首页 > 最新文献

Consciousness and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Towards a structural turn in consciousness science 实现意识科学的结构性转向
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103653
Johannes Kleiner

Recent activities in virtually all fields engaged in consciousness studies indicate early signs of a structural turn, where verbal descriptions or simple formalisations of conscious experiences are replaced by structural tools, most notably mathematical spaces. My goal here is to offer three comments that, in my opinion, are essential to avoid misunderstandings in these developments early on. These comments concern metaphysical premises of structural approaches, the viability of structure-preserving mappings, and the question of what a structure of conscious experience is in the first place. I will also explain what, in my opinion, are the great promises of structural methodologies and how they might impact consciousness science at large.

最近,几乎所有从事意识研究的领域都出现了结构性转向的早期迹象,即意识体验的语言描述或简单形式化被结构性工具(尤其是数学空间)所取代。我在这里的目的是提出三点意见,在我看来,这些意见对于避免在这些发展初期出现误解至关重要。这些评论涉及结构方法的形而上学前提、结构保留映射的可行性,以及意识经验的结构首先是什么的问题。我还将解释,在我看来,结构方法论的巨大前景是什么,以及它们可能对整个意识科学产生怎样的影响。
{"title":"Towards a structural turn in consciousness science","authors":"Johannes Kleiner","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103653","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103653","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent activities in virtually all fields engaged in consciousness studies indicate early signs of a structural turn, where verbal descriptions or simple formalisations of conscious experiences are replaced by structural tools, most notably mathematical spaces. My goal here is to offer three comments that, in my opinion, are essential to avoid misunderstandings in these developments early on. These comments concern metaphysical premises of structural approaches, the viability of structure-preserving mappings, and the question of what a structure of conscious experience is in the first place. I will also explain what, in my opinion, are the great promises of structural methodologies and how they might impact consciousness science at large.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000205/pdfft?md5=50786a0304317484bc8aceb2bba7546e&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000205-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139992776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
More than fulfilled expectations: An electrophysiological investigation of varying cause-effect relationships and schizotypal personality traits as related to the sense of agency 不只是满足期望:不同因果关系和精神分裂症人格特质与代入感相关的电生理学调查
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103667
Nena Luzi , Maria Chiara Piani , Daniela Hubl , Thomas Koenig

The sense of agency (SoA) is central to human experience. The comparator model, contrasting sensory prediction and action feedback, is influential but limited in explaining SoA. We investigated mechanisms beyond the comparator model, focusing on the processing of unpredictable stimuli, perimotor components of SoA, and their relation to schizotypy.

ERPs were recorded from 18 healthy participants engaged in button-pressing tasks while perceiving tones with varying causal relationships with their actions. We investigated the processing of non-causally related tones, contrasted this to causally related tones, and examined perimotor correlates of subjective expectancy and experience of agency.

We confirmed N100 attenuation for self-generated stimuli but found similar effects for expectancy-dependent processing of random tones. SoA also correlated with perimotor ERP components, modulated by schizotypy.

Thus, neural processes preceding actions contribute to the formation of SoA and are associated with schizotypy. Unpredictable events also undergo sensory attenuation, implying additional mechanisms contributing to SoA.

代入感(SoA)是人类经验的核心。比较器模型将感觉预测与行动反馈进行对比,对解释代入感很有影响,但作用有限。我们研究了比较器模型之外的机制,重点是不可预测刺激的处理、SoA 的运动周围成分以及它们与精神分裂症的关系。我们研究了非因果关系音调的处理过程,将其与因果关系音调进行了对比,并研究了主观预期和代理体验的周围运动相关性。我们证实了自发刺激的 N100 衰减,但发现对随机音调的预期依赖处理也有类似的效果。因此,行动前的神经过程有助于 SoA 的形成,并与精神分裂症有关。不可预测的事件也会发生感觉衰减,这意味着还有其他机制也会导致SoA。
{"title":"More than fulfilled expectations: An electrophysiological investigation of varying cause-effect relationships and schizotypal personality traits as related to the sense of agency","authors":"Nena Luzi ,&nbsp;Maria Chiara Piani ,&nbsp;Daniela Hubl ,&nbsp;Thomas Koenig","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103667","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103667","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The sense of agency (SoA) is central to human experience. The comparator model, contrasting sensory prediction and action feedback, is influential but limited in explaining SoA. We investigated mechanisms beyond the comparator model, focusing on the processing of unpredictable stimuli, perimotor components of SoA, and their relation to schizotypy.</p><p>ERPs were recorded from 18 healthy participants engaged in button-pressing tasks while perceiving tones with varying causal relationships with their actions. We investigated the processing of non-causally related tones, contrasted this to causally related tones, and examined perimotor correlates of subjective expectancy and experience of agency.</p><p>We confirmed N100 attenuation for self-generated stimuli but found similar effects for expectancy-dependent processing of random tones. SoA also correlated with perimotor ERP components, modulated by schizotypy.</p><p>Thus, neural processes preceding actions contribute to the formation of SoA and are associated with schizotypy. Unpredictable events also undergo sensory attenuation, implying additional mechanisms contributing to SoA.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139999630","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dissociation between temporal attention and Consciousness: Unconscious temporal cue induces temporal expectation effect 时间注意力与意识之间的分离:无意识时间线索诱发时间预期效应
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103670
Xiaowei Ding , Huichao Ji , Wenhao Yu , Luzi Xu , Youting Lin , Yanliang Sun

