首页 > 最新文献

Rasprave最新文献

英文 中文
Argumentative Values of Causality in French Constructions Like s’il échoue, c’est qu’il n’apprend pas ses leçons 法语结构中因果关系的议论性价值如果它失败了,就意味着它没有吸取教训
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.18
Bauvarie Mounga
Our paper focuses on the expression of causality, in particular its argumentative values in French constructions like s’il échoue, c’est qu’il n’apprend pas ses leçons. It should be noted that this type of constructions if P, Q, is observable when if P is factual, i.e. when the proposition P does not necessarily express a hypothesis as is generally the case, but an event that has happened or is going to happen. Thus, we find concessional, adversarial, additive and emphatic if P, Q systems. However, in the context of our paper, we will only devote ourselves to causative if-constructions. Our work will be structured in three parts. First, we will highlight the functioning of causality in factual if-constructions, then the enunciative postures generated by causality and finally the self-presentation of the speaker in French causative non hypothetical constructions.
我们的论文关注的是因果关系的表达,特别是它在法语结构中的论证价值,如s 'il cheoue, c 'est qu 'il n ' appred和pas ses leons。需要注意的是,如果P是事实命题,也就是说,当命题P不一定表达一个假设,而是一个已经发生或将要发生的事件时,这种类型的命题如果P, Q,是可以观察到的。因此,我们发现了P、Q系统的让步性、对抗性、加性和强性。然而,在本文的语境中,我们将只专注于使役if结构。我们的工作将分为三个部分。首先,我们将强调因果关系在事实if结构中的作用,然后是因果关系产生的发音姿势,最后是法语使役非假设结构中说话人的自我呈现。
{"title":"Argumentative Values of Causality in French Constructions Like s’il échoue, c’est qu’il n’apprend pas ses leçons","authors":"Bauvarie Mounga","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.18","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.18","url":null,"abstract":"Our paper focuses on the expression of causality, in particular its argumentative values in French constructions like s’il échoue, c’est qu’il n’apprend pas ses leçons. It should be noted that this type of constructions if P, Q, is observable when if P is factual, i.e. when the proposition P does not necessarily express a hypothesis as is generally the case, but an event that has happened or is going to happen. Thus, we find concessional, adversarial, additive and emphatic if P, Q systems. However, in the context of our paper, we will only devote ourselves to causative if-constructions. Our work will be structured in three parts. First, we will highlight the functioning of causality in factual if-constructions, then the enunciative postures generated by causality and finally the self-presentation of the speaker in French causative non hypothetical constructions.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"135 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84024980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
C’est que et l’expression de la cause 这是原因的表达
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.11
Clara Destais, Christine Evain, Marie-Françoise Bourvon
L’analyse de la forme c’est que dans un corpus littéraire de 22 romans écrits en français par des auteurs hispanophones, montre qu’elle exprime la causalité. La forme c’est que est employée comme connecteur dans la construction si...c’est que. Mais c’est que est aussi employé seul en tant que marqueur discursif pour indiquer une relation de cause entre ce qui a été énoncé précédemment et ce qui suit.
