首页 > 最新文献

Consciousness and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Self-face processing in relation to self-referential tasks in 24-month-old infants: A study through eye movements and pupillometry measures 24个月婴儿自我面孔加工与自我参照任务的关系:一项通过眼动和瞳孔测量的研究。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103803
Hiroshi Nitta , Yusuke Uto , Kengo Chaya , Kazuhide Hashiya
The aim of the current study was to investigate visual scan patterns for the self-face in infants with the ability to recognize themselves with a photograph. 24-month-old infants (N = 32) were presented with faces including the self-face in the upright or inverted orientation. We also measured infants’ ability to recognize oneself in a mirror and with a photograph. Results showed that only in trials with the self-face was pupil dilation greater in the upright orientation than in the inverted orientation, and that eye movements and pupil dilation were not associated with PSR tasks. Our findings suggest that the processing of the self-face was processed in a manner similar to that of others, with longer and more fixations on eyes and nose, but infants allocated more attentional resources to processing upright self-face. Self-face processing in infancy may be independent of the understanding of the self beyond the here and now.
目前这项研究的目的是调查那些有能力通过照片认出自己的婴儿的自我脸的视觉扫描模式。对32名24月龄婴儿进行直立或倒立的面部观察。我们还测量了婴儿在镜子和照片中认出自己的能力。结果表明,只有在自我面孔的实验中,瞳孔在直立方向上的扩张大于在倒立方向上的扩张,而眼球运动和瞳孔扩张与自我识别任务无关。我们的研究结果表明,婴儿对自我面孔的处理方式与其他人相似,对眼睛和鼻子的注视时间更长,注视时间更长,但婴儿将更多的注意力资源分配给处理直立的自我面孔。婴儿期的自我面孔加工可能独立于对此时此地之外的自我的理解。
{"title":"Self-face processing in relation to self-referential tasks in 24-month-old infants: A study through eye movements and pupillometry measures","authors":"Hiroshi Nitta ,&nbsp;Yusuke Uto ,&nbsp;Kengo Chaya ,&nbsp;Kazuhide Hashiya","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103803","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103803","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The aim of the current study was to investigate visual scan patterns for the self-face in infants with the ability to recognize themselves with a photograph. 24-month-old infants (N = 32) were presented with faces including the self-face in the upright or inverted orientation. We also measured infants’ ability to recognize oneself in a mirror and with a photograph. Results showed that only in trials with the self-face was pupil dilation greater in the upright orientation than in the inverted orientation, and that eye movements and pupil dilation were not associated with PSR tasks. Our findings suggest that the processing of the self-face was processed in a manner similar to that of others, with longer and more fixations on eyes and nose, but infants allocated more attentional resources to processing upright self-face. Self-face processing in infancy may be independent of the understanding of the self beyond the here and now.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103803"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142903976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Functions of consciousness in emotional processing 意识在情绪处理中的功能。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103801
Dylan Ludwig
Contrary to the leading theories of consciousness on offer, it is a fruitful working hypothesis that conscious experiences facilitate a variety of functional capacities that are distinct to particular psychological tasks, individuals, and species (i.e., functional pluralism). In this paper, I illustrate this novel methodological point by identifying some of the functional contributions that consciousness makes to (human) emotional processing. I first consolidate empirical evidence of the capacities and limitations of unconscious emotional processing, drawing on a) experimental paradigms that employ the tools of vision science (masking and suppression of emotionally relevant stimuli), and b) theoretical and clinical research on emotional disorder (Generalized Anxiety Disorder). After comparing the functional characteristics of unconscious and conscious emotional processes, I argue that conscious experiences facilitate a cluster of functions that are specific to emotion, including increased capacities for representing fine-grained evaluative information, inhibition, and flexible response.
