首页 > 最新文献

Public Opinion Quarterly最新文献

英文 中文
Lying for Trump? Elite Cue-Taking and Expressive Responding on Vote Method 为特朗普撒谎?精英对投票方式的暗示与表达性回应
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-31 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfac045
Enrijeta Shino, Daniel A. Smith, Laura Uribe
Might elite cues affect how we vote? Extant literature focuses on effects of elite cues on candidate evaluation or policy preference, but we know little about how they might affect vote method preferences. Drawing on a large survey of validated Florida voters, including those who regularly vote by mail, we find that retrospective and prospective misreporting of vote method prior to the 2020 General Election was driven primarily by support for Trump. The president’s supporters who were most politically aware were most likely to disavow their own voting by mail and misreport their anticipated vote method in the November election. Understanding the effects—and limits—of elite cues on the politicization of self-reported political behavior has important implications for pollsters and campaigns, election administrators, voters, and the broader democratic electoral process.
精英暗示会影响我们的投票方式吗?现有文献关注精英线索对候选人评价或政策偏好的影响,但我们对他们如何影响投票方法偏好知之甚少。通过对佛罗里达州有效选民(包括那些经常通过邮件投票的选民)的大规模调查,我们发现,在2020年大选之前,对投票方法的回顾性和前瞻性误报主要是由对特朗普的支持推动的。最有政治意识的总统支持者最有可能否认自己通过邮件投票,并在11月的选举中错误报告他们预期的投票方式。了解精英线索对自我报告的政治行为政治化的影响和限制,对民意测验专家和竞选活动、选举管理人员、选民和更广泛的民主选举过程具有重要意义。
{"title":"Lying for Trump? Elite Cue-Taking and Expressive Responding on Vote Method","authors":"Enrijeta Shino, Daniel A. Smith, Laura Uribe","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfac045","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfac045","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Might elite cues affect how we vote? Extant literature focuses on effects of elite cues on candidate evaluation or policy preference, but we know little about how they might affect vote method preferences. Drawing on a large survey of validated Florida voters, including those who regularly vote by mail, we find that retrospective and prospective misreporting of vote method prior to the 2020 General Election was driven primarily by support for Trump. The president’s supporters who were most politically aware were most likely to disavow their own voting by mail and misreport their anticipated vote method in the November election. Understanding the effects—and limits—of elite cues on the politicization of self-reported political behavior has important implications for pollsters and campaigns, election administrators, voters, and the broader democratic electoral process.","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44374993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Does Social Desirability Bias Distort Survey Analyses of Ideology and Self-Interest? Evidence from a List Experiment on Progressive Taxation 社会期望偏差是否扭曲了意识形态和自我利益的调查分析?累进税清单实验的证据
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-31 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfac050
Tobias Heide-Jørgensen
The relative importance of ideological orientations and material self-interest as determinants of political attitudes is still discussed. Using a novel list experiment on opposition to progressive taxation embedded in a large representative Danish online survey (N = 2,010), I study how social desirability concerns bias the conclusions survey researchers draw regarding the influence of self-interest (gauged by income) and ideology (measured by left-right self-identifications) on public opinion. I find that right-wingers are much less opposed to progressive taxation when attitudes are measured indirectly and unobtrusively by means of the list experiment relative to asking directly about their opinions. In fact, rightists are no more against progressive taxation than leftists and centrists. Furthermore, opposition to tax progressivity is considerably lower among low-income individuals when social desirability bias is addressed, thereby increasing the attitudinal gap between low- and high-income individuals. The implications of the findings are that survey research risks exaggerating the importance of ideological orientations and underestimating how much political views reflect material self-interest.