对形式的分析是,在由讲西班牙语的作家用法语写的22部小说的文学语料库中,它表达了因果关系。它的形式是在建筑中用作连接器,如果…它是。但它也被单独用作话语标记,以表明前面所说的和后面所说的之间的因果关系。
{"title":"C’est que et l’expression de la cause","authors":"Clara Destais, Christine Evain, Marie-Françoise Bourvon","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.11","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.11","url":null,"abstract":"L’analyse de la forme c’est que dans un corpus littéraire de 22 romans écrits en français par des auteurs hispanophones, montre qu’elle exprime la causalité. La forme c’est que est employée comme connecteur dans la construction si...c’est que. Mais c’est que est aussi employé seul en tant que marqueur discursif pour indiquer une relation de cause entre ce qui a été énoncé précédemment et ce qui suit.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83365902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Distribution and Function of Virtual Reflexive Constructions in English 英语虚拟反身结构的分布与功能
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.19
Masaki Yasuhara
This paper sheds new light on the distributional and functional properties of virtual reflexive constructions (VRCs) in English (e.g., This problem solves ITSELF). VRCs describe the level of ease involved in performing the action denoted by the verb. They typically involve metaphorical interpretations, are compatible with adverbs such as virtually, and obtain a primary stress on the reflexive pronoun object. Two claims are presented in this paper: first, the subject of VRCs can be regarded as a crucial contributing factor (i.e., an essential factor in a causal relation). This analysis explains the metaphorical interpretations of VRCs, their compatibility with adverbs such as virtually, and the primary emphasis placed on the reflexive pronoun object. Second, VRCs typically act as anticausativizing causative verbs with no anticausative alternants via metaphor. Namely, VRCs express metaphorical anticausative situations. Causative verbs that can alternate with intransitive anticausative verbs (e.g., break, open, melt) tend to prefer the intransitive anticausative usage when describing anticausative situations (e.g., the vase broke) because intransitive anticausatives are simpler in form and less marked than VRCs. On the other hand, causative verbs with no anticausative alternant (e.g., write, sell) are more likely to occur in VRCs when describing anticausative situations. In other words, VRCs enable these causative verbs to express metaphorical anticausative situations. This analysis is supported by a corpus-based investigation.
本文对英语中虚拟反身结构(VRCs)的分布和功能特性(如:这个问题自己解决)有了新的认识。VRCs描述了执行动词所表示的动作所涉及的轻松程度。它们通常涉及隐喻解释,与副词(如virtual)兼容,并在反身代词宾语上获得主要重音。本文提出了两个主张:首先,VRCs的主体可以被视为一个关键的促成因素(即因果关系中的一个基本因素)。这一分析解释了虚拟现实动词的隐喻解释,它们与副词的兼容性,如虚拟现实副词,以及主要强调反身代词宾语。其次,虚拟实谓语动词通常通过隐喻充当反谓语动词,没有反谓语动词的替代词。也就是说,虚拟现实语言表达隐喻性的反修辞情境。可以与不及物反谓语动词交替使用的使役动词(如break, open, melt)在描述反谓语情景(如花瓶碎了)时倾向于使用不及物反谓语,因为不及物反谓语的形式比vrc更简单,标记更少。另一方面,没有反动词替代的使役动词(例如,write, sell)更有可能出现在VRCs中,用于描述反动词情况。换句话说,VRCs使这些使役动词能够表达隐喻性的反谓语情景。这一分析得到了基于语料库的调查的支持。
{"title":"The Distribution and Function of Virtual Reflexive Constructions in English","authors":"Masaki Yasuhara","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.19","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.19","url":null,"abstract":"This paper sheds new light on the distributional and functional properties of virtual reflexive constructions (VRCs) in English (e.g., This problem solves ITSELF). VRCs describe the level of ease involved in performing the action denoted by the verb. They typically involve metaphorical interpretations, are compatible with adverbs such as virtually, and obtain a primary stress on the reflexive pronoun object. Two claims are presented in this paper: first, the subject of VRCs can be regarded as a crucial contributing factor (i.e., an essential factor in a causal relation). This analysis explains the metaphorical interpretations of VRCs, their compatibility with adverbs such as virtually, and the primary emphasis placed on the reflexive pronoun object. Second, VRCs typically act as anticausativizing causative verbs with no anticausative alternants via metaphor. Namely, VRCs express metaphorical anticausative situations. Causative verbs that can alternate with intransitive anticausative verbs (e.g., break, open, melt) tend to prefer the intransitive anticausative usage when describing anticausative situations (e.g., the vase broke) because intransitive anticausatives are simpler in form and less marked than VRCs. On the other hand, causative verbs with no anticausative alternant (e.g., write, sell) are more likely to occur in VRCs when describing anticausative situations. In other words, VRCs enable these causative verbs to express metaphorical anticausative situations. This analysis is supported by a corpus-based investigation.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"115 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86035523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expressing Causality in Croatian L1 and L2 Argumentative Writing 克罗地亚语第一、二语议论文写作中的因果关系表达