与现有的主流意识理论相反,这是一个卓有成效的工作假设,即意识体验促进了各种不同于特定心理任务、个体和物种的功能能力(即功能多元主义)。在本文中,我通过识别意识对(人类)情绪处理的一些功能贡献来说明这一新颖的方法论观点。我首先巩固了无意识情绪处理能力和局限性的经验证据,借鉴了a)使用视觉科学工具的实验范式(掩盖和抑制情绪相关刺激),以及b)情绪障碍(广泛性焦虑障碍)的理论和临床研究。在比较了无意识和有意识的情感过程的功能特征后,我认为有意识的经历促进了一系列特定于情感的功能,包括表达细粒度评估信息、抑制和灵活反应的能力增强。
{"title":"Functions of consciousness in emotional processing","authors":"Dylan Ludwig","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103801","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103801","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Contrary to the leading theories of consciousness on offer, it is a fruitful working hypothesis that conscious experiences facilitate a variety of functional capacities that are distinct to particular psychological tasks, individuals, and species (i.e., <em>functional pluralism</em>). In this paper, I illustrate this novel methodological point by identifying some of the functional contributions that consciousness makes to (human) emotional processing. I first consolidate empirical evidence of the capacities and limitations of unconscious emotional processing, drawing on a) experimental paradigms that employ the tools of vision science (masking and suppression of emotionally relevant stimuli), and b) theoretical and clinical research on emotional disorder (Generalized Anxiety Disorder). After comparing the functional characteristics of unconscious and conscious emotional processes, I argue that conscious experiences facilitate a cluster of functions that are specific to emotion, including increased capacities for representing fine-grained evaluative information, inhibition, and flexible response.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103801"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142830900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence for the dependence of visual and kinesthetic motor imagery on isolated visual and motor practice 视觉和动觉运动意象依赖于孤立的视觉和运动练习的证据。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103802
Carrie M. Peters , Matthew W. Scott , Ryan Jin , Minghao Ma , Sarah N. Kraeutner , Nicola J. Hodges
Motor imagery (MI) is a cognitive process believed to rely on the representation developed through experience. The equivalence between MI and execution has been questioned and the relationship between experience types and MI is unclear. We tested how observational and physical practice of hand gesture sequences impacted visual and kinesthetic MI and transfer to the unpracticed effector. Three groups (n = 22/gp.); no-vision physical practice, observational practice and no-practice control, practiced and visually and kinesthetically imagined performing the sequences. MI was assessed using mental chronometry, a movement time (MT) congruency measure and subjective ratings. Physical practice improved kinesthetic MI ratings and observational practice improved visual MI ratings. Contrary to predictions, physical practice did not enhance timing congruency. Imagined MTs were longer in transfer after physical practice, suggesting MI was not based on the same representation. These data question ideas of equivalence, with poor temporal matching after no-vision physical practice.
运动意象(MI)是一种依赖于经验形成的表征的认知过程。MI和执行之间的等价性受到质疑,经验类型和MI之间的关系也不清楚。我们测试了手势序列的观察和身体练习如何影响视觉和动觉性心肌梗死并转移到未练习的效应器。3组(n = 22/gp);无视觉的身体练习,观察练习和无练习控制,练习和视觉上和动觉上想象表演的序列。MI评估使用心理计时法,运动时间(MT)一致性测量和主观评分。物理练习提高了动觉性心肌梗死评分,观察性训练提高了视觉性心肌梗死评分。与预测相反,物理练习并没有提高时间一致性。假设mt在物理练习后的转移时间更长,这表明MI不是基于相同的表征。这些数据质疑等效的观念,无视觉物理练习后的时间匹配性较差。
{"title":"Evidence for the dependence of visual and kinesthetic motor imagery on isolated visual and motor practice","authors":"Carrie M. Peters ,&nbsp;Matthew W. Scott ,&nbsp;Ryan Jin ,&nbsp;Minghao Ma ,&nbsp;Sarah N. Kraeutner ,&nbsp;Nicola J. Hodges","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103802","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103802","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Motor imagery (MI) is a cognitive process believed to rely on the representation developed through experience. The equivalence between MI and execution has been questioned and the relationship between experience types and MI is unclear. We tested how observational and physical practice of hand gesture sequences impacted visual and kinesthetic MI and transfer to the unpracticed effector. Three groups (n = 22/gp.); no-vision physical practice, observational practice and no-practice control, practiced and visually and kinesthetically imagined performing the sequences. MI was assessed using mental chronometry, a movement time (MT) congruency measure and subjective ratings. Physical practice improved kinesthetic MI ratings and observational practice improved visual MI ratings. Contrary to predictions, physical practice did not enhance timing congruency. Imagined MTs were longer in transfer after physical practice, suggesting MI was not based on the same representation. These data question ideas of equivalence, with poor temporal matching after no-vision physical practice.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103802"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142900260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How can virtual reality help to understand consciousness? A thematic analysis of students’ experiences in a novel virtual environment 虚拟现实如何帮助我们理解意识?学生在虚拟环境中的体验专题分析。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103792
Keelan R. Gorman, Aimee Wrightson-Hester, Michael Landman, Warren Mansell
Research on consciousness typically presents stimuli and records the responses that follow, to infer the intervening processes. Yet, VR allows ecological validity by giving the user freedom to continuously control their sensory input across three spatial dimensions via head and eye movement. We designed a virtual world in which the angle of view relates to the information complexity of the sensory input. We assessed its acceptability and feasibility, and explored the first-person experience. Ten university students were immersed in two different novel environments, then a semi-structured interview, guided by first-person video footage of the VR experience, elicited participants’ reports. The methodology proved feasible, and a thematic analysis was consistent with Mansell’s (2024) control theory perspective, and to a lesser degree, Integrated Information Theory (IIT) and Global Workspace Theory (GWT). We conclude that novel virtual environments provide an accessible, dynamic and valid way to gather evidence regarding different theories of consciousness.