The debate over the independence of attention and consciousness is ongoing. Prior studies have established that invisible spatial cues can direct attention. However, our exploration extends beyond spatial dimensions to temporal information as a potent guide for attention. A intriguing question arises: Can unconscious temporal cues trigger attentional orienting? To investigate, we employed a modified reaction-time task in Experiments 1 and 2, using Gabor stimuli or human facial stimuli as temporal cues rendered invisible through continuous flash suppression. We aimed to uncover temporal expectation effects (TE effects) without conscious awareness. Moreover, Experiments 3 and 4 probed the boundaries of this unconscious processing, assessing whether conscious temporal cues could modulate TE effects. Our results confirm that invisible temporal cues can induce TE effects, and these effects can be overridden by conscious temporal cues. This dissociation between temporal attention and consciousness provide a new perspective on our understanding of their relationship.

关于注意力和意识的独立性的争论仍在继续。先前的研究已经证实,看不见的空间线索可以引导注意力。然而,我们的探索超越了空间维度,将时间信息作为注意力的有力引导。这就产生了一个耐人寻味的问题:无意识的时间线索能否引发注意定向?为了探究这个问题,我们在实验 1 和 2 中采用了改进的反应时任务,使用 Gabor 刺激或人类面部刺激作为时间线索,通过连续闪光抑制使其隐形。我们的目的是在无意识的情况下发现时间期望效应(TE效应)。此外,实验 3 和 4 还探究了这种无意识加工的边界,评估了有意识的时间线索是否能调节时间期望效应。我们的结果证实,不可见的时间线索可以诱发TE效应,而这些效应可以被有意识的时间线索所覆盖。时间注意和意识之间的这种分离为我们理解它们之间的关系提供了一个新的视角。
{"title":"Dissociation between temporal attention and Consciousness: Unconscious temporal cue induces temporal expectation effect","authors":"Xiaowei Ding ,&nbsp;Huichao Ji ,&nbsp;Wenhao Yu ,&nbsp;Luzi Xu ,&nbsp;Youting Lin ,&nbsp;Yanliang Sun","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103670","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103670","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The debate over the independence of attention and consciousness is ongoing. Prior studies have established that invisible spatial cues can direct attention. However, our exploration extends beyond spatial dimensions to temporal information as a potent guide for attention. A intriguing question arises: Can unconscious temporal cues trigger attentional orienting? To investigate, we employed a modified reaction-time task in Experiments 1 and 2, using Gabor stimuli or human facial stimuli as temporal cues rendered invisible through continuous flash suppression. We aimed to uncover temporal expectation effects (TE effects) without conscious awareness. Moreover, Experiments 3 and 4 probed the boundaries of this unconscious processing, assessing whether conscious temporal cues could modulate TE effects. Our results confirm that invisible temporal cues can induce TE effects, and these effects can be overridden by conscious temporal cues. This dissociation between temporal attention and consciousness provide a new perspective on our understanding of their relationship.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139992777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing the commensurability of theories of consciousness: On the usefulness of common denominators in differentiating, integrating and testing hypotheses 评估意识理论的可比性:论共同标准在区分、整合和检验假设中的作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103668
K. Evers , M. Farisco , C.M.A. Pennartz