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.10
Lidija Cvikić, Ivana Trtanj
An argumentative text should present evidence that prove the author’s claims, which implies logical organization of the text and expression of a cause-and-effect relationship. This paper explores the way of expressing causality in argumentative writing in Croatian as a mother tongue (L1), and a foreign language (L2). The study investigates Croatian L1 and L2 speakers’ use of causative connectives in various types of syntactic structures, based on the corpus of 110 argumentative essays (Croatian L1 N=55, Croatian L2 N=55). The results show that in order to express causality, L1 and L2 speakers use the same connectives, in the same syntactic types, but not at the same frequency. Croatian L1 speakers showed strong preference for using pa as a typical causal connective for coordinated and jer for subordinate compound sentences. In comparison to Croatian L1 speakers, L2 speakers showed two tendencies: to overuse some connectives (jer, zato što), and to underuse the others (pa).
议论文应该提供证据来证明作者的观点,这意味着文本的逻辑组织和因果关系的表达。本文探讨了克罗地亚语作为母语(L1)和外语(L2)的议论文写作中因果关系的表达方式。该研究基于110篇议论文(克罗地亚语L1 N=55,克罗地亚语L2 N=55)的语料库,调查了克罗地亚语L1和L2说话者在各种句法结构中使用使役连接词的情况。结果表明,为了表达因果关系,第一语言和第二语言说话者在相同的句法类型中使用相同的连接词,但频率不同。母语为克罗地亚语的人在协调句中用pa作为典型因果连词,在从属复合句中用jer作为典型因果连词。与母语为克罗地亚语的人相比,第二语言的人表现出两种倾向:过度使用某些连接词(jer, zato što),而其他连接词使用不足(pa)。
{"title":"Expressing Causality in Croatian L1 and L2 Argumentative Writing","authors":"Lidija Cvikić, Ivana Trtanj","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.10","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.10","url":null,"abstract":"An argumentative text should present evidence that prove the author’s claims, which implies logical organization of the text and expression of a cause-and-effect relationship. This paper explores the way of expressing causality in argumentative writing in Croatian as a mother tongue (L1), and a foreign language (L2). The study investigates Croatian L1 and L2 speakers’ use of causative connectives in various types of syntactic structures, based on the corpus of 110 argumentative essays (Croatian L1 N=55, Croatian L2 N=55). The results show that in order to express causality, L1 and L2 speakers use the same connectives, in the same syntactic types, but not at the same frequency. Croatian L1 speakers showed strong preference for using pa as a typical causal connective for coordinated and jer for subordinate compound sentences. In comparison to Croatian L1 speakers, L2 speakers showed two tendencies: to overuse some connectives (jer, zato što), and to underuse the others (pa).","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"77 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77755450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Évolution de la causalité chez des enfants francophones 法语儿童因果关系的演变
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.17
C. Martinot
A partir de la restitution individuelle par 15 enfants de 6 et 8 ans d’une histoire qui vient de leur être lue, l’auteur montre une nette évolution entre 6 et 8 ans de la compréhension et de la production des 13 relations causales (RC) contenues dans le TS (texte source). Les 13 RC du TS sont identifiées par cinq marqueurs : lien sémantique (RC non marquée), marque morphologique (opposition présent/futur), marque syntaxique non spécifique au marquage causal (par, qui, à l’idée de …), marque lexicale (verbe devenir), marque syntaxique explicite (parce que). Les RC produites par les enfants sont systématiquement comparées aux RC du TS. Un premier résultat quantitatif montre que sur l’ensemble de toutes les RC pouvant être produites, les enfants de 6 ans en comprennent un quart tandis que les enfants de 8 ans en comprennent plus du tiers. Au niveau de l’ensemble des RC produites, les enfants de 6 ans expriment d’une façon ou d’une autre un peu plus du tiers des RC du TS et les 8 ans un peu plus de la moitié. Les 8 ans sont plus nombreux que les 6 ans à avoir modifié le sens de certaines RC. Ils expriment également la RC du TS avec un autre marqueur beaucoup plus souvent que les 6 ans. La plupart des marqueurs syntaxiques non spécifiques posent encore de nombreux problèmes de production aux 8 ans.
