首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Offspring and parent preferences for a spouse or in-law in an arranged marriage context 包办婚姻中后代和父母对配偶或姻亲的偏好
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612
Elizabeth Agey , Daniel Conroy-Beam , Steven J.C. Gaulin

Parental influence over mate choice and marriage is a norm across many cultures and throughout human history; however, more attention has been given to preferences of the mating individuals than to the preferences of parents selecting a son- or daughter-in-law. Using data collected in Nepal, a culture with a tradition of arranged marriages, we compare the preferences of unmarried adults and parents of unmarried adults using a budget allocation task in which respondents allocated two budgets (high and low) to design their ideal spouse or in-law from a set of 14 traits. To compare overall similarities and differences in allocations across all 14 traits, we calculated the Mahalanobis Distance for each dyad type (father-son, father-daughter, mother-son, and mother-daughter) for high and low budgets separately. Parents and offspring disagreed more in high budget allocations than in low budget allocations, indicating that they may agree more on the minimally necessary traits in a spouse/in-law. Parents and offspring showed the largest divergence in allocations over same caste/ethnicity (preferred more by parents) and physical attractiveness (preferred more by offspring), in line with previous comparisons of parent and offspring preferences. Differences in the preferences of parents and offspring were at least as large as differences in preferences between men and women in this sample, indicating that parent-offspring disagreement is substantial and should be given more consideration in the literature on mate choice. Additional research is needed to investigate how parents and offspring negotiate their preferences in arranged marriage contexts.

在人类历史上,父母对择偶和婚姻的影响是许多文化中的一种常态;然而,人们更多关注的是交配个体的偏好,而不是父母选择儿子或儿媳的偏好。尼泊尔是一个有着包办婚姻传统的国家,我们利用在尼泊尔收集到的数据,通过预算分配任务比较了未婚成年人和未婚成年人父母的偏好,在这个任务中,受访者分配了两种预算(高和低),从 14 种特征中设计他们理想的配偶或姻亲。为了比较所有 14 个特质在分配上的整体异同,我们分别计算了每个组合类型(父子、父女、母子和母女)在高预算和低预算上的马哈拉诺比斯距离(Mahalanobis Distance)。父母和子女在高预算分配中的分歧比低预算分配中更大,这表明他们可能在配偶/姻亲的最低必要特质上有更多的共识。父母和后代在同一种姓/种族(父母更偏好)和外貌吸引力(后代更偏好)方面的分配差异最大,这与之前父母和后代偏好的比较结果一致。在这一样本中,父母和后代的偏好差异至少与男女之间的偏好差异一样大,这表明父母和后代之间的分歧很大,在择偶文献中应给予更多的考虑。还需要进行更多的研究,以调查在包办婚姻的背景下,父母和后代是如何协商他们的偏好的。
{"title":"Offspring and parent preferences for a spouse or in-law in an arranged marriage context","authors":"Elizabeth Agey ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;Steven J.C. Gaulin","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106612","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Parental influence over mate choice and marriage is a norm across many cultures and throughout human history; however, more attention has been given to preferences of the mating individuals than to the preferences of parents selecting a son- or daughter-in-law. Using data collected in Nepal, a culture with a tradition of arranged marriages, we compare the preferences of unmarried adults and parents of unmarried adults using a budget allocation task in which respondents allocated two budgets (high and low) to design their ideal spouse or in-law from a set of 14 traits. To compare overall similarities and differences in allocations across all 14 traits, we calculated the Mahalanobis Distance for each dyad type (father-son, father-daughter, mother-son, and mother-daughter) for high and low budgets separately. Parents and offspring disagreed more in high budget allocations than in low budget allocations, indicating that they may agree more on the minimally necessary traits in a spouse/in-law. Parents and offspring showed the largest divergence in allocations over same caste/ethnicity (preferred more by parents) and physical attractiveness (preferred more by offspring), in line with previous comparisons of parent and offspring preferences. Differences in the preferences of parents and offspring were at least as large as differences in preferences between men and women in this sample, indicating that parent-offspring disagreement is substantial and should be given more consideration in the literature on mate choice. Additional research is needed to investigate how parents and offspring negotiate their preferences in arranged marriage contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106612"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000886/pdfft?md5=b47ccaaf221264cac69ad5c577434df4&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000886-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141978273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Female gorillas compete for food and males 雌性大猩猩与雄性大猩猩争夺食物
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611
Nikolaos Smit, Martha M. Robbins