对意识的研究通常呈现刺激并记录随之而来的反应,以推断干预过程。然而,VR通过让用户自由地通过头部和眼睛的运动来持续控制他们在三个空间维度上的感官输入,从而实现了生态有效性。我们设计了一个虚拟世界,其中的视角与感官输入的信息复杂性有关。我们评估了它的可接受性和可行性,并探索了第一人称体验。10名大学生沉浸在两个不同的新环境中,然后在VR体验的第一人称视频片段的指导下进行半结构化采访,引出参与者的报告。该方法被证明是可行的,并且主题分析与Mansell(2024)的控制论观点一致,并且在较小程度上与集成信息论(IIT)和全局工作空间理论(GWT)一致。我们的结论是,新颖的虚拟环境提供了一种可访问的、动态的和有效的方式来收集关于不同意识理论的证据。
{"title":"How can virtual reality help to understand consciousness? A thematic analysis of students’ experiences in a novel virtual environment","authors":"Keelan R. Gorman,&nbsp;Aimee Wrightson-Hester,&nbsp;Michael Landman,&nbsp;Warren Mansell","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103792","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103792","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Research on consciousness typically presents stimuli and records the responses that follow, to infer the intervening processes. Yet, VR allows ecological validity by giving the user freedom to continuously control their sensory input across three spatial dimensions via head and eye movement. We designed a virtual world in which the angle of view relates to the information complexity of the sensory input. We assessed its acceptability and feasibility, and explored the first-person experience. Ten university students were immersed in two different novel environments, then a semi-structured interview, guided by first-person video footage of the VR experience, elicited participants’ reports. The methodology proved feasible, and a thematic analysis was consistent with <span><span>Mansell’s (2024)</span></span> control theory perspective, and to a lesser degree, Integrated Information Theory (IIT) and Global Workspace Theory (GWT). We conclude that novel virtual environments provide an accessible, dynamic and valid way to gather evidence regarding different theories of consciousness.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103792"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142792831","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“The Giant Black Elephant with white Tusks stood in a field of Green Grass”: Cognitive and brain mechanisms underlying aphantasia “长着白色獠牙的巨型黑象站在绿草地上”:认知和大脑机制背后的幻觉
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103790
Paula Argueta , Julia Dominguez , Josie Zachman , Paul Worthington , Rajesh K. Kana
Aphantasia, a spectrum of inabilities creating and perceiving mental images, is becoming more of a focus in continued research to better understand functions of sensory perception and imagination. Current research on aphantasia is still in an era of exploration to find its underlying neural mechanisms, comorbidities and comparing levels of visual imagery to other cognitive functions. Through a systematic review, this article explores the most influential developments in aphantasia research. The search included 3 databases-PsycINFO, PubMed, and Web of Science. After a rigorous selection process, 52 studies are included in this review. The findings include new research themes across different studies such as relationships between aphantasia and diminished episodic and autobiographical memory, comorbidities including autism, attention, emotions, and neurobiological differences. By integrating diverse perspectives, this review aims to contribute to a deeper understanding of the cognitive processes underlying mental imagery and offers implications for further development in aphantasia research.
