首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
The controversial origins of war and peace: apes, foragers, and human evolution 战争与和平的争议性起源:猿、觅食者与人类进化
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106618
Luke Glowacki

The role of warfare in human evolution is among the most contentious topics in the evolutionary sciences. The debate is especially heated because many assume that whether our evolutionary ancestors were peaceful or warlike has important implications for modern human nature. One side argues that warfare has a deep evolutionary history, possible dating to the last common ancestor of bonobos, chimpanzees, and humans, while the other views war as a recent innovation, primarily developing with the rise of sedentism and agriculture. I show that although both positions have some support warranting consideration, each sometimes ignores uncertainties about human evolution and simplifies the complex reality of hunter-gatherer worlds. Many characterizations about the evolution of war are partial truths. Bonobos and chimpanzees provide important insights relevant for understanding the origins of war, but using either species as a model for human evolution has important limitations. Hunter-gatherers often had war, but like humans everywhere, our ancestors likely had a range of relationships depending on the context, including cooperative intergroup affiliation. Taken together, the evidence strongly suggests that small-scale warfare is part of our evolutionary history predating agriculture and sedentism, but that cooperation across group boundaries is also part our evolutionary legacy.

战争在人类进化中的作用是进化科学中最具争议的话题之一。这场争论尤为激烈,因为许多人认为,我们的进化祖先是和平的还是好战的,对现代人性有着重要影响。一方认为战争有着深厚的进化历史,可能可以追溯到倭黑猩猩、黑猩猩和人类的最后一个共同祖先,而另一方则认为战争是近代的创新,主要是随着定居主义和农业的兴起而发展起来的。我的研究表明,尽管这两种观点都有一些值得考虑的支持,但它们有时都忽略了人类进化的不确定性,简化了狩猎-采集世界的复杂现实。许多关于战争进化的描述都是片面的。倭黑猩猩和黑猩猩为了解战争的起源提供了重要的启示,但将这两种动物作为人类进化的模型有很大的局限性。狩猎采集者经常会发生战争,但就像世界各地的人类一样,我们的祖先很可能会根据不同的环境建立一系列的关系,包括群体间的合作关系。综合来看,这些证据有力地表明,小规模战争是人类在农业和定居之前进化历史的一部分,但跨越群体边界的合作也是人类进化遗产的一部分。
{"title":"The controversial origins of war and peace: apes, foragers, and human evolution","authors":"Luke Glowacki","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106618","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106618","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The role of warfare in human evolution is among the most contentious topics in the evolutionary sciences. The debate is especially heated because many assume that whether our evolutionary ancestors were peaceful or warlike has important implications for modern human nature. One side argues that warfare has a deep evolutionary history, possible dating to the last common ancestor of bonobos, chimpanzees, and humans, while the other views war as a recent innovation, primarily developing with the rise of sedentism and agriculture. I show that although both positions have some support warranting consideration, each sometimes ignores uncertainties about human evolution and simplifies the complex reality of hunter-gatherer worlds. Many characterizations about the evolution of war are partial truths. Bonobos and chimpanzees provide important insights relevant for understanding the origins of war, but using either species as a model for human evolution has important limitations. Hunter-gatherers often had war, but like humans everywhere, our ancestors likely had a range of relationships depending on the context, including cooperative intergroup affiliation. Taken together, the evidence strongly suggests that small-scale warfare is part of our evolutionary history predating agriculture and sedentism, but that cooperation across group boundaries is also part our evolutionary legacy.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106618"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142149492","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Core principles of melodic organisation emerge from transmission chains with random melodies 从随机旋律的传输链中得出旋律组织的核心原则
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106619
Tudor Popescu , Martin Rohrmeier

Music is a product of both biological and cultural evolution. Cultural transmission is the engine of cultural evolution and may play a role in the establishment of musical universals. Here, we examined how transmission dynamics can shape melodic features in music. Specifically, we tested whether random melodic seeds, in their transformation, take on properties known to characterise music within or even across cultures. Using an iterated learning paradigm, we investigated the transmission of random melodic seeds through a chain of non-musician participants (N = 64). We found that melodies reproduced vocally between “generations” became more similar to known musical scales, exhibited a predominance of consonant intervals, and reduced the number of scale degrees used. Additionally, we observed the previously documented tendency for large intervals to be followed by a change in direction, as well as features common to both music and speech including phrase-final lengthening and the Zipfian distribution of signalling units. As participants' vocalisations converged towards greater memorability, they exhibited decreased entropy, and their contours became smoother and more consistent. Finally, certain short melodic patterns became prominent motifs within the incipient musical “traditions” simulated by the chains. These emerging features may reflect a process shaped by (i) cognitive bottlenecks such as learnability; (ii) statistical properties of the processes and structures involved in inter-generational vocal transmission; but also by (iii) idiosyncratic cultural artefacts specific to the lab samples employed. Overall, our results demonstrate that fundamental aspects of melodic structure emerge naturally through the process of cultural transmission, as simulated in the lab.