As most organisms, humans and other apes compete for access to fitness-determining resources and competition is expected to increase with the competitor-to-resource ratio. We use 23 years of behavioural data on five wild groups from the two gorilla species which live in different socioecological environments, to test if female gorillas compete simultaneously for food, mates and/or protectors. Females were more aggressive to each other in medium-sized groups (∩ − shaped relationship) and when the groups contained more females but fewer males (lower sex ratio). Contrarily, female-female aggression was not influenced by the operational sex ratio and female mating activity. Hence, our results support the competition for food and protectors hypotheses but cast doubt on the competition for mates hypothesis. Yet, despite female western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) experiencing higher monopolization potential of food and a lower abundance of food and males, they did not exhibit higher aggression rates than female mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Altogether, these results suggest that female competition for males outside the mating context is not unique to humans and might have common evolutionary origins in humans and other apes.

与大多数生物一样,人类和其他类人猿也会为获得决定其生存能力的资源而竞争,而且竞争会随着竞争者与资源的比例增加而加剧。我们利用生活在不同社会生态环境中的两个大猩猩物种的五个野生群体长达 23 年的行为数据,来检验雌性大猩猩是否同时为食物、配偶和/或保护者而竞争。在中等规模的群体中(∩-形关系),以及在雌性较多、雄性较少(性别比例较低)的群体中,雌性大猩猩之间的攻击性更强。相反,雌性之间的攻击性不受性别比例和雌性交配活动的影响。因此,我们的研究结果支持食物竞争假说和保护者假说,但对配偶竞争假说表示怀疑。然而,尽管雌性西部大猩猩()对食物的垄断潜力较高,食物和雄性的丰度较低,但它们并没有表现出比雌性山地大猩猩()更高的攻击率。总之,这些结果表明,雌性在交配环境之外对雄性的竞争并非人类所独有,在人类和其他类人猿中可能有共同的进化起源。
{"title":"Female gorillas compete for food and males","authors":"Nikolaos Smit,&nbsp;Martha M. Robbins","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106611","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As most organisms, humans and other apes compete for access to fitness-determining resources and competition is expected to increase with the competitor-to-resource ratio. We use 23 years of behavioural data on five wild groups from the two gorilla species which live in different socioecological environments, to test if female gorillas compete simultaneously for food, mates and/or protectors. Females were more aggressive to each other in medium-sized groups (∩ − shaped relationship) and when the groups contained more females but fewer males (lower sex ratio). Contrarily, female-female aggression was not influenced by the operational sex ratio and female mating activity. Hence, our results support the competition for food and protectors hypotheses but cast doubt on the competition for mates hypothesis. Yet, despite female western gorillas (<em>Gorilla gorilla gorilla</em>) experiencing higher monopolization potential of food and a lower abundance of food and males, they did not exhibit higher aggression rates than female mountain gorillas (<em>Gorilla beringei beringei</em>). Altogether, these results suggest that female competition for males outside the mating context is not unique to humans and might have common evolutionary origins in humans and other apes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106611"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000874/pdfft?md5=35e3fac6fed5469fa275282063a9d92f&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000874-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141942061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Venting makes people prefer—and preferentially support—us over those we vent about 发泄让人们更喜欢--更倾向于支持我们,而不是我们发泄的对象
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608
Jaimie Arona Krems , Laureon A. Merrie , Nina N. Rodriguez , Keelah E.G. Williams