幻像症是一种创造和感知心理图像的一系列障碍,它正成为持续研究的焦点,以更好地理解感官知觉和想象的功能。目前对失视症的研究仍处于探索其潜在的神经机制、合并症以及视觉意象与其他认知功能水平比较的时代。本文通过系统回顾,探讨了幻像研究中最具影响力的进展。搜索包括3个数据库:psycinfo、PubMed和Web of Science。经过严格的筛选过程,本综述纳入了52项研究。这些发现包括不同研究的新研究主题,如缺失症与情景记忆和自传式记忆减少之间的关系,包括自闭症在内的合并症,注意力,情绪和神经生物学差异。通过对不同视角的整合,本综述旨在加深对心理意象认知过程的理解,并为幻影症研究的进一步发展提供启示。
{"title":"“The Giant Black Elephant with white Tusks stood in a field of Green Grass”: Cognitive and brain mechanisms underlying aphantasia","authors":"Paula Argueta ,&nbsp;Julia Dominguez ,&nbsp;Josie Zachman ,&nbsp;Paul Worthington ,&nbsp;Rajesh K. Kana","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103790","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103790","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Aphantasia, a spectrum of inabilities creating and perceiving mental images, is becoming more of a focus in continued research to better understand functions of sensory perception and imagination. Current research on aphantasia is still in an era of exploration to find its underlying neural mechanisms, comorbidities and comparing levels of visual imagery to other cognitive functions. Through a systematic review, this article explores the most influential developments in aphantasia research. The search included 3 databases-<em>PsycINFO, PubMed, and Web of Science.</em> After a rigorous selection process, 52 studies are included in this review. The findings include new research themes across different studies such as relationships between aphantasia and diminished episodic and autobiographical memory, comorbidities including autism, attention, emotions, and neurobiological differences. By integrating diverse perspectives, this review aims to contribute to a deeper understanding of the cognitive processes underlying mental imagery and offers implications for further development in aphantasia research.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103790"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142747028","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of the degree of action voluntariness on sense of agency in saccades 跳跳中行动自愿程度对代理感的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103793
Julian Gutzeit , Lynn Huestegge
Experiencing a sense of agency (SoA), the feeling of being in control over one’s actions and their outcomes, typically requires intentional and voluntary actions. Prior research has compared the association of voluntary versus completely involuntary actions with the SoA. Here, we leveraged unique characteristics of oculomotor actions to partially manipulate the degree of action voluntariness. Participants performed either highly automatized prosaccades or highly controlled (voluntary) anti-saccades, triggering a gaze-contingent visual action effect. We assessed explicit SoA ratings and temporal action and effect binding as an implicit SoA measure. Anti-saccades were associated with stronger action binding compared to prosaccades, demonstrating a robust association between higher action voluntariness and a stronger implicit sense of action agency. We conclude that our manipulation of action voluntariness may have impacted the implicit phenomenological feeling of bodily agency, but it did not affect the SoA over effect outcomes or explicit agency perception.
体验代理感(SoA),即控制自己的行为及其结果的感觉,通常需要有意和自愿的行为。先前的研究比较了SoA中自愿行为与完全非自愿行为的关联。在这里,我们利用动眼肌动作的独特特征来部分操纵动作自愿的程度。参与者要么进行高度自动化的扫视,要么进行高度受控的(自愿的)反扫视,从而触发随凝视而定的视觉动作效应。我们评估了显式SoA评级以及作为隐式SoA度量的时间动作和效果绑定。反扫视与前扫视相比,具有更强的动作约束,这表明更高的行动自愿性与更强的内隐行动代理感之间存在强大的关联。我们的结论是,我们对行动自愿的操纵可能影响了身体代理的内隐现象学感觉,但它不影响SoA效应结果或外显代理感知。
{"title":"The impact of the degree of action voluntariness on sense of agency in saccades","authors":"Julian Gutzeit ,&nbsp;Lynn Huestegge","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103793","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103793","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Experiencing a sense of agency (SoA), the feeling of being in control over one’s actions and their outcomes, typically requires intentional and voluntary actions. Prior research has compared the association of voluntary versus <em>completely</em> involuntary actions with the SoA. Here, we leveraged unique characteristics of oculomotor actions to <em>partially</em> manipulate the degree of action voluntariness. Participants performed either highly automatized prosaccades or highly controlled (voluntary) anti-saccades, triggering a gaze-contingent visual action effect. We assessed explicit SoA ratings and temporal action and effect binding as an implicit SoA measure. Anti-saccades were associated with stronger action binding compared to prosaccades, demonstrating a robust association between higher action voluntariness and a stronger implicit sense of action agency. We conclude that our manipulation of action voluntariness may have impacted the implicit phenomenological feeling of bodily agency, but it did not affect the SoA over effect outcomes or explicit agency perception.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"127 ","pages":"Article 103793"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142747159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Valence in perception: Are affective valence and visual brightness integral dimensions in visual experience? 感知中的情感:情绪情感和视觉亮度是视觉体验中不可或缺的两个维度吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103783