How deep is the current diversity in the panoply of theories to define consciousness, and to what extent do these theories share common denominators? Here we first examine to what extent different theories are commensurable (or comparable) along particular dimensions. We posit logical (and, when applicable, empirical) commensurability as a necessary condition for identifying common denominators among different theories. By consequence, dimensions for inclusion in a set of logically and empirically commensurable theories of consciousness can be proposed. Next, we compare a limited subset of neuroscience-based theories in terms of commensurability. This analysis does not yield a denominator that might serve to define a minimally unifying model of consciousness. Theories that seem to be akin by one denominator can be remote by another. We suggest a methodology of comparing different theories via multiple probing questions, allowing to discern overall (dis)similarities between theories. Despite very different background definitions of consciousness, we conclude that, if attention is paid to the search for a common methological approach to brain-consciousness relationships, it should be possible in principle to overcome the current Babylonian confusion of tongues and eventually integrate and merge different theories.

目前,定义意识的理论多种多样,它们的共同点有多深?在此,我们首先探讨不同理论在特定维度上的可通约性(或可比性)。我们认为,逻辑上(以及在适用的情况下,经验上)的可比性是确定不同理论之间共同点的必要条件。因此,我们可以提出将哪些维度纳入一套逻辑上和经验上可相通的意识理论。接下来,我们将对基于神经科学的有限理论子集进行可比性比较。这一分析并没有得出可用于定义意识的最小统一模型的分母。在一个分母上看似相近的理论,在另一个分母上却可能相去甚远。我们建议采用一种方法,通过多个探究性问题对不同理论进行比较,从而找出理论之间的整体(不)相似性。尽管意识的背景定义大相径庭,但我们的结论是,如果重视寻找大脑与意识关系的共同方法论,原则上应该可以克服当前巴比伦式的语言混乱,并最终整合和融合不同的理论。
{"title":"Assessing the commensurability of theories of consciousness: On the usefulness of common denominators in differentiating, integrating and testing hypotheses","authors":"K. Evers ,&nbsp;M. Farisco ,&nbsp;C.M.A. Pennartz","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103668","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103668","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How deep is the current diversity in the panoply of theories to define consciousness, and to what extent do these theories share common denominators? Here we first examine to what extent different theories are commensurable (or comparable) along particular dimensions. We posit logical (and, when applicable, empirical) commensurability as a necessary condition for identifying common denominators among different theories. By consequence, dimensions for inclusion in a set of logically and empirically commensurable theories of consciousness can be proposed. Next, we compare a limited subset of neuroscience-based theories in terms of commensurability. This analysis does not yield a denominator that might serve to define a minimally unifying model of consciousness. Theories that seem to be akin by one denominator can be remote by another. We suggest a methodology of comparing different theories via multiple probing questions, allowing to discern overall (dis)similarities between theories. Despite very different background definitions of consciousness, we conclude that, if attention is paid to the search for a common methological approach to brain-consciousness relationships, it should be possible in principle to overcome the current Babylonian confusion of tongues and eventually integrate and merge different theories.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000357/pdfft?md5=2a79a0bcac518fe0666455920e5bc015&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000357-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139985351","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How (not) to demonstrate unconscious priming: Overcoming issues with post-hoc data selection, low power, and frequentist statistics 如何(不)证明无意识引物:克服事后数据选择、低功率和频数统计等问题
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103669
Timo Stein , Simon van Gaal , Johannes J. Fahrenfort

One widely used scientific approach to studying consciousness involves contrasting conscious operations with unconscious ones. However, challenges in establishing the absence of conscious awareness have led to debates about the extent and existence of unconscious processes. We collected experimental data on unconscious semantic priming, manipulating prime presentation duration to highlight the critical role of the analysis approach in attributing priming effects to unconscious processing. We demonstrate that common practices like post-hoc data selection, low statistical power, and frequentist statistical testing can erroneously support claims of unconscious priming. Conversely, adopting best practices like direct performance-awareness contrasts, Bayesian tests, and increased statistical power can prevent such erroneous conclusions. Many past experiments, including our own, fail to meet these standards, casting doubt on previous claims about unconscious processing. Implementing these robust practices will enhance our understanding of unconscious processing and shed light on the functions and neural mechanisms of consciousness.