意识形态取向和物质私利作为政治态度决定因素的相对重要性仍在讨论中。在丹麦一项具有代表性的大型在线调查中,使用了一项关于反对累进税的新颖列表实验(N = 2010年),我研究了社会可取性是如何使调查研究人员得出的关于利己主义(以收入衡量)和意识形态(以左右自我认同衡量)对公众舆论影响的结论产生偏见的。我发现,与直接询问他们的意见相比,当通过列表实验间接而不引人注目地衡量态度时,右翼分子对累进税的反对要少得多。事实上,右派并不比左派和中间派更反对累进税。此外,当社会愿望偏见得到解决时,低收入个人对税收累进性的反对要低得多,从而增加了低收入和高收入个人之间的态度差距。调查结果的含义是,调查研究有夸大意识形态取向重要性和低估政治观点在多大程度上反映物质私利的风险。
{"title":"Does Social Desirability Bias Distort Survey Analyses of Ideology and Self-Interest? Evidence from a List Experiment on Progressive Taxation","authors":"Tobias Heide-Jørgensen","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfac050","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfac050","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The relative importance of ideological orientations and material self-interest as determinants of political attitudes is still discussed. Using a novel list experiment on opposition to progressive taxation embedded in a large representative Danish online survey (N = 2,010), I study how social desirability concerns bias the conclusions survey researchers draw regarding the influence of self-interest (gauged by income) and ideology (measured by left-right self-identifications) on public opinion. I find that right-wingers are much less opposed to progressive taxation when attitudes are measured indirectly and unobtrusively by means of the list experiment relative to asking directly about their opinions. In fact, rightists are no more against progressive taxation than leftists and centrists. Furthermore, opposition to tax progressivity is considerably lower among low-income individuals when social desirability bias is addressed, thereby increasing the attitudinal gap between low- and high-income individuals. The implications of the findings are that survey research risks exaggerating the importance of ideological orientations and underestimating how much political views reflect material self-interest.","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45297533","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Strategic Discrimination in the 2020 Democratic Primary 2020年民主党初选中的战略歧视
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-31 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfac051
Jon Green, Brian F. Schaffner, Sam Luks
Primary voters frequently support the candidates they think have a greater chance of winning the general election over the candidates who most closely reflect their policy preferences—a perception referred to as “electability.” While electability is typically taken to mean ideological moderation, recent research highlights the potential for candidates’ demographic characteristics to affect such perceptions. Using a conjoint experiment conducted with a sample of nearly 3,000 likely Democratic primary voters in June 2019, we show that women and candidates of color were seen as less electable than their white, male counterparts despite being preferred more frequently, holding policy stances and general election strategies constant. These effects were independent of respondents’ hostile sexism and racial resentment, and mediation analysis indicates that electability concerns reduced overall support for women and candidates of color. The results replicate and extend recent findings related to “strategic discrimination” in the US electorate.
初选选民经常支持他们认为比最能反映他们政策偏好的候选人更有机会赢得大选的候选人,这种看法被称为“可选举性”。虽然可选举性通常被认为意味着意识形态温和,最近的研究强调了候选人的人口特征可能会影响这种看法。2019年6月,我们对近3000名可能的民主党初选选民进行了一项联合实验,结果表明,尽管女性和有色人种候选人更受青睐,政策立场和大选策略不变,但她们的当选率低于白人和男性候选人。这些影响与受访者敌对的性别歧视和种族怨恨无关,调解分析表明,对可选举性的担忧降低了对女性和有色人种候选人的总体支持。这一结果复制并扩展了最近与美国选民“战略歧视”有关的研究结果。
{"title":"Strategic Discrimination in the 2020 Democratic Primary","authors":"Jon Green, Brian F. Schaffner, Sam Luks","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfac051","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfac051","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Primary voters frequently support the candidates they think have a greater chance of winning the general election over the candidates who most closely reflect their policy preferences—a perception referred to as “electability.” While electability is typically taken to mean ideological moderation, recent research highlights the potential for candidates’ demographic characteristics to affect such perceptions. Using a conjoint experiment conducted with a sample of nearly 3,000 likely Democratic primary voters in June 2019, we show that women and candidates of color were seen as less electable than their white, male counterparts despite being preferred more frequently, holding policy stances and general election strategies constant. These effects were independent of respondents’ hostile sexism and racial resentment, and mediation analysis indicates that electability concerns reduced overall support for women and candidates of color. The results replicate and extend recent findings related to “strategic discrimination” in the US electorate.","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41380943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Updating amidst Disagreement: New Experimental Evidence on Partisan Cues 分歧中的更新:党派线索的新实验证据
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-31 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfac053
Anthony Fowler, William G. Howell
In this era of hyper-polarization and partisan animosity, do people incorporate the viewpoints of their political opponents? Perhaps not. An important body of research, in fact, finds that the provision of information about opponents’ policy views leads survey respondents to reflexively adopt the opposite position. In this paper, we demonstrate that such findings arise from incomplete experimental designs and a particular measurement strategy. In a series of experiments that vary information about both parties’ positions simultaneously and that solicit continuous, rather than discrete, policy positions, we find that partisans update their beliefs in accordance with the positions of Republican and Democratic leaders alike. Partisans are not perennially determined to disagree. Rather, they are often willing to incorporate opposing viewpoints about a wide range of policy issues.