People vent, as when airing grievances about one mutual friend to another. Contrary to a Freudian account, such social venting does not alleviate anger. So, what function might it serve? That people bestow more and more likely support on relatively better-liked friends—support which is associated with greater health, happiness, and economic mobility—highlights a largely overlooked challenge in social groups: competing within the group for certain group members' affections and support. Social venting might be one effective tool for meeting this challenge. We test this—and also compare venting's efficacy with other forms of communication, including a well-studied tactic of partner competition (competitor derogation). In six experiments with U.S. CloudResearch participants (N = 1723), venting causes listeners (people vented to) to prefer venters over targets (people vented about) and to preferentially benefit better-liked venters over targets in a modified Dictator Game. By obscuring the venters' intent to aggress against the target, venting might communicate target-harming information in a way that buffers venters from being perceived unfavorably. Effective venting might thus manipulate listeners' attitudes and behavior in venters' favor.

人们会发泄,比如向另一个共同的朋友倾诉不满。与弗洛伊德的说法相反,这样做并不能缓解愤怒。那么,它能起到什么作用呢?人们对相对更喜欢的朋友给予更多和更可能的支持--这种支持与更大的健康、幸福和经济流动性相关--凸显了社会群体中一个被忽视的挑战:在群体中争夺某些群体成员的情感和支持。社交发泄可能是应对这一挑战的有效工具之一。我们检验了这一点,并比较了发泄与其他交流形式的功效,包括一种研究得很透彻的伙伴竞争策略(竞争者贬损)。在以美国云研究(CloudResearch)参与者(= 1723 人)为对象的六项实验中,发泄会使倾听者(被发泄者)更喜欢发泄者,而不是发泄对象(被发泄者),并且在修改后的独裁者游戏中,更喜欢发泄者,而不是发泄对象。通过掩盖发泄者攻击目标的意图,发泄可能会以一种缓冲的方式传达伤害目标的信息,从而避免发泄者被认为是不利的。因此,有效的发泄可能会操纵听众的态度和行为,使之有利于发泄者。
{"title":"Venting makes people prefer—and preferentially support—us over those we vent about","authors":"Jaimie Arona Krems ,&nbsp;Laureon A. Merrie ,&nbsp;Nina N. Rodriguez ,&nbsp;Keelah E.G. Williams","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106608","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People vent, as when airing grievances about one mutual friend to another. Contrary to a Freudian account, such <em>social venting</em> does not alleviate anger. So, what function might it serve? That people bestow more and more likely support on relatively better-liked friends—support which is associated with greater health, happiness, and economic mobility—highlights a largely overlooked challenge in social groups: competing within the group for certain group members' affections and support. Social venting might be one effective tool for meeting this challenge. We test this—and also compare venting's efficacy with other forms of communication, including a well-studied tactic of partner competition (competitor derogation). In six experiments with U.S. CloudResearch participants (<em>N</em> = 1723), venting causes listeners (people vented to) to prefer venters over targets (people vented about) and to preferentially benefit better-liked venters over targets in a modified Dictator Game. By obscuring the venters' intent to aggress against the target, venting might communicate target-harming information in a way that buffers venters from being perceived unfavorably. Effective venting might thus manipulate listeners' attitudes and behavior in venters' favor.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106608"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141942059","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cultural evolution of witchcraft beliefs 巫术信仰的文化演变
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610
Sarah Peacey , Baihui Wu , Rebecca Grollemund , Ruth Mace