通过15名6至8岁的儿童对刚刚读到的故事的个人再现,作者显示了在6至8年的时间里,对TS(源文本)中包含的13种因果关系(cr)的理解和产生的明显演变。TS中的13个cr由5个标记来识别:语义联系(未标记的cr)、形态标记(现在/未来的对立)、非特定于因果标记的句法标记(par, qui, al '想法…)、词汇标记(动词成为)、显性句法标记(因为)。RC制作的儿童得到系统相比,RC / TS。第一个定量的结果表明,在整个所有RC可生产,6岁的儿童,包括四分之一,而8岁的儿童,包括超过1 / 3。就所有产生的cr而言,6岁的儿童以某种方式表达了TS cr的三分之一多一点,8岁的儿童表达了一半多一点。8岁的孩子比6岁的孩子更有可能改变一些cr的含义。他们也比6岁时更频繁地用另一个标记来表达TS的RC。大多数非特异性的句法标记在8岁时仍然存在许多生产问题。
{"title":"Évolution de la causalité chez des enfants francophones","authors":"C. Martinot","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.17","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.17","url":null,"abstract":"A partir de la restitution individuelle par 15 enfants de 6 et 8 ans d’une histoire qui vient de leur être lue, l’auteur montre une nette évolution entre 6 et 8 ans de la compréhension et de la production des 13 relations causales (RC) contenues dans le TS (texte source). Les 13 RC du TS sont identifiées par cinq marqueurs : lien sémantique (RC non marquée), marque morphologique (opposition présent/futur), marque syntaxique non spécifique au marquage causal (par, qui, à l’idée de …), marque lexicale (verbe devenir), marque syntaxique explicite (parce que). Les RC produites par les enfants sont systématiquement comparées aux RC du TS. Un premier résultat quantitatif montre que sur l’ensemble de toutes les RC pouvant être produites, les enfants de 6 ans en comprennent un quart tandis que les enfants de 8 ans en comprennent plus du tiers. Au niveau de l’ensemble des RC produites, les enfants de 6 ans expriment d’une façon ou d’une autre un peu plus du tiers des RC du TS et les 8 ans un peu plus de la moitié. Les 8 ans sont plus nombreux que les 6 ans à avoir modifié le sens de certaines RC. Ils expriment également la RC du TS avec un autre marqueur beaucoup plus souvent que les 6 ans. La plupart des marqueurs syntaxiques non spécifiques posent encore de nombreux problèmes de production aux 8 ans.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"111 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86801258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hrvatski kao strani jezik na području poslovanja i ekonomije u Sloveniji
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.3
Igor Ivašković
Istraživanje percepcije znanja i korisnosti hrvatskoga jezika među slovenskim studentima na području ekonomije i poslovanja provedeno je s ciljem dobivanja relevantnih informacija na temelju kojih bi se mogla donijeti odluka o uvođenju izbornog predmeta Hrvatski jezik za ekonomiju i poslovanje u slovenski sveučilišni sustav. Rezultati pokazuju da hrvatski jezik kod proučavane populacije u kontekstu 21 stranog jezika zauzima drugo mjesto prema prosječnoj razini znanja i treće mjesto prema percepciji korisnosti ovladavanja jezikom. Kod proučavane studentske populacije ujedno postoji i razmjerno velik interes za učenje hrvatskog jezika, koji je kod te mlađe slovenske populacije jasno identificiran kao zaseban jezik.