音乐是生物和文化进化的产物。文化传播是文化进化的动力,并可能在音乐普遍性的建立中发挥作用。在此,我们研究了传播动力学如何塑造音乐中的旋律特征。具体来说,我们测试了随机旋律种子在其转化过程中是否具有已知的特性,这些特性是音乐在文化内甚至在不同文化间的特征。通过迭代学习范式,我们研究了随机旋律种子在非音乐家参与者(64 人)中的传播。我们发现,"代际 "之间用声音再现的旋律与已知音阶更加相似,表现出辅音音程的优势,并减少了音阶度数的使用。此外,我们还观察到了之前记录的大音程后方向变化的趋势,以及音乐和语音的共同特征,包括乐句最后的延长和信号单元的齐普菲式分布。随着参与者的发声越来越容易记忆,它们的熵值也在下降,轮廓也变得更加平滑和连贯。最后,某些简短的旋律模式成为链条所模拟的初期音乐 "传统 "中的突出主题。这些新出现的特征可能反映了以下因素所形成的过程:(i) 认知瓶颈,如可学性;(ii) 声乐代际传播过程和结构的统计特性;(iii) 所使用的实验室样本特有的特异文化人工制品。总之,我们的研究结果表明,旋律结构的基本方面是在实验室模拟的文化传播过程中自然形成的。
{"title":"Core principles of melodic organisation emerge from transmission chains with random melodies","authors":"Tudor Popescu ,&nbsp;Martin Rohrmeier","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106619","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106619","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Music is a product of both biological and cultural evolution. Cultural transmission is the engine of cultural evolution and may play a role in the establishment of musical universals. Here, we examined how transmission dynamics can shape melodic features in music. Specifically, we tested whether random melodic seeds, in their transformation, take on properties known to characterise music within or even across cultures. Using an iterated learning paradigm, we investigated the transmission of random melodic seeds through a chain of non-musician participants (<em>N</em> = 64). We found that melodies reproduced vocally between “generations” became more similar to known musical scales, exhibited a predominance of consonant intervals, and reduced the number of scale degrees used. Additionally, we observed the previously documented tendency for large intervals to be followed by a change in direction, as well as features common to both music and speech including phrase-final lengthening and the Zipfian distribution of signalling units. As participants' vocalisations converged towards greater memorability, they exhibited decreased entropy, and their contours became smoother and more consistent. Finally, certain short melodic patterns became prominent motifs within the incipient musical “traditions” simulated by the chains. These emerging features may reflect a process shaped by (i) cognitive bottlenecks such as learnability; (ii) statistical properties of the processes and structures involved in inter-generational vocal transmission; but also by (iii) idiosyncratic cultural artefacts specific to the lab samples employed. Overall, our results demonstrate that fundamental aspects of melodic structure emerge naturally through the process of cultural transmission, as simulated in the lab.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106619"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000953/pdfft?md5=9776a8d21dff0cfe7719d2183bd1def4&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000953-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142128440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hormonal contraceptive use, not menstrual cycle phase, is associated with reduced interest in competition 使用荷尔蒙避孕药(而非月经周期阶段)与竞赛兴趣降低有关
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106616
Lindsie Catherine Arthur , Brock Bastian , Khandis Rose Blake

Despite numerous studies attempting to understand the influence of fertility and hormonal contraceptives on competitive outcomes, the association between these variables remain a contentious issue. Using a longitudinal, online diary study with over 5600 daily observations (N = 302 community and undergraduate student participants), we explored the effects of fertility probability and hormonal contraceptive use on a range of self-report competitive outcomes. Specifically, we sought to replicate existing findings by examining whether self-reported competitive motivation increases mid-cycle and how hormonal contraceptive use affects these patterns. We also sought to extend this literature by collecting a range of self-report measures of competitive behavior across the menstrual cycle. Using an evolutionary and data-driven framework, we combined six behaviors females use to compete for mates and status: gossip, social comparison, negative evaluations of others, appearance enhancement, giving advice and taking sexy self-portraits. Contrary to our predictions, fertility probability was not associated with fluctuations in self-reported competitive motivation or behavior. However, replicating existing literature, hormonal contraceptive users reported a general lack of interest in competition compared to non-users. This latter finding raises important questions about the long-term implications of hormonal contraceptive use and suggests a need for further research to investigate the psychosocial effects of hormonal contraceptives. Finally, as a near direct replication of an existing longitudinal study, the current research offers a unique contribution by considering the role of context in hormone mediated research. We question the idea that a failure to replicate reflects the quality of the theory or the research, but instead suggest that context should be considered when developing psychological theory.