在这个高度两极分化和党派仇恨的时代,人们是否融入了政治对手的观点?也许不是。事实上,一项重要的研究发现,提供反对者政策观点的信息会导致受访者本能地采取相反的立场。在本文中,我们证明了这些发现是由不完整的实验设计和特定的测量策略引起的。在一系列实验中,我们发现党派人士根据共和党和民主党领导人的立场来更新他们的信仰,这些实验同时改变了有关两党立场的信息,并寻求持续而非离散的政策立场。党派人士并不总是下定决心不同意。相反,他们往往愿意在广泛的政策问题上纳入相反的观点。
{"title":"Updating amidst Disagreement: New Experimental Evidence on Partisan Cues","authors":"Anthony Fowler, William G. Howell","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfac053","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfac053","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In this era of hyper-polarization and partisan animosity, do people incorporate the viewpoints of their political opponents? Perhaps not. An important body of research, in fact, finds that the provision of information about opponents’ policy views leads survey respondents to reflexively adopt the opposite position. In this paper, we demonstrate that such findings arise from incomplete experimental designs and a particular measurement strategy. In a series of experiments that vary information about both parties’ positions simultaneously and that solicit continuous, rather than discrete, policy positions, we find that partisans update their beliefs in accordance with the positions of Republican and Democratic leaders alike. Partisans are not perennially determined to disagree. Rather, they are often willing to incorporate opposing viewpoints about a wide range of policy issues.","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47326963","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Increasing the Acceptance of Smartphone-Based Data Collection. 提高智能手机数据收集的接受度。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad019
Alexander Wenz, Florian Keusch

To study human behavior, social scientists are increasingly collecting data from mobile apps and sensors embedded in smartphones. A major challenge of studies implemented on general population samples, however, is that participation rates are rather low. While previous research has started to investigate the factors affecting individuals' decision to participate in such studies, less is known about features of the study design which are under the researcher's control and can increase the acceptance of smartphone-based data collection methods. Guided by the Technology Acceptance Model, we varied study characteristics in a vignette experiment to examine their effect on individuals' willingness to download a research app on their smartphone. Data were collected from 1,876 members of the NORC AmeriSpeak Panel, a probability-based panel of the general population aged 18+ in the United States. Respondents were randomly assigned to eight vignettes and, after each vignette, were asked to rate their willingness to participate in the described hypothetical study. The results show that individuals are more willing to participate in smartphone-based studies where they have some control over the data collection process, by having the option either to temporarily switch off the data collection or to review the data before submission. Furthermore, they are more willing to participate in research to which they are invited via postal letter rather than receiving a postal letter plus a phone call from an interviewer who walks them through the app installation. Finally, unconditional incentives increase their willingness to engage with smartphone-based data collection over conditional incentives.