研究意识的一种广泛使用的科学方法是将有意识操作与无意识操作进行对比。然而,在确定无意识意识方面的挑战导致了关于无意识过程的程度和存在的争论。我们收集了关于无意识语义引物的实验数据,通过操纵引物呈现的持续时间来突出分析方法在将引物效应归因于无意识加工方面的关键作用。我们证明,事后数据选择、低统计功率和频数统计测试等常见做法会错误地支持无意识引物的说法。相反,采用最佳实践,如直接性能-意识对比、贝叶斯测试和提高统计能力,则可以避免此类错误结论。过去的许多实验,包括我们自己的实验,都没有达到这些标准,从而使人们对以前关于无意识加工的说法产生怀疑。实施这些强有力的实践将增强我们对无意识加工的理解,并揭示意识的功能和神经机制。
{"title":"How (not) to demonstrate unconscious priming: Overcoming issues with post-hoc data selection, low power, and frequentist statistics","authors":"Timo Stein ,&nbsp;Simon van Gaal ,&nbsp;Johannes J. Fahrenfort","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103669","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103669","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One widely used scientific approach to studying consciousness involves contrasting conscious operations with unconscious ones. However, challenges in establishing the absence of conscious awareness have led to debates about the extent and existence of unconscious processes. We collected experimental data on unconscious semantic priming, manipulating prime presentation duration to highlight the critical role of the analysis approach in attributing priming effects to unconscious processing. We demonstrate that common practices like post-hoc data selection, low statistical power, and frequentist statistical testing can erroneously support claims of unconscious priming. Conversely, adopting best practices like direct performance-awareness contrasts, Bayesian tests, and increased statistical power can prevent such erroneous conclusions. Many past experiments, including our own, fail to meet these standards, casting doubt on previous claims about unconscious processing. Implementing these robust practices will enhance our understanding of unconscious processing and shed light on the functions and neural mechanisms of consciousness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000369/pdfft?md5=ce5d8c8e8511451b1f8b569f2766d025&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000369-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139935543","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual perceptual processing is unaffected by cognitive fatigue 视觉感知处理不受认知疲劳的影响。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103666
Kathleen J. Peters, Dana Maslovat, Anthony N. Carlsen

Cognitive fatigue (CF) can lead to an increase in the latency of simple reaction time, although the processes involved in this delay are unknown. One potential explanation is that a longer time may be required for sensory processing of relevant stimuli. To investigate this possibility, the current study used a visual inspection time task to measure perceptual processing speed before and after a CF (math and memory) or non-fatiguing (documentary film) intervention. Subjective fatigue and simple reaction time significantly increased following the CF, but not the non-fatiguing intervention, confirming that CF was induced. Conversely, there was no effect of CF on inspection time task performance. It was therefore concluded that the speed of perceptual processing is not significantly impacted by CF, and thus is unlikely to underlie CF-related reaction time increases. Instead, increases in simple reaction time latency in CF may be due to delays in response preparation or initiation.

认知疲劳(CF)会导致简单反应时间的延迟增加,但这种延迟所涉及的过程尚不清楚。一种可能的解释是,对相关刺激进行感官处理可能需要更长的时间。为了研究这种可能性,本研究使用了视觉检查时间任务来测量知觉处理速度,分别在数学和记忆(CF)或非疲劳(纪录片)干预前后进行。主观疲劳和简单反应时间在 CF 干预后明显增加,而在非疲劳干预后没有增加,这证实了 CF 是诱导性的。相反,CF 对检查时间任务的表现没有影响。因此得出的结论是,知觉处理的速度并未受到 CF 的显著影响,因此不太可能是 CF 导致反应时间增加的原因。相反,CF 中简单反应时间潜伏期的增加可能是由于反应准备或启动延迟造成的。
{"title":"Visual perceptual processing is unaffected by cognitive fatigue","authors":"Kathleen J. Peters,&nbsp;Dana Maslovat,&nbsp;Anthony N. Carlsen","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103666","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103666","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cognitive fatigue (CF) can lead to an increase in the latency of simple reaction time, although the processes involved in this delay are unknown. One potential explanation is that a longer time may be required for sensory processing of relevant stimuli. To investigate this possibility, the current study used a visual inspection time task to measure perceptual processing speed before and after a CF (math and memory) or non-fatiguing (documentary film) intervention. Subjective fatigue and simple reaction time significantly increased following the CF, but not the non-fatiguing intervention, confirming that CF was induced. Conversely, there was no effect of CF on inspection time task performance. It was therefore concluded that the speed of perceptual processing is not significantly impacted by CF, and thus is unlikely to underlie CF-related reaction time increases. Instead, increases in simple reaction time latency in CF may be due to delays in response preparation or initiation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000333/pdfft?md5=d346b513bc3c7c099a8e27094f790a05&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000333-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139934105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intentional binding – Is it just causal binding? A replication study of Suzuki et al. (2019) 有意结合--只是因果结合吗?铃木等人(2019)的重复研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103665
Michael Wiesing, Eckart Zimmermann