尽管有许多研究试图了解生育率和荷尔蒙避孕药对竞技结果的影响,但这些变量之间的关联仍然是一个有争议的问题。我们利用一项纵向在线日记研究,每天观察超过 5600 次(N = 302 名社区和本科生参与者),探讨了生育概率和激素避孕药的使用对一系列自我报告的竞技结果的影响。具体来说,我们试图通过研究自我报告的竞争动机是否会在周期中期增加,以及激素避孕药的使用如何影响这些模式,来复制现有的研究结果。我们还试图通过收集月经周期中一系列自我报告的竞争行为测量指标来扩展这些文献。利用进化和数据驱动的框架,我们综合了女性用来竞争配偶和地位的六种行为:闲聊、社会比较、对他人的负面评价、提升外表、提供建议和拍摄性感自画像。与我们的预测相反,生育概率与自我报告的竞争动机或行为的波动无关。然而,与现有文献相同的是,与不使用激素避孕药的人相比,使用激素避孕药的人普遍对竞争缺乏兴趣。后一项发现提出了使用激素避孕药的长期影响的重要问题,并表明需要进一步研究激素避孕药对社会心理的影响。最后,作为对现有纵向研究的近乎直接的复制,本研究通过考虑环境在激素媒介研究中的作用做出了独特的贡献。我们对 "复制失败反映了理论或研究的质量 "这一观点提出质疑,并建议在发展心理学理论时应考虑背景因素。
{"title":"Hormonal contraceptive use, not menstrual cycle phase, is associated with reduced interest in competition","authors":"Lindsie Catherine Arthur ,&nbsp;Brock Bastian ,&nbsp;Khandis Rose Blake","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106616","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106616","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite numerous studies attempting to understand the influence of fertility and hormonal contraceptives on competitive outcomes, the association between these variables remain a contentious issue. Using a longitudinal, online diary study with over 5600 daily observations (<em>N</em> = 302 community and undergraduate student participants), we explored the effects of fertility probability and hormonal contraceptive use on a range of self-report competitive outcomes. Specifically, we sought to replicate existing findings by examining whether self-reported competitive motivation increases mid-cycle and how hormonal contraceptive use affects these patterns. We also sought to extend this literature by collecting a range of self-report measures of competitive behavior across the menstrual cycle. Using an evolutionary and data-driven framework, we combined six behaviors females use to compete for mates and status: gossip, social comparison, negative evaluations of others, appearance enhancement, giving advice and taking sexy self-portraits. Contrary to our predictions, fertility probability was not associated with fluctuations in self-reported competitive motivation or behavior. However, replicating existing literature, hormonal contraceptive users reported a general lack of interest in competition compared to non-users. This latter finding raises important questions about the long-term implications of hormonal contraceptive use and suggests a need for further research to investigate the psychosocial effects of hormonal contraceptives. Finally, as a near direct replication of an existing longitudinal study, the current research offers a unique contribution by considering the role of context in hormone mediated research. We question the idea that a failure to replicate reflects the quality of the theory or the research, but instead suggest that context should be considered when developing psychological theory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106616"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000928/pdfft?md5=521dd869b8cbc462b7d9a1732d19f5ff&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000928-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142117680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Generosity as a status signal: Higher-testosterone men exhibit greater altruism in the dictator game 慷慨是一种地位信号:睾丸激素较高的男性在独裁者游戏中表现出更强的利他主义
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106615
Julie Novakova , Petr Tureček , Kamila Machová , Kateřina Sýkorová , Vojtěch Zíka , Jaroslav Flegr

Altruistic behavior can be modulated by many factors including hormonal levels, but their reported effects remain mixed. Understanding the proximate mechanisms of altruism such as these can help test predictions of ultimate, evolutionary explanations. We investigated the relationship of the endogenous salivary levels of testosterone and cortisol with Dictator Game (DG) offers as a proxy of altruism on a sample of general-population participants (N = 158, 84 F, 74 M). Bayesian data analysis and model comparison showed both testosterone and cortisol were negatively correlated with DG offers in women, while higher testosterone levels were associated with greater generosity in men. These results suggest that high testosterone may promote altruistic behavior in the service of status-seeking among men.