为了研究人类行为,社会科学家越来越多地从移动应用程序和嵌入智能手机的传感器中收集数据。然而,对一般人口样本进行研究的一个主要挑战是参与率相当低。虽然之前的研究已经开始调查影响个人决定参与此类研究的因素,但对研究设计的特征知之甚少,这些特征在研究人员的控制下,可以增加对基于智能手机的数据收集方法的接受度。在技术接受模型的指导下,我们在一个小插曲实验中改变了研究特征,以检验它们对个人在智能手机上下载研究应用程序的意愿的影响。数据收集自NORC AmeriSpeak小组的1876名成员,该小组是一个基于概率的小组,主要针对美国18岁以上的普通人群。受访者被随机分配到八个小插曲中,在每个小插曲之后,被要求评估他们参与所描述的假设研究的意愿。结果表明,个人更愿意参与基于智能手机的研究,他们对数据收集过程有一定的控制,可以选择暂时关闭数据收集或在提交之前查看数据。此外,他们更愿意参与通过邮寄信件邀请他们参加的研究,而不是通过邮寄信件和面试官的电话来指导他们安装应用程序。最后,与有条件激励相比,无条件激励会增加他们参与基于智能手机的数据收集的意愿。
{"title":"Increasing the Acceptance of Smartphone-Based Data Collection.","authors":"Alexander Wenz,&nbsp;Florian Keusch","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad019","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad019","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To study human behavior, social scientists are increasingly collecting data from mobile apps and sensors embedded in smartphones. A major challenge of studies implemented on general population samples, however, is that participation rates are rather low. While previous research has started to investigate the factors affecting individuals' decision to participate in such studies, less is known about features of the study design which are under the researcher's control and can increase the acceptance of smartphone-based data collection methods. Guided by the Technology Acceptance Model, we varied study characteristics in a vignette experiment to examine their effect on individuals' willingness to download a research app on their smartphone. Data were collected from 1,876 members of the NORC AmeriSpeak Panel, a probability-based panel of the general population aged 18+ in the United States. Respondents were randomly assigned to eight vignettes and, after each vignette, were asked to rate their willingness to participate in the described hypothetical study. The results show that individuals are more willing to participate in smartphone-based studies where they have some control over the data collection process, by having the option either to temporarily switch off the data collection or to review the data before submission. Furthermore, they are more willing to participate in research to which they are invited via postal letter rather than receiving a postal letter plus a phone call from an interviewer who walks them through the app installation. Finally, unconditional incentives increase their willingness to engage with smartphone-based data collection over conditional incentives.</p>","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 2","pages":"357-388"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/35/22/nfad019.PMC10339101.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9823636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Income Source Confusion Using the SILC. 使用SILC混淆收入来源。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad025
Christopher Robert Bollinger, Iva Valentinova Tasseva

We use a unique panel of household survey data-the Austrian version of the European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions (SILC) for 2008-2011-which have been linked to individual administrative records on both state unemployment benefits and earnings. We assess the extent and structure of misreporting across similar benefits and between benefits and earnings. We document that many respondents fail to report participation in one or more of the unemployment programs. Moreover, they inflate earnings for periods when they are unemployed but receiving unemployment compensation. To demonstrate the impact of income source confusion on estimators, we estimate standard Mincer wage equations. Since unemployment is associated with lower education, the reports of unemployment benefits as earnings bias downward the returns to education. Failure to report unemployment benefits also leads to substantial sample bias when selecting on these benefits, as one might in estimating the returns to job training.

我们使用了一组独特的家庭调查数据——奥地利版的2008-2011年欧盟收入和生活条件统计数据(SILC)——这些数据与国家失业救济和收入的个人行政记录相关联。我们评估了类似福利之间以及福利和收入之间误报的程度和结构。我们记录了许多受访者没有报告参与一个或多个失业计划。此外,他们在失业但领取失业补偿金的时期会夸大收入。为了证明收入来源混淆对估计者的影响,我们估计了标准明瑟工资方程。由于失业与较低的受教育程度有关,因此将失业救济金作为收入的报告倾向于降低教育回报。在选择这些福利时,未报告失业福利也会导致大量的样本偏差,就像在估计职业培训的回报时一样。
{"title":"Income Source Confusion Using the SILC.","authors":"Christopher Robert Bollinger,&nbsp;Iva Valentinova Tasseva","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad025","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad025","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We use a unique panel of household survey data-the Austrian version of the European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions (SILC) for 2008-2011-which have been linked to individual administrative records on both state unemployment benefits and earnings. We assess the extent and structure of misreporting across similar benefits and between benefits and earnings. We document that many respondents fail to report participation in one or more of the unemployment programs. Moreover, they inflate earnings for periods when they are unemployed but receiving unemployment compensation. To demonstrate the impact of income source confusion on estimators, we estimate standard Mincer wage equations. Since unemployment is associated with lower education, the reports of unemployment benefits as earnings bias downward the returns to education. Failure to report unemployment benefits also leads to substantial sample bias when selecting on these benefits, as one might in estimating the returns to job training.</p>","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 Suppl 1","pages":"542-574"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10496567/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10252680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Public Opinion and Cyberterrorism. 舆论与网络恐怖主义。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad006
Ryan Shandler, Nadiya Kostyuk, Harry Oppenheimer

Research into cyber-conflict, public opinion, and international security is burgeoning, yet the field suffers from an absence of conceptual agreement about key terms. For instance, every time a cyberattack takes place, a public debate erupts as to whether it constitutes cyberterrorism. This debate bears significant consequences, seeing as the ascription of a "terrorism" label enables the application of heavy-handed counterterrorism powers and heightens the level of perceived threat among the public. In light of widespread conceptual disagreement in cyberspace, we assert that public opinion plays a heightened role in understanding the nature of cyber threats. We construct a typological framework to illuminate the attributes that drive the public classification of an attack as cyberterrorism, which we test through a ratings-based conjoint experiment in the United States, the United Kingdom, and Israel (N = 21,238 observations). We find that the public (1) refrains from labeling attacks by unknown actors or hacker collectives as cyberterrorism; and (2) classifies attacks that disseminate sensitive data as terrorism to a greater extent even than physically explosive attacks. Importantly, the uniform public perspectives across the three countries challenge a foundational tenet of public opinion and international relations scholarship that divided views among elites on foreign policy matters will be reflected by a divided public. This study concludes by providing a definitive conceptual baseline to support future research on the topic.