Intentional actions produce a temporal compression between the action and its outcome, known as intentional binding. However, Suzuki et al. (2019) recently showed that temporal compression can be observed without intentional actions. However, their results show a clear regression to the mean, which might have confounded the estimates of temporal intervals. To control these effects, we presented temporal intervals block-wise. Indeed, we found systematically greater compression for active than passive trials, in contrast to Suzuki et al. (2019). In our second experiment, our goal was to conceptually replicate the previous study. However, we were unable to reproduce their results and instead found more pronounced temporal compression in active trials compared to passive ones. In a subsequent attempt at a direct replication, we did not observe the same findings as the original study. Our findings reinforce the theory that intentions rather than causality cause temporal binding.

During the preparation of this work, the authors used ChatGPT in order to improve the readability of the paper. After using this tool/service, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.

有意行为会在行为和结果之间产生时间压缩,即所谓的有意约束。然而,Suzuki 等人(2019)最近的研究表明,在没有有意行为的情况下也能观察到时间压缩。然而,他们的结果显示出明显的均值回归,这可能会混淆对时间间隔的估计。为了控制这些影响,我们分块呈现了时间间隔。事实上,我们发现主动试验比被动试验有系统性的更大压缩,这与 Suzuki 等人(2019)的结果形成了鲜明对比。在第二个实验中,我们的目标是在概念上复制之前的研究。然而,我们无法再现他们的结果,反而发现主动试验的时间压缩比被动试验更明显。在随后的直接复制尝试中,我们没有观察到与最初研究相同的结果。我们的研究结果加强了意图而非因果关系导致时间绑定的理论。在本文的准备过程中,作者使用了 ChatGPT,以提高论文的可读性。使用该工具/服务后,作者根据需要对内容进行了审阅和编辑,并对出版物的内容负全部责任。
{"title":"Intentional binding – Is it just causal binding? A replication study of Suzuki et al. (2019)","authors":"Michael Wiesing,&nbsp;Eckart Zimmermann","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103665","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103665","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Intentional actions produce a temporal compression between the action and its outcome, known as intentional binding. However, Suzuki et al. (2019) recently showed that temporal compression can be observed without intentional actions. However, their results show a clear regression to the mean, which might have confounded the estimates of temporal intervals. To control these effects, we presented temporal intervals block-wise. Indeed, we found systematically greater compression for active than passive trials, in contrast to Suzuki et al. (2019). In our second experiment, our goal was to conceptually replicate the previous study. However, we were unable to reproduce their results and instead found more pronounced temporal compression in active trials compared to passive ones. In a subsequent attempt at a direct replication, we did not observe the same findings as the original study. Our findings reinforce the theory that intentions rather than causality cause temporal binding.</p><p>During the preparation of this work, the authors used ChatGPT in order to improve the readability of the paper. After using this tool/service, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000321/pdfft?md5=a7abf6e001dea660b5a941472006e746&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000321-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139731562","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Clarifying and measuring the characteristics of experiences that involve a loss of self or a dissolution of its boundaries 澄清和衡量那些涉及自我丧失或边界解体的经历的特征
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103655
Nicholas K. Canby , Jared Lindahl , Willoughby B. Britton , James V. Córdova

Mystical experience, non-dual awareness, selflessness, self-transcendent experience, and ego-dissolution have become increasingly prominent constructs in meditation and psychedelic research. However, these constructs and their measures tend to be highly overlapping, imprecise, and poorly integrated with similar pathological experiences. The present study seeks to clarify the common factors involved in the characteristics of these experiences using precise distinctions across an array of experience contexts (including meditation, psychedelics, and psychopathology). Participants (N = 386) completed an online survey about an experience that involved either a dissolution of self-boundaries or a loss of selfhood. Confirmatory factor analyses resulted in 16 experience characteristics, including multiple types of changes in sense of self, co-occurring phenomenology, and cognitive and affective responses. Qualitative thematic analysis provided rich descriptions of experience characteristics. Taken together, results lead to a more specific measurement model and descriptive account of experiences involving a loss of self or self-boundary.