利他行为可以受到包括荷尔蒙水平在内的多种因素的调节,但据报道,这些因素的影响仍然参差不齐。了解利他主义的近似机制有助于检验最终进化解释的预测。我们调查了睾酮和皮质醇的内源性唾液水平与独裁者游戏(DG)提议(作为利他主义的替代物)之间的关系。贝叶斯数据分析和模型比较显示,女性的睾酮和皮质醇与 DG 提议呈负相关,而男性的睾酮水平越高,其慷慨程度越高。这些结果表明,高睾酮可能会促进男性为追求地位而做出利他行为。
{"title":"Generosity as a status signal: Higher-testosterone men exhibit greater altruism in the dictator game","authors":"Julie Novakova ,&nbsp;Petr Tureček ,&nbsp;Kamila Machová ,&nbsp;Kateřina Sýkorová ,&nbsp;Vojtěch Zíka ,&nbsp;Jaroslav Flegr","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106615","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106615","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Altruistic behavior can be modulated by many factors including hormonal levels, but their reported effects remain mixed. Understanding the proximate mechanisms of altruism such as these can help test predictions of ultimate, evolutionary explanations. We investigated the relationship of the endogenous salivary levels of testosterone and cortisol with Dictator Game (DG) offers as a proxy of altruism on a sample of general-population participants (<em>N</em> = 158, 84 F, 74 M). Bayesian data analysis and model comparison showed both testosterone and cortisol were negatively correlated with DG offers in women, while higher testosterone levels were associated with greater generosity in men. These results suggest that high testosterone may promote altruistic behavior in the service of status-seeking among men.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106615"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142048809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are there fitness benefits to violence? The case of medieval Iceland 暴力对健康有益吗?中世纪冰岛的案例
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106614
R.I.M. Dunbar , Anna Wallette

Males that behave violently to achieve social and reproductive objectives are a widespread phenomenon among mammals, as well as humans. Because this kind of behaviour can be socially very disruptive, its continued survival in human populations, in particular, remains a puzzle. We use historical data on the pedigrees of medieval Icelandic Vikings to test the hypothesis that males who killed had higher fitness than other males. Whereas most studies that examine the evolutionary benefits of behaviour focus on the numbers of offspring sired, in this paper we measure fitness more directly in terms of the number of grandchildren produced both directly and indirectly via collateral relatives, as well as determining the costs of pursuing alternative strategies in terms of experienced mortality rates. We show that, on average, killers gain a very significant fitness advantage despite the often high costs they pay and, more importantly, that they had a dramatic effect on the fitness of their male kin. We suggest that such behaviour represents a phenotypic, rather than genetic, response to opportunities provided by local circumstances.

雄性动物为了达到社交和繁殖目的而采取暴力行动,这是哺乳动物和人类的普遍现象。由于这种行为会对社会造成极大的破坏,因此它在人类种群中的持续生存仍然是一个谜。我们利用中世纪冰岛维京人的血统历史数据来验证这样一个假设,即杀戮的雄性比其他雄性具有更高的适应能力。研究行为对进化带来的益处时,大多数研究都侧重于所生后代的数量,而在本文中,我们更直接地从直接或通过旁系亲属间接产生的孙子数量来衡量适存度,并从所经历的死亡率来确定采取替代策略的成本。我们的研究表明,平均而言,杀戮者尽管付出了高昂的代价,但却获得了非常显著的适应优势,更重要的是,他们对其雄性亲属的适应能力产生了巨大的影响。我们认为,这种行为是对当地环境提供的机会的一种表型反应,而不是遗传反应。
{"title":"Are there fitness benefits to violence? The case of medieval Iceland","authors":"R.I.M. Dunbar ,&nbsp;Anna Wallette","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106614","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106614","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Males that behave violently to achieve social and reproductive objectives are a widespread phenomenon among mammals, as well as humans. Because this kind of behaviour can be socially very disruptive, its continued survival in human populations, in particular, remains a puzzle. We use historical data on the pedigrees of medieval Icelandic Vikings to test the hypothesis that males who killed had higher fitness than other males. Whereas most studies that examine the evolutionary benefits of behaviour focus on the numbers of offspring sired, in this paper we measure fitness more directly in terms of the number of grandchildren produced both directly and indirectly via collateral relatives, as well as determining the costs of pursuing alternative strategies in terms of experienced mortality rates. We show that, on average, killers gain a very significant fitness advantage despite the often high costs they pay and, more importantly, that they had a dramatic effect on the fitness of their male kin. We suggest that such behaviour represents a phenotypic, rather than genetic, response to opportunities provided by local circumstances.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106614"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000904/pdfft?md5=5853514e10c49b4213a77b751539c61b&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000904-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141978271","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cost structures and socioecological conditions impact the fitness outcomes of human alloparental care in agent-based model simulations 在基于代理的模型模拟中,成本结构和社会生态条件对人类全亲照料的适应性结果的影响
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106613
Elic M. Weitzel , Kurt M. Wilson , Laure Spake , Susan B. Schaffnit , Robert Lynch , Rebecca Sear , John H. Shaver , Mary K. Shenk , Richard Sosis