{"title":"Hrvatski kao strani jezik na području poslovanja i ekonomije u Sloveniji","authors":"Igor Ivašković","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.3","url":null,"abstract":"Istraživanje percepcije znanja i korisnosti hrvatskoga jezika među slovenskim studentima na području ekonomije i poslovanja provedeno je s ciljem dobivanja relevantnih informacija na temelju kojih bi se mogla donijeti odluka o uvođenju izbornog predmeta Hrvatski jezik za ekonomiju i poslovanje u slovenski sveučilišni sustav. Rezultati pokazuju da hrvatski jezik kod proučavane populacije u kontekstu 21 stranog jezika zauzima drugo mjesto prema prosječnoj razini znanja i treće mjesto prema percepciji korisnosti ovladavanja jezikom. Kod proučavane studentske populacije ujedno postoji i razmjerno velik interes za učenje hrvatskog jezika, koji je kod te mlađe slovenske populacije jasno identificiran kao zaseban jezik.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"32 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84214770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Les constructions participiales détachées pour exprimer la causalité 分离分词结构来表达因果关系
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.12
O. Gagnon, Ann F. Laberge
L’article interroge le rôle des constructions détachées participiales (CDpart) dans l’expression de la causalité. Dans une étude s’étant intéressée au rôle textuel des CDpart en français, il a été observé que les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence textuelle à trois niveaux d’arrimage définis par Gagnon (2008, 2015) : 1) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan référentiel puisque, en tant que proposition réduite, elles possèdent un référent sous-jacent pouvant être relié à un syntagme nominal (SN) présent dans le contexte et jouent ainsi un rôle dans la continuité thématique; 2) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan informatif puisque, en tant que prédications secondes, elles véhiculent une information nouvelle de second plan et opèrent une hiérarchisation des informations; 3) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan évènementiel puisqu’elles permettent d’établir une relation entre l’événement décrit au PP et celui décrit dans la principale. L’analyse des CDpart sur le plan évènementiel a fait ressortir que dans un grand nombre de cas, la CDpart se trouve en position frontale et que dans ces cas, la relation le plus souvent établie entre l’évènement décrit au PP et celui décrit dans la principale est une relation de cause (Combettes 2005), comme dans l’exemple suivant tiré du corpus (Laberge 2019) : Favorisant le repli sur soi, il (le téléphone cellulaire) rompt le dialogue en famille ou entre amis lors d’un repas (...). L’article présente l’analyse qui a été faite, du point de vue évènementiel, de quelques occurrences de CDpart relevées dans des textes rédigés par des étudiants universitaires de premier cycle ou dans des textes journalistiques. Les résultats de cette analyse confirment que les CDpart doivent être envisagées comme ressources linguistiques pouvant exprimer la causalité en français.
