The results showed that when emotion was irrelevant to the task, it impaired congruency judgements performance, regardless of a distractor or a target, while task-relevant emotion facilitated this process. These findings indicate that the impact of emotion on congruency judgements during action understanding depends on the degree of emotion-task relevance.

行动理解中的一致性判断有助于个体对突发事件做出及时调整,而这一过程可能会受到情绪的影响。以往的研究表明,情绪对冲突处理的影响是相互矛盾的,这可能是由于情绪与任务之间的相关程度不同造成的。然而,迄今为止,还没有研究系统地操纵相关性来探讨情绪如何影响行动理解中的一致性判断。我们采用了线索-目标范式,并控制了情绪刺激在目标界面上的呈现方式,设置了三个实验:情绪作为任务无关的干扰物、任务无关的目标和任务相关的目标。结果表明,当情绪与任务无关时,不管是干扰物还是目标,都会影响一致性判断的表现,而与任务相关的情绪则会促进这一过程。这些研究结果表明,在动作理解过程中,情绪对一致性判断的影响取决于情绪与任务的相关程度。
{"title":"Task relevance alters the effect of emotion on congruency judgments during action understanding","authors":"Yiheng Chen ,&nbsp;Qiwei Zhao ,&nbsp;Yueyi Ding ,&nbsp;Yingzhi Lu","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103682","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103682","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The congruency judgments in action understanding helps individuals make timely adjustments to unexpected occurrence, and this process may be influenced by emotion. Previous research has showed contradictory effect of emotion on conflict processing, possibly due to the degree of relevance between emotion and task. However, to date, no study has systematically manipulated the relevance to explore how emotion affects congruency judgments in action understanding. We employed a cue-target paradigm and controlled the way emotional stimuli were presented on the target interface, setting up three experiments: emotion served as task-irrelevant distractor, task-irrelevant target and task-relevant target. The results showed that when emotion was irrelevant to the task, it impaired congruency judgements performance, regardless of a distractor or a target, while task-relevant emotion facilitated this process. These findings indicate that the impact of emotion on congruency judgements during action understanding depends on the degree of emotion-task relevance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140327772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct reciprocity and reputation shape trust decisions similarly in blind and sighted individuals 直接互惠和声誉对盲人和视力正常者的信任决策影响相似
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103683
Maria Arioli , Chiara Ferrari , Lotfi B. Merabet , Zaira Cattaneo

This study addresses the effects of blindness on trust. Using an auditory version of the multi-round Trust Game, we investigated the effect of reputation and reciprocity on trust decisions in early blind and sighted participants. During each round of the game, participants were endowed with a sum of money and had to decide how much they wanted to invest in their partners, who were manipulated as a function of their good or bad reputation and individualistic or cooperative behavior. The data showed that negative first impression about the partner (bad reputation and/or selfish behavior) impacted more blind participants than sighted ones. However, following repeated interactions with the partners, the overall mean investment aligned between the blind and sighted groups. We interpret these findings as suggesting that blindness may guide participants to a more cautionary behavior when dealing with partners with negative initial characteristics.

本研究探讨了失明对信任的影响。我们使用听觉版的多轮信任游戏,研究了声誉和互惠对早期失明者和视力正常者的信任决策的影响。在每一轮游戏中,参与者都会得到一笔钱,他们必须决定要在伙伴身上投资多少钱,而伙伴的好坏声誉、个人主义或合作行为都会受到影响。数据显示,对伴侣的负面第一印象(坏名声和/或自私行为)对盲人参与者的影响大于明眼人。然而,在与伙伴反复互动后,盲人组和健视人组的总体平均投资趋于一致。我们将这些发现解释为,盲人可能会引导参与者在与具有负面初始特征的伙伴交往时采取更加谨慎的行为。
{"title":"Direct reciprocity and reputation shape trust decisions similarly in blind and sighted individuals","authors":"Maria Arioli ,&nbsp;Chiara Ferrari ,&nbsp;Lotfi B. Merabet ,&nbsp;Zaira Cattaneo","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103683","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103683","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study addresses the effects of blindness on trust. Using an auditory version of the multi-round Trust Game, we investigated the effect of reputation and reciprocity on trust decisions in early blind and sighted participants. During each round of the game, participants were endowed with a sum of money and had to decide how much they wanted to invest in their partners, who were manipulated as a function of their good or bad reputation and individualistic or cooperative behavior. The data showed that negative first impression about the partner (bad reputation and/or selfish behavior) impacted more blind participants than sighted ones. However, following repeated interactions with the partners, the overall mean investment aligned between the blind and sighted groups. We interpret these findings as suggesting that blindness may guide participants to a more cautionary behavior when dealing with partners with negative initial characteristics.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000503/pdfft?md5=5e33a70cbdd9fa43e2ad1d2bac5574a4&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000503-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140309737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The sense of agency in near and far space 远近空间的代入感
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103672
Marika Mariano , Giulia Stanco , Damiano Ignazio Graps , Ileana Rossetti , Nadia Bolognini , Eraldo Paulesu , Laura Zapparoli