Alloparental care—care provided to children who are not one's offspring—is ubiquitous across human populations. Empirical research reveals socioecological variation in who cares for children, but less attention has been paid to the type of care provided. To better understand the fitness outcomes of different forms of alloparental care, or allocare, we categorize such care into two broad forms based on economic cost structures: additive cost and declining marginal cost allocare. Additive cost allocare requires alloparents to pay equal costs for each child to whom care is provided, while declining marginal cost allocare entails reduced costs for additional children beyond the first. Given this general typology, we investigate how fitness is impacted by the type of allocare provided in socioecological conditions of scarcity or abundance. Results of an agent-based model indicate that allocare has fitness benefits in nearly all circumstances, but the impact of cost structures depends on resource availability. In contexts of abundance, the cost structure of allocare does not matter as individuals' reproductive success is instead constrained by fertility and mortality more than the availability of resources or time. In conditions of scarcity, however, the greatest increases in reproductive success are achieved when allocare has a declining marginal cost structure. This is due to an economy of scale permitting alloparents to scale up their care at discounted rates. Consequently, we expect allocare practices to exhibit these patterns cross-culturally: in contexts of scarcity allocare is anticipated to be focused on practices with declining marginal cost structures and to be much less variable than in contexts of abundance. We discuss several ethnographic examples that are consistent with the overall findings of our simulations, and we conclude with recommendations for future modeling and empirical work on allocare.

全亲照料--照料非亲生子女--在人类中普遍存在。实证研究揭示了谁照顾孩子的社会生态差异,但对所提供照顾的类型关注较少。为了更好地理解不同形式的全亲照护(或称allocare)对健康的影响,我们根据经济成本结构将这种照护分为两大类:加成成本和边际成本递减的allocare。加法成本分配法要求全职父母为每个接受照顾的孩子支付相等的成本,而递减边际成本分配法则要求在照顾第一个孩子之外的其他孩子时降低成本。鉴于这一一般类型,我们研究了在稀缺或富裕的社会生态条件下,提供的分配抚养类型会如何影响适应性。基于代理的模型结果表明,几乎在所有情况下,"分配 "都会对健康有益,但成本结构的影响取决于资源的可用性。在资源丰富的情况下,allocare 的成本结构并不重要,因为个体的繁殖成功更多地是受生育率和死亡率的制约,而不是受资源或时间的制约。然而,在资源匮乏的情况下,当分配成本的边际成本结构下降时,生殖成功率的提高幅度最大。这是因为规模经济允许所有父母以折扣价扩大照料规模。因此,我们预计分配照料的做法在不同文化中都会表现出这些模式:在稀缺的环境中,分配照料预计会集中在边际成本结构递减的做法上,其可变性要比在丰富的环境中小得多。我们讨论了几个与我们的模拟结果相一致的人种学实例,最后我们对未来的 "分配式 "建模和实证工作提出了建议。
{"title":"Cost structures and socioecological conditions impact the fitness outcomes of human alloparental care in agent-based model simulations","authors":"Elic M. Weitzel ,&nbsp;Kurt M. Wilson ,&nbsp;Laure Spake ,&nbsp;Susan B. Schaffnit ,&nbsp;Robert Lynch ,&nbsp;Rebecca Sear ,&nbsp;John H. Shaver ,&nbsp;Mary K. Shenk ,&nbsp;Richard Sosis","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106613","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106613","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Alloparental care—care provided to children who are not one's offspring—is ubiquitous across human populations. Empirical research reveals socioecological variation in who cares for children, but less attention has been paid to the type of care provided. To better understand the fitness outcomes of different forms of alloparental care, or allocare, we categorize such care into two broad forms based on economic cost structures: additive cost and declining marginal cost allocare. Additive cost allocare requires alloparents to pay equal costs for each child to whom care is provided, while declining marginal cost allocare entails reduced costs for additional children beyond the first. Given this general typology, we investigate how fitness is impacted by the type of allocare provided in socioecological conditions of scarcity or abundance. Results of an agent-based model indicate that allocare has fitness benefits in nearly all circumstances, but the impact of cost structures depends on resource availability. In contexts of abundance, the cost structure of allocare does not matter as individuals' reproductive success is instead constrained by fertility and mortality more than the availability of resources or time. In conditions of scarcity, however, the greatest increases in reproductive success are achieved when allocare has a declining marginal cost structure. This is due to an economy of scale permitting alloparents to scale up their care at discounted rates. Consequently, we expect allocare practices to exhibit these patterns cross-culturally: in contexts of scarcity allocare is anticipated to be focused on practices with declining marginal cost structures and to be much less variable than in contexts of abundance. We discuss several ethnographic examples that are consistent with the overall findings of our simulations, and we conclude with recommendations for future modeling and empirical work on allocare.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106613"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141978272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Offspring and parent preferences for a spouse or in-law in an arranged marriage context 包办婚姻中后代和父母对配偶或姻亲的偏好
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612
Elizabeth Agey , Daniel Conroy-Beam , Steven J.C. Gaulin