本文探讨分词分离结构(CDpart)在因果关系表达中的作用。随着研究的兴趣在CDpart法文文本的作用,有人评论说,三级捆绑CDpart文本的一致性有助于界定由Gagnon(2008、2015):(1)CDpart一致性有助于工具包上。因为,作为提案的不便,她们拥有一个协调人标的,可链接到一个介词短语(SN)范围内,本专题,从而起到了一定的作用,在连续性;2) CDpart有助于信息的一致性,因为作为次要信息,它们传递新的背景信息,并对信息进行排序;3) CDpart有助于事件的一致性,因为它们允许在PP中描述的事件和主要事件之间建立关系。分析CDpartévènementiel上发现,在大量的案例、CDpart位于前端的位置,对于这些情况,本次活动之间建立的关系通常PP述及与主要是一种因果关系的描述(Combettes 2005)以下示例中一样,从语料库(Laberge 2019):为促进闭关自守,(手机),打破了在家人或朋友之间的对话在一餐(...)。本文从事件的角度,对本科生撰写的文章或新闻文章中发现的一些此类事件进行了分析。这一分析的结果证实,应将CDpart视为能够用法语表达因果关系的语言资源。
{"title":"Les constructions participiales détachées pour exprimer la causalité","authors":"O. Gagnon, Ann F. Laberge","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.12","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.12","url":null,"abstract":"L’article interroge le rôle des constructions détachées participiales (CDpart) dans l’expression de la causalité. Dans une étude s’étant intéressée au rôle textuel des CDpart en français, il a été observé que les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence textuelle à trois niveaux d’arrimage définis par Gagnon (2008, 2015) : 1) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan référentiel puisque, en tant que proposition réduite, elles possèdent un référent sous-jacent pouvant être relié à un syntagme nominal (SN) présent dans le contexte et jouent ainsi un rôle dans la continuité thématique; 2) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan informatif puisque, en tant que prédications secondes, elles véhiculent une information nouvelle de second plan et opèrent une hiérarchisation des informations; 3) les CDpart contribuent à la cohérence sur le plan évènementiel puisqu’elles permettent d’établir une relation entre l’événement décrit au PP et celui décrit dans la principale. L’analyse des CDpart sur le plan évènementiel a fait ressortir que dans un grand nombre de cas, la CDpart se trouve en position frontale et que dans ces cas, la relation le plus souvent établie entre l’évènement décrit au PP et celui décrit dans la principale est une relation de cause (Combettes 2005), comme dans l’exemple suivant tiré du corpus (Laberge 2019) : Favorisant le repli sur soi, il (le téléphone cellulaire) rompt le dialogue en famille ou entre amis lors d’un repas (...). L’article présente l’analyse qui a été faite, du point de vue évènementiel, de quelques occurrences de CDpart relevées dans des textes rédigés par des étudiants universitaires de premier cycle ou dans des textes journalistiques. Les résultats de cette analyse confirment que les CDpart doivent être envisagées comme ressources linguistiques pouvant exprimer la causalité en français.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"23 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75617337","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
La présence de l’énonciateur des assertions causales dans des dissertations écrites par les étudiants de FLE 在FLE学生撰写的论文中,因果命题的陈述者的存在
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.16
Anna Kucharska
L’article vise à analyser la causalité en tant que moyen argumentatif dans les dissertations écrites par les apprenants de FLE. La dissertation est une tâche souvent utilisée pendant les cours de langue étrangère pour enseigner et vérifier la capacité des apprenants à mener à bien le discours argumentatif. Nous présumons que le locuteur qui écrit une dissertation, considérée ici comme un énoncé argumentatif, crée un acte de parole avec le but de persuader ses interlocuteurs. Pour atteindre cet objectif plus facilement, il est censé nuancer les relations causales en les modifiant soit par la citation de données objectives, scientifiques, soit au moyen d’une vision particulière d’une autorité ou du groupe auquel appartient l’interlocuteur. L’analyse des compositions permettra d’observer l’attitude des locuteurs par rapport à la tâche de production écrite dans le cadre de leur formation linguistique.