Hilla Jacobson , Zohar Ongil , Daniel Algom , Marius Usher
A fundamental question in the domain of affect and conscious perception is whether the former can impact the latter. Traditionally, perception and affect were conceived as largely independent. Against this backdrop, it was recently argued that the affective valence of a stimulus can modulate the perceptual experience of its sensory features. An alternative hypothesis is that perceptual experiences have a valenced aspect over and above their sensory aspects, with these two aspects interacting and comprising integral perceptual dimensions. To test this, we carried out two experiments deploying Wendell Garner’s speeded classification paradigm to decide whether visual brightness and affective valence are separable or integral dimensions. We found Garner interference, documenting that brightness and valence are integral dimensions. We did not observe effects of congruity – responses to bright positive stimuli were not faster than to bright negative stimuli — providing no support for affect induced changes in the perception of brightness.
情感和有意识感知领域的一个基本问题是,前者是否会影响后者。传统上,人们认为知觉和情感在很大程度上是独立的。在此背景下,最近有一种观点认为,刺激物的情感价值可以调节对其感官特征的知觉体验。另一种假设是,知觉体验除了感官方面之外,还有情感方面,这两个方面相互作用,构成了完整的知觉维度。为了验证这一点,我们采用温德尔-加纳(Wendell Garner)的加速分类范式进行了两项实验,以确定视觉亮度和情感价位是可分离的还是不可分割的维度。我们没有观察到一致性的影响--对明亮的正面刺激的反应速度并不比对明亮的负面刺激的反应速度快--这就不支持情感会诱发亮度感知的变化。
{"title":"Valence in perception: Are affective valence and visual brightness integral dimensions in visual experience?","authors":"Hilla Jacobson ,&nbsp;Zohar Ongil ,&nbsp;Daniel Algom ,&nbsp;Marius Usher","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103783","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103783","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>A fundamental question in the domain of affect and conscious perception is whether the former can impact the latter. Traditionally, perception and affect were conceived as largely independent. Against this backdrop, it was recently argued that the affective valence of a stimulus can modulate the perceptual experience of its sensory features. An alternative hypothesis is that perceptual experiences have a valenced aspect over and above their sensory aspects, with these two aspects interacting and comprising integral perceptual dimensions. To test this, we carried out two experiments deploying Wendell Garner’s speeded classification paradigm to decide whether visual brightness and affective valence are <em>separable</em> or <em>integral</em> dimensions.<!--> <!-->We found Garner interference, documenting that brightness and valence are integral dimensions. We did not observe effects of congruity – responses to bright positive stimuli were not faster than to bright negative stimuli — providing no support for affect induced changes in the perception of brightness.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"126 ","pages":"Article 103783"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142632367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Better bridges: Integrating the neuroscience and philosophy of consciousness 更好的桥梁:整合神经科学与意识哲学。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103774
Benjamin Kozuch
Contemporary consciousness research has given rise to numerous theories in both the philosophical and neuroscientific domains (such as higher-order theory and global neuronal workspace), raising the question as to how well each is supported. This article develops a relatively novel method for determining this, which is to use evidence, not just from a theory’s own domain, but also from its complementary domain (e.g., neuroscientific evidence is used to judge a philosophical theory, and vice versa). This approach works when a neuroscientific and a philosophical theory are conceptually linked, allowing evidence confirming or disconfirming one theory to do the same for the other. After developing this method, the article uses it to draw conclusions concerning some of our leading neuroscientific and philosophical theories of consciousness, including first- and second-order representationalism and theories emphasizing the prefrontal cortex’s role in consciousness.