对网络冲突、公众舆论和国际安全的研究正在蓬勃发展,但该领域对关键术语缺乏概念上的一致。例如,每次发生网络攻击,就会爆发一场关于是否构成网络恐怖主义的公开辩论。这场辩论产生了重要的后果,因为给“恐怖主义”贴上标签,可以使严厉的反恐力量得以运用,并提高公众对威胁的感知程度。鉴于网络空间中普遍存在的概念分歧,我们认为公众舆论在理解网络威胁的本质方面发挥着重要作用。我们构建了一个类型框架来阐明驱动攻击作为网络恐怖主义的公共分类的属性,我们通过基于评级的联合实验在美国,英国和以色列进行了测试(N = 21,238个观察值)。我们发现公众(1)避免将未知行为者或黑客集体的攻击标记为网络恐怖主义;(2)将传播敏感数据的攻击归类为恐怖主义,甚至比物理爆炸攻击更大。重要的是,这三个国家统一的公众观点挑战了公众舆论和国际关系学术的基本原则,即精英阶层对外交政策问题的分歧意见将反映在分歧的公众中。本研究的结论是提供了一个明确的概念基线,以支持未来对该主题的研究。
{"title":"Public Opinion and Cyberterrorism.","authors":"Ryan Shandler,&nbsp;Nadiya Kostyuk,&nbsp;Harry Oppenheimer","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad006","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research into cyber-conflict, public opinion, and international security is burgeoning, yet the field suffers from an absence of conceptual agreement about key terms. For instance, every time a cyberattack takes place, a public debate erupts as to whether it constitutes <i>cyberterrorism</i>. This debate bears significant consequences, seeing as the ascription of a \"terrorism\" label enables the application of heavy-handed counterterrorism powers and heightens the level of perceived threat among the public. In light of widespread conceptual disagreement in cyberspace, we assert that public opinion plays a heightened role in understanding the nature of cyber threats. We construct a typological framework to illuminate the attributes that drive the public classification of an attack as cyberterrorism, which we test through a ratings-based conjoint experiment in the United States, the United Kingdom, and Israel (N = 21,238 observations). We find that the public (1) refrains from labeling attacks by unknown actors or hacker collectives as cyberterrorism; and (2) classifies attacks that disseminate sensitive data as terrorism to a greater extent even than physically explosive attacks. Importantly, the uniform public perspectives across the three countries challenge a foundational tenet of public opinion and international relations scholarship that divided views among elites on foreign policy matters will be reflected by a divided public. This study concludes by providing a definitive conceptual baseline to support future research on the topic.</p>","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 1","pages":"92-119"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10127534/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9363802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Affective Polarization in Comparative and Longitudinal Perspective. 比较与纵向视角下的情感极化。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad004
Diego Garzia, Frederico Ferreira da Silva, Simon Maye

Existent research shows that affective polarization has been intensifying in some publics, diminishing in others, and remaining stable in most. We contribute to this debate by providing the most encompassing comparative and longitudinal account of affective polarization so far. We resort to a newly assembled dataset able to track partisan affect, with varying time series, in eighteen democracies over the last six decades. We present results based on two different operational measures of affective polarization: Reiljan's Affective Polarization Index, based on reported partisans only, and Wagner's weighted distance from the most liked party, based on the whole electorate. Our reassessment of affective polarization among partisans confirms that an intensifying trend is observable in a number of countries but it is, by no means, generalizable to all established democracies. Regarding the longitudinal assessment of affective polarization among the electorate, we confirm that US citizens have become more affectively polarized over time.