本文的目的是分析因果关系作为一种论证手段在外语学习者的论文。论文是外语课程中经常使用的一项任务,用来教授和测试学习者进行议论性演讲的能力。我们假设,写论文的人,在这里被认为是一个论证陈述,创造了一个言语行为,目的是说服他的对话者。为了更容易实现这一目标,因果关系应该通过引用客观的科学数据或通过对话者所属的权威或群体的特定观点来修改它们。通过对作文的分析,我们可以观察说话者在语言训练中对写作任务的态度。
{"title":"La présence de l’énonciateur des assertions causales dans des dissertations écrites par les étudiants de FLE","authors":"Anna Kucharska","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.16","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.16","url":null,"abstract":"L’article vise à analyser la causalité en tant que moyen argumentatif dans les dissertations écrites par les apprenants de FLE. La dissertation est une tâche souvent utilisée pendant les cours de langue étrangère pour enseigner et vérifier la capacité des apprenants à mener à bien le discours argumentatif. Nous présumons que le locuteur qui écrit une dissertation, considérée ici comme un énoncé argumentatif, crée un acte de parole avec le but de persuader ses interlocuteurs. Pour atteindre cet objectif plus facilement, il est censé nuancer les relations causales en les modifiant soit par la citation de données objectives, scientifiques, soit au moyen d’une vision particulière d’une autorité ou du groupe auquel appartient l’interlocuteur. L’analyse des compositions permettra d’observer l’attitude des locuteurs par rapport à la tâche de production écrite dans le cadre de leur formation linguistique.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"376 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80577396","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Some Notes on Experiencer Causatives and DOM in Romanian 罗马尼亚语中经验使役句和DOM的几点注意
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.14
Julie Goncharov, M. Irimia
This paper focuses on object experiencer (OE) causatives in Romanian, identifying a less discussed pattern of variation. The results of a pilot study indicate that for a class of speakers such predicates are not grammatical with an indefinite object, if the latter is not differentially marked. A second class of speakers can accept unmarked objects but only if access to direct evidence of the event is available. As these restrictions set aside OE causatives from physical causatives, a non-trivial question refers to the nature of this difference. An analysis is proposed that revolves around a pragmatic distinction between OE verbs and physical causatives. More precisely, insights put forward by pragmatic investigations of OE verbs have consolidated the observation, which we follow here, that these types of predicates presuppose a perception event in which the object of the asserted event is a perceiver. We further propose that the perception presupposition can be established in the context either by differential object marking (DOM), which has an independently motivated sentience feature, or by direct evidence. Subsequently, we also show that an analysis along these lines gives better results when addressing these types of splits against more general interactions between causatives, inanimate subjects and DOM; under previous accounts, the facts under discussion are either unpredicted or not straightforward to derive.
本文着重于罗马尼亚语中的客体体验(OE)使役,确定了一种较少讨论的变化模式。一项初步研究的结果表明,对于一类说话者来说,如果不确定对象没有区别标记,这样的谓词与不确定对象是不符合语法的。第二类说话者可以接受没有标记的物体,但只有在可以获得事件的直接证据的情况下。由于这些限制将原声使役与物理使役分开,一个重要的问题涉及到这种区别的本质。本文围绕原声动词和物理使役的语用区别进行分析。更准确地说,对英语动词的语用研究所提出的见解巩固了我们在这里遵循的观察结果,即这些类型的谓词预设了一个感知事件,其中断言事件的对象是感知者。我们进一步提出,可以通过具有独立动机的感知特征的差分对象标记(DOM)或直接证据在上下文中建立感知预设。随后,我们还表明,在处理这些类型的分裂时,沿着这些思路进行分析可以获得更好的结果,而不是使词、无生命主体和DOM之间更一般的相互作用;根据以前的说法,讨论中的事实要么是不可预测的,要么是无法直接推导出来的。
{"title":"Some Notes on Experiencer Causatives and DOM in Romanian","authors":"Julie Goncharov, M. Irimia","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.14","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.14","url":null,"abstract":"This paper focuses on object experiencer (OE) causatives in Romanian, identifying a less discussed pattern of variation. The results of a pilot study indicate that for a class of speakers such predicates are not grammatical with an indefinite object, if the latter is not differentially marked. A second class of speakers can accept unmarked objects but only if access to direct evidence of the event is available. As these restrictions set aside OE causatives from physical causatives, a non-trivial question refers to the nature of this difference. An analysis is proposed that revolves around a pragmatic distinction between OE verbs and physical causatives. More precisely, insights put forward by pragmatic investigations of OE verbs have consolidated the observation, which we follow here, that these types of predicates presuppose a perception event in which the object of the asserted event is a perceiver. We further propose that the perception presupposition can be established in the context either by differential object marking (DOM), which has an independently motivated sentience feature, or by direct evidence. Subsequently, we also show that an analysis along these lines gives better results when addressing these types of splits against more general interactions between causatives, inanimate subjects and DOM; under previous accounts, the facts under discussion are either unpredicted or not straightforward to derive.