当代意识研究在哲学和神经科学领域(如高阶理论和全局神经元工作空间)都产生了许多理论,这就提出了一个问题:每种理论的支持程度如何?本文提出了一种相对新颖的判断方法,即不仅使用理论自身领域的证据,还使用其互补领域的证据(例如,使用神经科学证据来判断哲学理论,反之亦然)。当神经科学理论和哲学理论在概念上相互关联时,这种方法就能发挥作用,使证实或不证实一种理论的证据也能用于另一种理论。在发展了这种方法之后,文章用它来得出一些关于意识的主要神经科学和哲学理论的结论,包括一阶和二阶表象主义以及强调前额叶皮层在意识中的作用的理论。
{"title":"Better bridges: Integrating the neuroscience and philosophy of consciousness","authors":"Benjamin Kozuch","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103774","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103774","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Contemporary consciousness research has given rise to numerous theories in both the philosophical and neuroscientific domains (such as higher-order theory and global neuronal workspace), raising the question as to how well each is supported. This article develops a relatively novel method for determining this, which is to use evidence, not just from a theory’s own domain, but also from its complementary domain (e.g., neuroscientific evidence is used to judge a philosophical theory, and vice versa). This approach works when a neuroscientific and a philosophical theory are conceptually linked, allowing evidence confirming or disconfirming one theory to do the same for the other. After developing this method, the article uses it to draw conclusions concerning some of our leading neuroscientific and philosophical theories of consciousness, including first- and second-order representationalism and theories emphasizing the prefrontal cortex’s role in consciousness.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"126 ","pages":"Article 103774"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142569217","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How mortality awareness regulates intertemporal Choice: A joint effect of endpoint reminder and retrospective episodic thinking 死亡意识如何调节时际选择:终点提醒和回溯性偶发思维的共同作用
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103787
Peng Wang , XT (XiaoTian) Wang
Although life is lived forward, it can only be understood backward. From the perspective of life-history theory, we propose that mortality reminders may induce backward thinking from the endpoint of life and regulate intertemporal choice between smaller-and-sooner (SS) and large-and-later rewards (LL), thereby helping individuals with time management. Experiment 1 compared the effects of mortality (endpoint) imagination with the imagination of being at age 70 and found that only mortality imagination significantly reduced delay discounting, the extent to which a delayed reward is discounted. In Experiment 2, mortality reminders companied by retrospective episodic mental time travel significantly reduced delay discounting compared to prospective episodic processing. However, the effect disappeared when the end-of-life reminder was replaced with an old-age (70 years) reminder. Highlighting the inevitability of death, coupled with retrospective episodic thinking, promoted future-oriented preferences, suggesting that end-of-life meditation is likely to induce retrospective mental time travels.
虽然生命是向前延续的,但对它的理解却只能是向后的。从生命史理论的角度出发,我们提出,死亡提醒可能会诱发从生命终点开始的逆向思维,并调节在较小且较快的回报(SS)和较大且较晚的回报(LL)之间的时际选择,从而帮助个体进行时间管理。实验 1 比较了死亡(终点)想象和 70 岁想象的效果,发现只有死亡想象能显著降低延迟折现,即延迟奖励的折现程度。在实验 2 中,与前瞻性历时性处理相比,伴随着回溯性历时性心理时空旅行的死亡提醒能显著降低延迟折现。然而,当临终提醒换成老年(70 岁)提醒时,这种效果就消失了。强调死亡的不可避免性,再加上回顾性历时思维,促进了面向未来的偏好,这表明临终冥想很可能会诱发回顾性心理时空旅行。
{"title":"How mortality awareness regulates intertemporal Choice: A joint effect of endpoint reminder and retrospective episodic thinking","authors":"Peng Wang ,&nbsp;XT (XiaoTian) Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103787","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103787","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Although life is lived forward, it can only be understood backward. From the perspective of life-history theory, we propose that mortality reminders may induce backward thinking from the endpoint of life and regulate intertemporal choice between smaller-and-sooner (SS) and large-and-later rewards (LL), thereby helping individuals with time management. Experiment 1 compared the effects of mortality (endpoint) imagination with the imagination of being at age 70 and found that only mortality imagination significantly reduced delay discounting, the extent to which a delayed reward is discounted. In Experiment 2, mortality reminders companied by retrospective episodic mental time travel significantly reduced delay discounting compared to prospective episodic processing. However, the effect disappeared when the end-of-life reminder was replaced with an old-age (70 years) reminder. Highlighting the inevitability of death, coupled with retrospective episodic thinking, promoted future-oriented preferences, suggesting that end-of-life meditation is likely to induce retrospective mental time travels.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"126 ","pages":"Article 103787"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142660932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multiple independent components contribute to event-related potential correlates of conscious vision 多个独立成分对有意识视觉的事件相关电位相关性做出了贡献。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103785