现有的研究表明,情感两极分化在一些公众中加剧,在另一些公众中减弱,但在大多数公众中保持稳定。我们通过提供迄今为止最全面的情感两极分化的比较和纵向说明来促进这场辩论。我们采用了一个新组装的数据集,该数据集能够追踪过去六十年来18个民主国家中不同时间序列的党派影响。我们提出的结果基于两种不同的情感极化的操作措施:Reiljan的情感极化指数,仅基于报告的党派,和瓦格纳的加权距离最受欢迎的政党,基于全体选民。我们对党派之间情感两极分化的重新评估证实,在一些国家可以观察到一种加剧的趋势,但这绝不是所有老牌民主国家的普遍趋势。关于选民情感两极分化的纵向评估,我们确认,随着时间的推移,美国公民已经变得更加情感两极分化。
{"title":"Affective Polarization in Comparative and Longitudinal Perspective.","authors":"Diego Garzia,&nbsp;Frederico Ferreira da Silva,&nbsp;Simon Maye","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Existent research shows that affective polarization has been intensifying in some publics, diminishing in others, and remaining stable in most. We contribute to this debate by providing the most encompassing comparative and longitudinal account of affective polarization so far. We resort to a newly assembled dataset able to track partisan affect, with varying time series, in eighteen democracies over the last six decades. We present results based on two different operational measures of affective polarization: Reiljan's Affective Polarization Index, based on reported partisans only, and Wagner's weighted distance from the most liked party, based on the whole electorate. Our reassessment of affective polarization among partisans confirms that an intensifying trend is observable in a number of countries but it is, by no means, generalizable to all established democracies. Regarding the longitudinal assessment of affective polarization among the electorate, we confirm that US citizens have become more affectively polarized over time.</p>","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 1","pages":"219-231"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10127533/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9718987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Truth and Bias, Left and Right: Testing Ideological Asymmetries with a Realistic News Supply. 真相与偏见,左与右:用现实的新闻供给检验意识形态的不对称性。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad013
Bernhard Clemm von Hohenberg

The debate around "fake news" has raised the question of whether liberals and conservatives differ, first, in their ability to discern true from false information, and second, in their tendency to give more credit to information that is ideologically congruent. Typical designs to measure these asymmetries select, often arbitrarily, a small set of news items as experimental stimuli without clear reference to a "population of information." This pre-registered study takes an alternative approach by, first, conceptualizing estimands in relation to all political news. Second, to represent this target population, it uses a set of 80 randomly sampled items from a large collection of articles from Google News and three fact-checking sites. In a subsequent survey, a quota sample of US participants (n = 1,393) indicate whether they believe the news items to be true. Conservatives are less truth-discerning than liberals, but also less affected by the congruence of news.