在冥想和迷幻研究中,神秘体验、非二元意识、无私、自我超越体验和自我解体已成为日益突出的概念。然而,这些概念及其测量方法往往与类似的病理体验高度重叠、不精确且结合不紧密。本研究试图通过对一系列体验情境(包括冥想、迷幻药和精神病理学)的精确区分,来阐明这些体验特征所涉及的共同因素。参与者(N = 386)完成了一项在线调查,内容涉及自我界限的消解或自我身份的丧失。确认性因素分析得出了 16 种体验特征,包括多种类型的自我意识变化、共存现象以及认知和情感反应。定性主题分析提供了丰富的体验特征描述。综上所述,研究结果为涉及丧失自我或自我边界的体验提供了更具体的测量模型和描述性说明。
{"title":"Clarifying and measuring the characteristics of experiences that involve a loss of self or a dissolution of its boundaries","authors":"Nicholas K. Canby ,&nbsp;Jared Lindahl ,&nbsp;Willoughby B. Britton ,&nbsp;James V. Córdova","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103655","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103655","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mystical experience, non-dual awareness, selflessness, self-transcendent experience, and ego-dissolution have become increasingly prominent constructs in meditation and psychedelic research. However, these constructs and their measures tend to be highly overlapping, imprecise, and poorly integrated with similar pathological experiences. The present study seeks to clarify the common factors involved in the characteristics of these experiences using precise distinctions across an array of experience contexts (including meditation, psychedelics, and psychopathology). Participants (N = 386) completed an online survey about an experience that involved either a dissolution of self-boundaries or a loss of selfhood. Confirmatory factor analyses resulted in 16 experience characteristics, including multiple types of changes in sense of self, co-occurring phenomenology, and cognitive and affective responses. Qualitative thematic analysis provided rich descriptions of experience characteristics. Taken together, results lead to a more specific measurement model and descriptive account of experiences involving a loss of self or self-boundary.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139718270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Viral simulations in dreams: The effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on threatening dream content in a Finnish sample of diary dreams 梦境中的病毒模拟:COVID-19 大流行对芬兰日记梦样本中威胁性梦境内容的影响。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103651
Ville Loukola , Jarno Tuominen , Santeri Kirsilä , Annimaaria Kyyhkynen , Maron Lahdenperä , Lilja Parkkali , Emilia Ranta , Eveliina Malinen , Sanni Vanhanen , Katariina Välimaa , Henri Olkoniemi , Antti Revonsuo , Katja Valli

Previous research indicates that the COVID-19 pandemic has affected dreaming negatively. We compared 1132 dreams collected with prospective two-week dream diary during the pandemic to 166 dreams collected before the pandemic. We hypothesized that the pandemic would increase the number of threatening events, threats related to diseases, and the severity of threats. We also hypothesized that dreams that include direct references to the pandemic will include more threatening events, more disease-related threats, and more severe threats. In contradiction with our hypotheses, results showed no differences between pandemic and pre-pandemic samples in the number of threats, threats related to diseases, or severe threats. However, dreams with direct references to the pandemic had more threats, disease-related threats, and severe threats. Our results thus do not suggest a significant overall increase in nightmarish or threatening dream content during the pandemic but show a more profound effect on a minority of dreams.