Witchcraft beliefs are historically and geographically widespread, but little is known about the cultural inheritance processes that may explain their variation between populations. A core component of witchcraft belief is that certain people (‘witches’) are thought to harm others using supernatural means. Various traits, which we refer to as the ‘witchcraft phenotype’ accompany these beliefs. Some can be classified as ‘symbolic culture’, including ideas about the typical behaviour of witches and concepts such as familiars (witches' magical helpers), and demographic traits such as the age and sex of those likely to be accused. We conducted an exploratory study of the cultural evolution of 31 witchcraft traits to examine their inferred ancestry and associations with historic population movements. We coded a dataset from ethnographic accounts of Bantu and Bantoid-speaking societies in sub-Saharan Africa (N = 84) and analysed it using phylogenetic comparative methods (PCMs). Our results estimate that while some traits, such as an ordeal to test for witchcraft, have deep history, others, such as accusations of children, may have evolved more recently, or are limited to specific clusters of societies. Demographic and symbolic cultural traits do not typically co-evolve. Our findings suggest traits have different transmission patterns, and these may result from benefits they provide or from universal psychological mechanisms that produce their recurrent evolution.

巫术信仰在历史上和地理上都很普遍,但人们对其文化传承过程却知之甚少,而这种文化传承过程可以解释不同人群之间的巫术信仰差异。巫术信仰的核心内容是认为某些人("女巫")会使用超自然手段伤害他人。伴随这些信仰而来的是各种特征,我们称之为 "巫术表型"。其中一些可以归类为 "符号文化",包括对女巫典型行为的看法和诸如 "家人"(女巫的魔法帮手)等概念,以及人口统计学特征,如可能被指控者的年龄和性别。我们对 31 种巫术特征的文化演变进行了探索性研究,以考察这些特征的推断祖先以及与历史上人口迁移的关联。我们对撒哈拉以南非洲班图人和班图语社会的人种学描述(=84)进行了编码,并使用系统发育比较方法(PCMs)对其进行了分析。我们的结果估计,虽然某些特征(如测试巫术的折磨)具有深远的历史,但其他特征(如对儿童的指控)可能是最近才发展起来的,或仅限于特定的社会群组。人口特征和象征性文化特征通常不会共同进化。我们的研究结果表明,文化特征具有不同的传播模式,这可能是由于文化特征带来了不同的利益,也可能是由于普遍的心理机制导致了文化特征的反复演化。
{"title":"The cultural evolution of witchcraft beliefs","authors":"Sarah Peacey ,&nbsp;Baihui Wu ,&nbsp;Rebecca Grollemund ,&nbsp;Ruth Mace","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106610","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Witchcraft beliefs are historically and geographically widespread, but little is known about the cultural inheritance processes that may explain their variation between populations. A core component of witchcraft belief is that certain people (‘witches’) are thought to harm others using supernatural means. Various traits, which we refer to as the ‘witchcraft phenotype’ accompany these beliefs. Some can be classified as ‘symbolic culture’, including ideas about the typical behaviour of witches and concepts such as familiars (witches' magical helpers), and demographic traits such as the age and sex of those likely to be accused. We conducted an exploratory study of the cultural evolution of 31 witchcraft traits to examine their inferred ancestry and associations with historic population movements. We coded a dataset from ethnographic accounts of Bantu and Bantoid-speaking societies in sub-Saharan Africa (<em>N</em> = 84) and analysed it using phylogenetic comparative methods (PCMs). Our results estimate that while some traits, such as an ordeal to test for witchcraft, have deep history, others, such as accusations of children, may have evolved more recently, or are limited to specific clusters of societies. Demographic and symbolic cultural traits do not typically co-evolve. Our findings suggest traits have different transmission patterns, and these may result from benefits they provide or from universal psychological mechanisms that produce their recurrent evolution.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106610"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141882636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's evolved cues to promote caregiving: Are voices more powerful than thoughts in signaling young children's attributes and needs to adults? 儿童进化出的促进照料的线索:在向成人传递幼儿的属性和需求时,声音是否比想法更有力?
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609
Carlos Hernández Blasi , David F. Bjorklund , Sonia Agut , Francisco Lozano Nomdedeu , Miguel Ángel Martínez