围绕“假新闻”的辩论引发了这样一个问题:首先,自由派和保守派在辨别真假信息的能力上是否存在差异;其次,他们倾向于更多地相信与意识形态一致的信息。衡量这些不对称性的典型设计通常是武断地选择一小部分新闻作为实验刺激,而没有明确提及“信息总体”。这项预先登记的研究采用了另一种方法,首先,将与所有政治新闻相关的估计概念化。其次,为了表示这一目标人群,它从谷歌新闻和三个事实核查网站的大量文章中随机抽取了80个项目。在随后的一项调查中,美国参与者的配额样本(n = 1393)表明他们是否相信新闻是真实的。保守派对真相的辨别能力不如自由派,但也不太受新闻一致性的影响。
{"title":"Truth and Bias, Left and Right: Testing Ideological Asymmetries with a Realistic News Supply.","authors":"Bernhard Clemm von Hohenberg","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad013","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The debate around \"fake news\" has raised the question of whether liberals and conservatives differ, first, in their ability to discern true from false information, and second, in their tendency to give more credit to information that is ideologically congruent. Typical designs to measure these asymmetries select, often arbitrarily, a small set of news items as experimental stimuli without clear reference to a \"population of information.\" This pre-registered study takes an alternative approach by, first, conceptualizing estimands in relation to all political news. Second, to represent this target population, it uses a set of 80 randomly sampled items from a large collection of articles from Google News and three fact-checking sites. In a subsequent survey, a quota sample of US participants (n = 1,393) indicate whether they believe the news items to be true. Conservatives are less truth-discerning than liberals, but also less affected by the congruence of news.</p>","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 2","pages":"267-292"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10371040/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9889896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Augmenting Surveys with Paradata, Administrative Data, and Contextual Data. 扩充调查与Paradata,行政数据,和上下文数据。
IF 3.4 1区 社会学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/poq/nfad026
Joseph W Sakshaug, Bella Struminskaya
Over the last decades, there has been growing interest in augmenting survey data with alternative data sources, such as paradata, administrative data, and contextual data. Paradata, for instance, refers to data related to the process of collecting survey data during the field period, which are not directly derived from respondents’ answers to survey questions, but rather are a byproduct of the data collection process (Couper 1998; Kreuter 2013). This may include data from call records, keystroke data in computer-administered surveys, interviewer observations, and more. Administrative data refers to externally created process data that are often linked to individual respondent records by matching personal information (Calderwood and Lessof 2009). For instance, social surveys may link respondents’ interview data (conditional on consent) to tax, insurance, voter registration, and other government databases. Finally, contextual data comprises external sources of information that measure various aspects of the respondent’s physical, social, or informational environment (Fortin-Rittberger et al. 2016). This could involve aggregate data on the demographic composition of a respondent’s neighborhood or organizational characteristics of their place of work, as well as data on respondents’ behaviors, environments, and social networks from wearables, sensors, apps, and digital traces from social media or web browsing. Survey researchers are increasingly utilizing these data sources to enhance their substantive and methodological research and address complex research questions that are difficult (or impossible) to answer using survey data alone. Paradata, for instance, are employed in survey production to monitor fieldwork, increase data collection efficiencies, investigate measurement errors, and assess and correct for nonresponse errors (Biemer et al. 2013; Wagner et al. 2012; Yan and Olson 2013). Likewise, linked administrative data are
{"title":"Augmenting Surveys with Paradata, Administrative Data, and Contextual Data.","authors":"Joseph W Sakshaug,&nbsp;Bella Struminskaya","doi":"10.1093/poq/nfad026","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfad026","url":null,"abstract":"Over the last decades, there has been growing interest in augmenting survey data with alternative data sources, such as paradata, administrative data, and contextual data. Paradata, for instance, refers to data related to the process of collecting survey data during the field period, which are not directly derived from respondents’ answers to survey questions, but rather are a byproduct of the data collection process (Couper 1998; Kreuter 2013). This may include data from call records, keystroke data in computer-administered surveys, interviewer observations, and more. Administrative data refers to externally created process data that are often linked to individual respondent records by matching personal information (Calderwood and Lessof 2009). For instance, social surveys may link respondents’ interview data (conditional on consent) to tax, insurance, voter registration, and other government databases. Finally, contextual data comprises external sources of information that measure various aspects of the respondent’s physical, social, or informational environment (Fortin-Rittberger et al. 2016). This could involve aggregate data on the demographic composition of a respondent’s neighborhood or organizational characteristics of their place of work, as well as data on respondents’ behaviors, environments, and social networks from wearables, sensors, apps, and digital traces from social media or web browsing. Survey researchers are increasingly utilizing these data sources to enhance their substantive and methodological research and address complex research questions that are difficult (or impossible) to answer using survey data alone. Paradata, for instance, are employed in survey production to monitor fieldwork, increase data collection efficiencies, investigate measurement errors, and assess and correct for nonresponse errors (Biemer et al. 2013; Wagner et al. 2012; Yan and Olson 2013). Likewise, linked administrative data are","PeriodicalId":51359,"journal":{"name":"Public Opinion Quarterly","volume":"87 Suppl 1","pages":"475-479"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/6b/b5/nfad026.PMC10496571.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10252676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Public Opinion Quarterly
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1