Parental influence over mate choice and marriage is a norm across many cultures and throughout human history; however, more attention has been given to preferences of the mating individuals than to the preferences of parents selecting a son- or daughter-in-law. Using data collected in Nepal, a culture with a tradition of arranged marriages, we compare the preferences of unmarried adults and parents of unmarried adults using a budget allocation task in which respondents allocated two budgets (high and low) to design their ideal spouse or in-law from a set of 14 traits. To compare overall similarities and differences in allocations across all 14 traits, we calculated the Mahalanobis Distance for each dyad type (father-son, father-daughter, mother-son, and mother-daughter) for high and low budgets separately. Parents and offspring disagreed more in high budget allocations than in low budget allocations, indicating that they may agree more on the minimally necessary traits in a spouse/in-law. Parents and offspring showed the largest divergence in allocations over same caste/ethnicity (preferred more by parents) and physical attractiveness (preferred more by offspring), in line with previous comparisons of parent and offspring preferences. Differences in the preferences of parents and offspring were at least as large as differences in preferences between men and women in this sample, indicating that parent-offspring disagreement is substantial and should be given more consideration in the literature on mate choice. Additional research is needed to investigate how parents and offspring negotiate their preferences in arranged marriage contexts.

在人类历史上,父母对择偶和婚姻的影响是许多文化中的一种常态;然而,人们更多关注的是交配个体的偏好,而不是父母选择儿子或儿媳的偏好。尼泊尔是一个有着包办婚姻传统的国家,我们利用在尼泊尔收集到的数据,通过预算分配任务比较了未婚成年人和未婚成年人父母的偏好,在这个任务中,受访者分配了两种预算(高和低),从 14 种特征中设计他们理想的配偶或姻亲。为了比较所有 14 个特质在分配上的整体异同,我们分别计算了每个组合类型(父子、父女、母子和母女)在高预算和低预算上的马哈拉诺比斯距离(Mahalanobis Distance)。父母和子女在高预算分配中的分歧比低预算分配中更大,这表明他们可能在配偶/姻亲的最低必要特质上有更多的共识。父母和后代在同一种姓/种族(父母更偏好)和外貌吸引力(后代更偏好)方面的分配差异最大,这与之前父母和后代偏好的比较结果一致。在这一样本中,父母和后代的偏好差异至少与男女之间的偏好差异一样大,这表明父母和后代之间的分歧很大,在择偶文献中应给予更多的考虑。还需要进行更多的研究,以调查在包办婚姻的背景下,父母和后代是如何协商他们的偏好的。
{"title":"Offspring and parent preferences for a spouse or in-law in an arranged marriage context","authors":"Elizabeth Agey ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;Steven J.C. Gaulin","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Parental influence over mate choice and marriage is a norm across many cultures and throughout human history; however, more attention has been given to preferences of the mating individuals than to the preferences of parents selecting a son- or daughter-in-law. Using data collected in Nepal, a culture with a tradition of arranged marriages, we compare the preferences of unmarried adults and parents of unmarried adults using a budget allocation task in which respondents allocated two budgets (high and low) to design their ideal spouse or in-law from a set of 14 traits. To compare overall similarities and differences in allocations across all 14 traits, we calculated the Mahalanobis Distance for each dyad type (father-son, father-daughter, mother-son, and mother-daughter) for high and low budgets separately. Parents and offspring disagreed more in high budget allocations than in low budget allocations, indicating that they may agree more on the minimally necessary traits in a spouse/in-law. Parents and offspring showed the largest divergence in allocations over same caste/ethnicity (preferred more by parents) and physical attractiveness (preferred more by offspring), in line with previous comparisons of parent and offspring preferences. Differences in the preferences of parents and offspring were at least as large as differences in preferences between men and women in this sample, indicating that parent-offspring disagreement is substantial and should be given more consideration in the literature on mate choice. Additional research is needed to investigate how parents and offspring negotiate their preferences in arranged marriage contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106612"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000886/pdfft?md5=b47ccaaf221264cac69ad5c577434df4&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000886-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141978273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Female gorillas compete for food and males 雌性大猩猩与雄性大猩猩争夺食物
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611
Nikolaos Smit, Martha M. Robbins

As most organisms, humans and other apes compete for access to fitness-determining resources and competition is expected to increase with the competitor-to-resource ratio. We use 23 years of behavioural data on five wild groups from the two gorilla species which live in different socioecological environments, to test if female gorillas compete simultaneously for food, mates and/or protectors. Females were more aggressive to each other in medium-sized groups (∩ − shaped relationship) and when the groups contained more females but fewer males (lower sex ratio). Contrarily, female-female aggression was not influenced by the operational sex ratio and female mating activity. Hence, our results support the competition for food and protectors hypotheses but cast doubt on the competition for mates hypothesis. Yet, despite female western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) experiencing higher monopolization potential of food and a lower abundance of food and males, they did not exhibit higher aggression rates than female mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Altogether, these results suggest that female competition for males outside the mating context is not unique to humans and might have common evolutionary origins in humans and other apes.