The sense of agency is the ability to recognize that we are the actors of our actions and their consequences.

We explored whether and how spatial cues may modulate the agency experience by manipulating the ecological validity of the experimental setup (real-space or computer-based setup) and the distance of the action-outcome (near or far).

We tested 58 healthy adults collecting explicit agency judgments and the perceived time interval between movements and outcomes (to quantify the intentional binding phenomenon, an implicit index of agency).

Participants show greater implicit agency for voluntary actions when there is a temporal and spatial action-outcome contingency. Conversely, participants reported similar explicit agency for outcomes appearing in the near and far space. Notably, these effects were independent of the ecological validity of the setting.

These results suggest that spatial proximity, realistic or illusory, is essential for feeling implicitly responsible for the consequences of our actions.

我们通过操纵实验设置(真实空间或基于计算机的设置)的生态有效性和动作-结果的距离(近或远),探讨了空间线索是否以及如何调节代理体验。我们对 58 名健康成年人进行了测试,收集了他们的显性代理判断以及动作与结果之间的感知时间间隔(量化意向束缚现象,这是代理的隐性指标)。相反,参与者对出现在近空间和远空间的结果表现出相似的显性代理。值得注意的是,这些效应与环境的生态有效性无关。这些结果表明,空间接近性,无论是现实的还是虚幻的,对于感觉到自己对行为后果负有隐性责任都是至关重要的。
{"title":"The sense of agency in near and far space","authors":"Marika Mariano ,&nbsp;Giulia Stanco ,&nbsp;Damiano Ignazio Graps ,&nbsp;Ileana Rossetti ,&nbsp;Nadia Bolognini ,&nbsp;Eraldo Paulesu ,&nbsp;Laura Zapparoli","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103672","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103672","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The sense of agency is the ability to recognize that we are the actors of our actions and their consequences.</p><p>We explored whether and how spatial cues may modulate the agency experience by manipulating the ecological validity of the experimental setup (real-space or computer-based setup) and the distance of the action-outcome (near or far).</p><p>We tested 58 healthy adults collecting explicit agency judgments and the perceived time interval between movements and outcomes (to quantify the intentional binding phenomenon, an implicit index of agency).</p><p>Participants show greater implicit agency for voluntary actions when there is a temporal and spatial action-outcome contingency. Conversely, participants reported similar explicit agency for outcomes appearing in the near and far space. Notably, these effects were independent of the ecological validity of the setting.</p><p>These results suggest that spatial proximity, realistic or illusory, is essential for feeling implicitly responsible for the consequences of our actions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000394/pdfft?md5=a883a68580c70408cd8ce8dca4d09f89&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000394-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140051823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mapping the pre-reflective experience of “self” to the brain - An ERP study 将 "自我 "的前反思体验映射到大脑--ERP 研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103654
Piani Maria Chiara , Gerber Bettina Salome , Koenig Thomas , Morishima Yosuke , Nordgaard Julie , Jandl Martin

The neural underpinnings of selfhood encompass pre-reflective and reflective self-experience. The former refers to a basic, immediate experience of being a self, while the latter involves cognition and introspection. Although neural correlates of reflective self-experience have been studied, the pre-reflective remains underinvestigated.

This research aims to bridge this gap by comparatively investigating ERP correlates of reading first- vs. third-person pronouns – approximating pre-reflective self-experience – and self- vs. other-related adjectives – approximating reflective self-experience – in 30 healthy participants.