Children have evolved “psychological weapons” to endear them to adults, enhancing their chances of surviving. Earlier research has shown that, during early childhood, caregivers feel positively attracted by children's vocal and cognitive cues of immaturity, which in turn provide adults with information about children's attributes and needs. The purpose of this study was to disentangle which of these two cues (vocal or cognitive), if either, might be more relevant for adults in assessing children's attributes and needs. College students (n = 273) listened to four pairs of children reasoning either in a mature or an immature manner about two types of thinking, one we labeled supernatural that reflects “magical thinking” (e.g., “The sun's not out because it's mad”) and the other we labeled natural that reflects abilities such as estimating one's cognition abilities or inhibition (e.g., “I can remember all the words you showed me”). In one condition (Consistent), the immaturity/maturity of children's reasoning matched the immaturity/maturity of their voices, whereas in the other condition (Inconsistent) they did not. Results revealed that, regardless of the type of reasoning, children's vocal cues prevailed over cognitive cues for assessing attributes of positive affect and helplessness. Conversely, children's cognitive cues prevailed over vocal cues for assessing intelligence (but only for supernatural thinking), and negative affect (but only for natural thinking). The results reveal natural selection's use of different cues of immaturity to promote caregiving during early childhood and reflect the complexity of multimodal features when adults evaluate young children.

儿童进化出了让成人喜爱的 "心理武器",从而增加了他们生存的机会。早先的研究表明,在幼儿期,照料者会被儿童的声音和认知上的不成熟暗示所吸引,这反过来又为成人提供了有关儿童属性和需求的信息。本研究的目的是要弄清这两种线索(声音线索和认知线索)中,哪一种(如果有的话)对成人评估儿童的属性和需求更有意义。大学生(n = 273)聆听了四对儿童以成熟或不成熟的方式就两种思维类型进行的推理,其中一种我们称为超自然思维,反映了 "神奇思维"(如 "太阳没出来是因为它疯了"),另一种我们称为自然思维,反映了估计自己的认知能力或抑制能力(如 "我能记住你给我看的所有单词")。在一种情况下(一致),儿童推理的不成熟/成熟度与他们声音的不成熟/成熟度相匹配,而在另一种情况下(不一致)则不然。结果显示,无论推理类型如何,在评估积极情感和无助感属性时,儿童的声音线索都优先于认知线索。相反,在评估智力(但仅限于超自然思维)和负面情绪(但仅限于自然思维)时,儿童的认知线索优先于声音线索。这些结果揭示了自然选择使用不同的不成熟线索来促进幼儿期的照顾,并反映了成人评价幼儿时多模态特征的复杂性。
{"title":"Children's evolved cues to promote caregiving: Are voices more powerful than thoughts in signaling young children's attributes and needs to adults?","authors":"Carlos Hernández Blasi ,&nbsp;David F. Bjorklund ,&nbsp;Sonia Agut ,&nbsp;Francisco Lozano Nomdedeu ,&nbsp;Miguel Ángel Martínez","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children have evolved “psychological weapons” to endear them to adults, enhancing their chances of surviving. Earlier research has shown that, during early childhood, caregivers feel positively attracted by children's vocal and cognitive cues of immaturity, which in turn provide adults with information about children's attributes and needs. The purpose of this study was to disentangle which of these two cues (vocal or cognitive), if either, might be more relevant for adults in assessing children's attributes and needs. College students (<em>n</em> = 273) listened to four pairs of children reasoning either in a mature or an immature manner about two types of thinking, one we labeled <em>supernatural</em> that reflects “magical thinking” (e.g., “The sun's not out because it's mad”) and the other we labeled <em>natural</em> that reflects abilities such as estimating one's cognition abilities or inhibition (e.g., “I can remember all the words you showed me”). In one condition (<em>Consistent</em>), the immaturity/maturity of children's reasoning matched the immaturity/maturity of their voices, whereas in the other condition (<em>Inconsistent</em>) they did not. Results revealed that, regardless of the type of reasoning, children's vocal cues prevailed over cognitive cues for assessing attributes of positive affect and helplessness. Conversely, children's cognitive cues prevailed over vocal cues for assessing intelligence (but only for supernatural thinking), and negative affect (but only for natural thinking). The results reveal natural selection's use of different cues of immaturity to promote caregiving during early childhood and reflect the complexity of multimodal features when adults evaluate young children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106609"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000850/pdfft?md5=f1efee7fcf5b8676c619e7bbb95045b7&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000850-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Religious signaling and prosociality: A review of the literature 宗教信号与亲社会性:文献综述
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002
Stefanie B. Northover , Tadeg Quillien , Daniel Conroy-Beam , Adam B. Cohen