与大多数生物一样,人类和其他类人猿也会为获得决定其生存能力的资源而竞争,而且竞争会随着竞争者与资源的比例增加而加剧。我们利用生活在不同社会生态环境中的两个大猩猩物种的五个野生群体长达 23 年的行为数据,来检验雌性大猩猩是否同时为食物、配偶和/或保护者而竞争。在中等规模的群体中(∩-形关系),以及在雌性较多、雄性较少(性别比例较低)的群体中,雌性大猩猩之间的攻击性更强。相反,雌性之间的攻击性不受性别比例和雌性交配活动的影响。因此,我们的研究结果支持食物竞争假说和保护者假说,但对配偶竞争假说表示怀疑。然而,尽管雌性西部大猩猩()对食物的垄断潜力较高,食物和雄性的丰度较低,但它们并没有表现出比雌性山地大猩猩()更高的攻击率。总之,这些结果表明,雌性在交配环境之外对雄性的竞争并非人类所独有,在人类和其他类人猿中可能有共同的进化起源。
{"title":"Female gorillas compete for food and males","authors":"Nikolaos Smit,&nbsp;Martha M. Robbins","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As most organisms, humans and other apes compete for access to fitness-determining resources and competition is expected to increase with the competitor-to-resource ratio. We use 23 years of behavioural data on five wild groups from the two gorilla species which live in different socioecological environments, to test if female gorillas compete simultaneously for food, mates and/or protectors. Females were more aggressive to each other in medium-sized groups (∩ − shaped relationship) and when the groups contained more females but fewer males (lower sex ratio). Contrarily, female-female aggression was not influenced by the operational sex ratio and female mating activity. Hence, our results support the competition for food and protectors hypotheses but cast doubt on the competition for mates hypothesis. Yet, despite female western gorillas (<em>Gorilla gorilla gorilla</em>) experiencing higher monopolization potential of food and a lower abundance of food and males, they did not exhibit higher aggression rates than female mountain gorillas (<em>Gorilla beringei beringei</em>). Altogether, these results suggest that female competition for males outside the mating context is not unique to humans and might have common evolutionary origins in humans and other apes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106611"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000874/pdfft?md5=35e3fac6fed5469fa275282063a9d92f&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000874-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141942061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Venting makes people prefer—and preferentially support—us over those we vent about 发泄让人们更喜欢--更倾向于支持我们,而不是我们发泄的对象
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608
Jaimie Arona Krems , Laureon A. Merrie , Nina N. Rodriguez , Keelah E.G. Williams

People vent, as when airing grievances about one mutual friend to another. Contrary to a Freudian account, such social venting does not alleviate anger. So, what function might it serve? That people bestow more and more likely support on relatively better-liked friends—support which is associated with greater health, happiness, and economic mobility—highlights a largely overlooked challenge in social groups: competing within the group for certain group members' affections and support. Social venting might be one effective tool for meeting this challenge. We test this—and also compare venting's efficacy with other forms of communication, including a well-studied tactic of partner competition (competitor derogation). In six experiments with U.S. CloudResearch participants (N = 1723), venting causes listeners (people vented to) to prefer venters over targets (people vented about) and to preferentially benefit better-liked venters over targets in a modified Dictator Game. By obscuring the venters' intent to aggress against the target, venting might communicate target-harming information in a way that buffers venters from being perceived unfavorably. Effective venting might thus manipulate listeners' attitudes and behavior in venters' favor.

人们会发泄,比如向另一个共同的朋友倾诉不满。与弗洛伊德的说法相反,这样做并不能缓解愤怒。那么,它能起到什么作用呢?人们对相对更喜欢的朋友给予更多和更可能的支持--这种支持与更大的健康、幸福和经济流动性相关--凸显了社会群体中一个被忽视的挑战:在群体中争夺某些群体成员的情感和支持。社交发泄可能是应对这一挑战的有效工具之一。我们检验了这一点,并比较了发泄与其他交流形式的功效,包括一种研究得很透彻的伙伴竞争策略(竞争者贬损)。在以美国云研究(CloudResearch)参与者(= 1723 人)为对象的六项实验中,发泄会使倾听者(被发泄者)更喜欢发泄者,而不是发泄对象(被发泄者),并且在修改后的独裁者游戏中,更喜欢发泄者,而不是发泄对象。通过掩盖发泄者攻击目标的意图,发泄可能会以一种缓冲的方式传达伤害目标的信息,从而避免发泄者被认为是不利的。因此,有效的发泄可能会操纵听众的态度和行为,使之有利于发泄者。
{"title":"Venting makes people prefer—and preferentially support—us over those we vent about","authors":"Jaimie Arona Krems ,&nbsp;Laureon A. Merrie ,&nbsp;Nina N. Rodriguez ,&nbsp;Keelah E.G. Williams","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People vent, as when airing grievances about one mutual friend to another. Contrary to a Freudian account, such <em>social venting</em> does not alleviate anger. So, what function might it serve? That people bestow more and more likely support on relatively better-liked friends—support which is associated with greater health, happiness, and economic mobility—highlights a largely overlooked challenge in social groups: competing within the group for certain group members' affections and support. Social venting might be one effective tool for meeting this challenge. We test this—and also compare venting's efficacy with other forms of communication, including a well-studied tactic of partner competition (competitor derogation). In six experiments with U.S. CloudResearch participants (<em>N</em> = 1723), venting causes listeners (people vented to) to prefer venters over targets (people vented about) and to preferentially benefit better-liked venters over targets in a modified Dictator Game. By obscuring the venters' intent to aggress against the target, venting might communicate target-harming information in a way that buffers venters from being perceived unfavorably. Effective venting might thus manipulate listeners' attitudes and behavior in venters' favor.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106608"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141942059","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cultural evolution of witchcraft beliefs 巫术信仰的文化演变
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610
Sarah Peacey , Baihui Wu , Rebecca Grollemund , Ruth Mace