We found differential neural engagement between pre-reflective and reflective self-experience at 254–310 ms post-stimulus onset. Source estimation suggested that our sensor-level results could be plausibly explained by the involvement of cortical midline structures and default mode network in the general sense of self but selective recruitment of anterior cingulate and top-down dorsal attention network in the pre-reflective self. These findings offer a deeper understanding of the experiential self, especially pre-reflective, providing a foundation for investigating self-disorders.

自我身份的神经基础包括前反思和反思性自我体验。前者是指作为一个自我的基本、直接的体验,后者则涉及认知和反省。本研究旨在通过比较研究 30 名健康参与者在阅读第一人称代词与第三人称代词(近似于前反思性自我体验)以及自我与他人相关形容词(近似于反思性自我体验)时的 ERP 相关性,来弥补这一空白。源估算表明,我们的传感器水平结果可以合理地解释为,一般自我感觉涉及大脑皮层中线结构和默认模式网络,而前反思性自我感觉则选择性地招募前扣带回和自上而下的背侧注意网络。这些发现加深了人们对经验自我,尤其是前反思自我的理解,为研究自我失调症提供了基础。
{"title":"Mapping the pre-reflective experience of “self” to the brain - An ERP study","authors":"Piani Maria Chiara ,&nbsp;Gerber Bettina Salome ,&nbsp;Koenig Thomas ,&nbsp;Morishima Yosuke ,&nbsp;Nordgaard Julie ,&nbsp;Jandl Martin","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103654","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103654","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The neural underpinnings of selfhood encompass pre-reflective and reflective self-experience. The former refers to a basic, immediate experience of being a self, while the latter involves cognition and introspection. Although neural correlates of reflective self-experience have been studied, the pre-reflective remains underinvestigated.</p><p>This research aims to bridge this gap by comparatively investigating ERP correlates of reading first- vs. third-person pronouns – approximating pre-reflective self-experience – and self- vs. other-related adjectives – approximating reflective self-experience – in 30 healthy participants.</p><p>We found differential neural engagement between pre-reflective and reflective self-experience at 254–310 ms post-stimulus onset. Source estimation suggested that our sensor-level results could be plausibly explained by the involvement of cortical midline structures and default mode network in the general sense of self but selective recruitment of anterior cingulate and top-down dorsal attention network in the pre-reflective self. These findings offer a deeper understanding of the experiential self, especially pre-reflective, providing a foundation for investigating self-disorders.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000217/pdfft?md5=4933acccf78f3aad92c666051eda93ec&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000217-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139992778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of self-related information in the sense of agency 自我相关信息在代入感中的作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103671
Guanhua Huang , Xun Jia , Yuanmeng Zhang , Ke Zhao , Xiaolan Fu

Sense of agency (SoA) refers to the subjective experience of controlling one’s actions and their subsequent consequences. The present study endeavors to investigate the impact of how different degrees of self-related stimuli as action outcomes on the sense of agency by observing the temporal binding effect. Results showed that self-related sound significantly altered temporal binding, notably influencing outcome binding. A post-hoc explanation model effectively elucidated the role of self-related information in the formation of the sense of agency.

代入感(SoA)是指控制自己的行为及其后续后果的主观体验。本研究试图通过观察时间束缚效应,研究作为行动结果的不同程度的自我相关刺激对代理感的影响。结果表明,与自我相关的声音会明显改变时间束缚,特别是影响结果束缚。事后解释模型有效地阐明了自我相关信息在代入感形成过程中的作用。
{"title":"The role of self-related information in the sense of agency","authors":"Guanhua Huang ,&nbsp;Xun Jia ,&nbsp;Yuanmeng Zhang ,&nbsp;Ke Zhao ,&nbsp;Xiaolan Fu","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103671","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103671","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Sense of agency (SoA) refers to the subjective experience of controlling one’s actions and their subsequent consequences. The present study endeavors to investigate the impact of how different degrees of self-related stimuli as action outcomes on the sense of agency by observing the temporal binding effect. Results showed that self-related sound significantly altered temporal binding, notably influencing outcome binding. A post-hoc explanation model effectively elucidated the role of self-related information in the formation of the sense of agency.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139992779","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Consciousness and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1