以前的研究表明,COVID-19 大流行对做梦产生了负面影响。我们将大流行期间通过前瞻性两周梦境日记收集到的 1132 个梦境与大流行前收集到的 166 个梦境进行了比较。我们假设大流行会增加威胁事件的数量、与疾病有关的威胁以及威胁的严重程度。我们还假设,直接提到大流行的梦境会包含更多的威胁事件、更多与疾病相关的威胁和更严重的威胁。与我们的假设相矛盾的是,结果显示大流行与大流行前的样本在威胁的数量、与疾病相关的威胁或严重威胁方面没有差异。然而,直接提及大流行的梦境中的威胁、与疾病相关的威胁和严重威胁都更多。因此,我们的研究结果并不表明在大流行期间梦魇或威胁性梦境内容总体上有显著增加,但却表明对少数梦境产生了更深远的影响。
{"title":"Viral simulations in dreams: The effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on threatening dream content in a Finnish sample of diary dreams","authors":"Ville Loukola ,&nbsp;Jarno Tuominen ,&nbsp;Santeri Kirsilä ,&nbsp;Annimaaria Kyyhkynen ,&nbsp;Maron Lahdenperä ,&nbsp;Lilja Parkkali ,&nbsp;Emilia Ranta ,&nbsp;Eveliina Malinen ,&nbsp;Sanni Vanhanen ,&nbsp;Katariina Välimaa ,&nbsp;Henri Olkoniemi ,&nbsp;Antti Revonsuo ,&nbsp;Katja Valli","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103651","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103651","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research indicates that the COVID-19 pandemic has affected dreaming negatively. We compared 1132 dreams collected with prospective two-week dream diary during the pandemic to 166 dreams collected before the pandemic. We hypothesized that the pandemic would increase the number of threatening events, threats related to diseases, and the severity of threats. We also hypothesized that dreams that include direct references to the pandemic will include more threatening events, more disease-related threats, and more severe threats. In contradiction with our hypotheses, results showed no differences between pandemic and pre-pandemic samples in the number of threats, threats related to diseases, or severe threats. However, dreams with direct references to the pandemic had more threats, disease-related threats, and severe threats. Our results thus do not suggest a significant overall increase in nightmarish or threatening dream content during the pandemic but show a more profound effect on a minority of dreams.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810024000187/pdfft?md5=3ef49044ab0c0812bc031c46dd4e429f&pid=1-s2.0-S1053810024000187-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139713304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Goal characteristics predict the occurrence of goal-related events through belief in future occurrence 目标特征通过对未来发生的信念来预测目标相关事件的发生
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103649
Claudia Garcia Jimenez, Arnaud D'Argembeau

While previous studies have highlighted the role of episodic future thinking in goal pursuit, the underlying cognitive mechanisms remain unexplored. Episodic future thinking may promote goal pursuit by shaping the feeling that imagined events will (or will not) happen in the future – referred to as belief in future occurrence. We investigated whether goal self-concordance (Experiment 1) and other goal characteristics identified as influential in goal pursuit (Experiment 2) modulate belief in the future occurrence of goal-related events and predict the actual occurrence of these events. Results showed that goal self-concordance, engagement, and expectancy had an indirect effect on the actual occurrence of events, which was (partially) mediated by belief in future occurrence. The mediating role of belief supports the view that belief in future occurrence when imagining events conveys useful information, allowing us to make informed decisions and undertake adaptive actions in the process of goal pursuit.

尽管以往的研究已经强调了外显未来思维在目标追求中的作用,但其背后的认知机制仍有待探索。外显未来思维可能会通过塑造想象中的事件会(或不会)在未来发生的感觉(即对未来发生的信念)来促进目标追求。我们研究了目标自我一致性(实验 1)和其他被认为对目标追求有影响的目标特征(实验 2)是否会调节对目标相关事件未来发生的信念,并预测这些事件的实际发生。结果表明,目标自我一致性、参与度和期望值对事件的实际发生有间接影响,而这种影响(部分)是由对未来发生的信念所中介的。信念的中介作用支持了这样一种观点,即在想象事件时相信未来会发生可以传递有用的信息,让我们在追求目标的过程中做出明智的决定并采取适应性行动。
{"title":"Goal characteristics predict the occurrence of goal-related events through belief in future occurrence","authors":"Claudia Garcia Jimenez,&nbsp;Arnaud D'Argembeau","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103649","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103649","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>While previous studies have highlighted the role of episodic future thinking in goal pursuit, the underlying cognitive mechanisms remain unexplored. Episodic future thinking may promote goal pursuit by shaping the feeling that imagined events will (or will not) happen in the future – referred to as <em>belief in future occurrence</em>. We investigated whether goal self-concordance (Experiment 1) and other goal characteristics identified as influential in goal pursuit (Experiment 2) modulate belief in the future occurrence of goal-related events and predict the actual occurrence of these events. Results showed that goal self-concordance, engagement, and expectancy had an indirect effect on the actual occurrence of events, which was (partially) mediated by belief in future occurrence. The mediating role of belief supports the view that belief in future occurrence when imagining events conveys useful information, allowing us to make informed decisions and undertake adaptive actions in the process of goal pursuit.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139699972","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Consciousness and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1