Witchcraft beliefs are historically and geographically widespread, but little is known about the cultural inheritance processes that may explain their variation between populations. A core component of witchcraft belief is that certain people (‘witches’) are thought to harm others using supernatural means. Various traits, which we refer to as the ‘witchcraft phenotype’ accompany these beliefs. Some can be classified as ‘symbolic culture’, including ideas about the typical behaviour of witches and concepts such as familiars (witches' magical helpers), and demographic traits such as the age and sex of those likely to be accused. We conducted an exploratory study of the cultural evolution of 31 witchcraft traits to examine their inferred ancestry and associations with historic population movements. We coded a dataset from ethnographic accounts of Bantu and Bantoid-speaking societies in sub-Saharan Africa (N = 84) and analysed it using phylogenetic comparative methods (PCMs). Our results estimate that while some traits, such as an ordeal to test for witchcraft, have deep history, others, such as accusations of children, may have evolved more recently, or are limited to specific clusters of societies. Demographic and symbolic cultural traits do not typically co-evolve. Our findings suggest traits have different transmission patterns, and these may result from benefits they provide or from universal psychological mechanisms that produce their recurrent evolution.

巫术信仰在历史上和地理上都很普遍,但人们对其文化传承过程却知之甚少,而这种文化传承过程可以解释不同人群之间的巫术信仰差异。巫术信仰的核心内容是认为某些人("女巫")会使用超自然手段伤害他人。伴随这些信仰而来的是各种特征,我们称之为 "巫术表型"。其中一些可以归类为 "符号文化",包括对女巫典型行为的看法和诸如 "家人"(女巫的魔法帮手)等概念,以及人口统计学特征,如可能被指控者的年龄和性别。我们对 31 种巫术特征的文化演变进行了探索性研究,以考察这些特征的推断祖先以及与历史上人口迁移的关联。我们对撒哈拉以南非洲班图人和班图语社会的人种学描述(=84)进行了编码,并使用系统发育比较方法(PCMs)对其进行了分析。我们的结果估计,虽然某些特征(如测试巫术的折磨)具有深远的历史,但其他特征(如对儿童的指控)可能是最近才发展起来的,或仅限于特定的社会群组。人口特征和象征性文化特征通常不会共同进化。我们的研究结果表明,文化特征具有不同的传播模式,这可能是由于文化特征带来了不同的利益,也可能是由于普遍的心理机制导致了文化特征的反复演化。
{"title":"The cultural evolution of witchcraft beliefs","authors":"Sarah Peacey ,&nbsp;Baihui Wu ,&nbsp;Rebecca Grollemund ,&nbsp;Ruth Mace","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Witchcraft beliefs are historically and geographically widespread, but little is known about the cultural inheritance processes that may explain their variation between populations. A core component of witchcraft belief is that certain people (‘witches’) are thought to harm others using supernatural means. Various traits, which we refer to as the ‘witchcraft phenotype’ accompany these beliefs. Some can be classified as ‘symbolic culture’, including ideas about the typical behaviour of witches and concepts such as familiars (witches' magical helpers), and demographic traits such as the age and sex of those likely to be accused. We conducted an exploratory study of the cultural evolution of 31 witchcraft traits to examine their inferred ancestry and associations with historic population movements. We coded a dataset from ethnographic accounts of Bantu and Bantoid-speaking societies in sub-Saharan Africa (<em>N</em> = 84) and analysed it using phylogenetic comparative methods (PCMs). Our results estimate that while some traits, such as an ordeal to test for witchcraft, have deep history, others, such as accusations of children, may have evolved more recently, or are limited to specific clusters of societies. Demographic and symbolic cultural traits do not typically co-evolve. Our findings suggest traits have different transmission patterns, and these may result from benefits they provide or from universal psychological mechanisms that produce their recurrent evolution.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106610"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141882636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1