Elisabetta Colombari , Henry Railo
Research has revealed two major event-related potential (ERP) markers of visual awareness: the earlier Visual Awareness Negativity (VAN, around 150–250 ms after stimulus onset), and the following Late Positivity (LP, around 300–500 ms after stimulus onset). Understanding the neural sources that give rise to VAN and LP is important in order to understand what kind of neural processes underlie conscious visual perception. Although the ERPs afford high temporal resolution, their spatial resolution is limited because multiple separate neural sources sum up at the scalp level. In the present study, we sought to characterize the locations and time-courses of independent neural sources underlying the ERP correlates of visual awareness by means of Independent Component Analysis (ICA). ICA allows identifying and localizing the temporal dynamics of different neural sources that contribute to the ERP correlates of conscious perception. The present results show that the cortical sources of VAN are localized to posterior areas including occipital and temporal cortex, while LP reflects a combination of multiple sources distributed among frontal, parietal and occipito-temporal cortex. Our findings suggest that conscious vision correlates with dynamically changing neural sources, developing in part in “accumulative fashion”: consciousness-related activity initially arises in few early sources and, subsequently, additional sources are engaged as a function of time.
The results further suggest that even early latency neural sources that correlate with conscious perception may also associate with action-related processes.
研究揭示了视觉意识的两个主要事件相关电位(ERP)标记:早期的视觉意识负性(VAN,刺激开始后约 150-250 毫秒)和随后的晚期正性(LP,刺激开始后约 300-500 毫秒)。了解引起 VAN 和 LP 的神经源对于了解有意识视觉感知的神经过程非常重要。虽然ERPs具有很高的时间分辨率,但其空间分辨率却很有限,因为多个独立的神经源会在头皮水平相加。在本研究中,我们试图通过独立分量分析(ICA)来描述视觉意识的ERP相关性所依赖的独立神经源的位置和时间序列。ICA 可以识别和定位不同神经源的时间动态,这些神经源对有意识感知的 ERP 相关性做出了贡献。目前的研究结果表明,VAN 的皮质来源定位在后部区域,包括枕叶和颞叶皮质,而 LP 则反映了分布在额叶、顶叶和枕颞皮质的多个来源的组合。我们的研究结果表明,有意识视觉与动态变化的神经源相关,部分神经源以 "累积方式 "发展:与意识相关的活动最初出现在少数早期神经源中,随后随着时间的推移,更多的神经源参与进来。研究结果进一步表明,即使是与有意识感知相关的早期潜伏期神经源也可能与行动相关过程有关。
{"title":"Multiple independent components contribute to event-related potential correlates of conscious vision","authors":"Elisabetta Colombari ,&nbsp;Henry Railo","doi":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103785","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.concog.2024.103785","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Research has revealed two major event-related potential (ERP) markers of visual awareness: the earlier Visual Awareness Negativity (VAN, around 150–250 ms after stimulus onset), and the following Late Positivity (LP, around 300–500 ms after stimulus onset). Understanding the neural sources that give rise to VAN and LP is important in order to understand what kind of neural processes underlie conscious visual perception. Although the ERPs afford high temporal resolution, their spatial resolution is limited because multiple separate neural sources sum up at the scalp level. In the present study, we sought to characterize the locations and time-courses of independent neural sources underlying the ERP correlates of visual awareness by means of Independent Component Analysis (ICA). ICA allows identifying and localizing the temporal dynamics of different neural sources that contribute to the ERP correlates of conscious perception. The present results show that the cortical sources of VAN are localized to posterior areas including occipital and temporal cortex, while LP reflects a combination of multiple sources distributed among frontal, parietal and occipito-temporal cortex. Our findings suggest that conscious vision correlates with dynamically changing neural sources, developing in part in “accumulative fashion”: consciousness-related activity initially arises in few early sources and, subsequently, additional sources are engaged as a function of time.</div><div>The results further suggest that even early latency neural sources that correlate with conscious perception may also associate with action-related processes.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51358,"journal":{"name":"Consciousness and Cognition","volume":"126 ","pages":"Article 103785"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142632366","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Consciousness and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1