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"117 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79971716","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Causality in Children’s Oral and Written Narrative Retelling 儿童口头和书面叙述复述的因果关系
IF 0.2 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.31724/rihjj.48.1.9
Tomislava Bošnjak Botica, Jelena Kuvač Kraljević
One of the main prerequisites for understanding and producing coherent oral discourse or written text is successful understanding and production of causal relations. During both production, children have at their disposal a wide range of linguistic modes to mark it, some of which are more explicit and others more implicit. In this study, retelling was used as a method to elicit narratives that served as a tool for analysing causal relations. Retelling enables exploring the linguistic reformulation of the syntactic structures of a previously stored story and the analysis of the overlap between the language content which child is exposed to (language input) and the language that child produces (language output). Two groups of children, aged 10 (N = 23) and 12 (N = 30), were exposed to the story at two time points; in the first they had to retell it orally and in the second they had to write it. The conducted analyses showed that 12-years old children produced in total more causal relations than 10-year-old in written modality only. This difference is explained by the greater writing competence of 12-year-old children in the production of more complex syntactic structures. Furthermore, both groups of children in both modalities dominantly used the same causal markers that are primarily grammatical. All these findings point to the children’s ability to reformulate causal relations regardless of the language content to which they were previously exposed.
理解和产生连贯的口头话语或书面文本的主要先决条件之一是成功地理解和产生因果关系。在这两种生产过程中,儿童都有广泛的语言模式来标记它,其中一些更明确,另一些更含蓄。在这项研究中,复述被用作一种方法来引出叙述,作为分析因果关系的工具。复述可以探索先前存储的故事的句法结构的语言重新表述,并分析儿童接触的语言内容(语言输入)和儿童产生的语言(语言输出)之间的重叠。两组10岁(N = 23)和12岁(N = 30)的儿童分别在两个时间点听故事;在第一组中,他们必须口头复述,在第二组中,他们必须写下来。所进行的分析表明,12岁儿童比10岁儿童仅在书面形式中产生更多的因果关系。这种差异可以解释为12岁儿童在产生更复杂的句法结构方面具有更强的写作能力。此外,两组儿童在两种模式下都主要使用相同的主要是语法的因果标记。所有这些发现都表明,无论孩子们之前接触的语言内容是什么,他们都有能力重新构建因果关系。
{"title":"Causality in Children’s Oral and Written Narrative Retelling","authors":"Tomislava Bošnjak Botica, Jelena Kuvač Kraljević","doi":"10.31724/rihjj.48.1.9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.48.1.9","url":null,"abstract":"One of the main prerequisites for understanding and producing coherent oral discourse or written text is successful understanding and production of causal relations. During both production, children have at their disposal a wide range of linguistic modes to mark it, some of which are more explicit and others more implicit. In this study, retelling was used as a method to elicit narratives that served as a tool for analysing causal relations. Retelling enables exploring the linguistic reformulation of the syntactic structures of a previously stored story and the analysis of the overlap between the language content which child is exposed to (language input) and the language that child produces (language output). Two groups of children, aged 10 (N = 23) and 12 (N = 30), were exposed to the story at two time points; in the first they had to retell it orally and in the second they had to write it. The conducted analyses showed that 12-years old children produced in total more causal relations than 10-year-old in written modality only. This difference is explained by the greater writing competence of 12-year-old children in the production of more complex syntactic structures. Furthermore, both groups of children in both modalities dominantly used the same causal markers that are primarily grammatical. All these findings point to the children’s ability to reformulate causal relations regardless of the language content to which they were previously exposed.","PeriodicalId":51986,"journal":{"name":"Rasprave","volume":"78 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74009013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Rasprave
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1