The costly signaling theory of religion states that costly religious behaviors, badges, and bans (“religious practice” for short) are signals of commitment to the ingroup and its moral code. Such signals are proposed to increase cooperation. Here we review the empirical literature, which suggests that religious actors are often perceived as especially trustworthy and may be more likely recipients of help and cooperation. The evidence does not present a clear picture regarding the actual trustworthiness nor prosocial tendencies of religious actors. Limited available evidence suggests that routine forms of religious behavior are associated with ingroup favoritism. High-cost, infrequent, highly social forms of religious practice are associated with an increase in religious identity, but also an expanded social identity and greater tolerance for outgroup members. Following the literature review, we provide a discussion of proposed future research directions pertaining to the costs and benefits of religious practice, moderators, secular versus religious practice, and mediation of the relationship between observed religious practice and perceptions of religious actors' trustworthiness.

宗教的代价信号理论认为,代价高昂的宗教行为、徽章和禁令(简称 "宗教实践")是对内聚群体及其道德准则做出承诺的信号。这种信号被认为可以增加合作。在此,我们回顾了实证文献,这些文献表明,宗教行为者通常被认为特别值得信赖,可能更容易获得帮助与合作。关于宗教行为者的实际可信度或亲社会倾向,证据并没有给出清晰的描述。有限的现有证据表明,常规形式的宗教行为与内群体偏袒有关。高成本、非经常性、高度社会化的宗教活动形式与宗教认同感的增强有关,但也与社会认同感的扩大和对外群体成员的更大容忍度有关。在文献综述之后,我们就宗教实践的成本与收益、调节因素、世俗实践与宗教实践、观察到的宗教实践与对宗教行为者可信度认知之间关系的调解等方面的未来研究方向进行了讨论。
{"title":"Religious signaling and prosociality: A review of the literature","authors":"Stefanie B. Northover ,&nbsp;Tadeg Quillien ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;Adam B. Cohen","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The costly signaling theory of religion states that costly religious behaviors, badges, and bans (“religious practice” for short) are signals of commitment to the ingroup and its moral code. Such signals are proposed to increase cooperation. Here we review the empirical literature, which suggests that religious actors are often perceived as especially trustworthy and may be more likely recipients of help and cooperation. The evidence does not present a clear picture regarding the actual trustworthiness nor prosocial tendencies of religious actors<em>.</em> Limited available evidence suggests that routine forms of religious behavior are associated with ingroup favoritism. High-cost, infrequent, highly social forms of religious practice are associated with an increase in religious identity, but also an expanded social identity and greater tolerance for outgroup members. Following the literature review, we provide a discussion of proposed future research directions pertaining to the costs and benefits of religious practice, moderators, secular versus religious practice, and mediation of the relationship between observed religious practice and perceptions of religious actors' trustworthiness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106593"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141606889","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why women cheat: testing evolutionary hypotheses for female infidelity in a multinational sample 女性为何出轨:在多国样本中检验女性不忠的进化假设
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595
Macken Murphy , Caroline A. Phillips , Khandis R. Blake

While scholars largely agree men's infidelity evolved by increasing offspring quantity, the evolutionary drivers of women's infidelity remain debated. The “good genes” (dual mating strategy) hypothesis posits infidelity allows women to pair the preferred genes of an affair partner with the preferred investment of their primary partner (Gangstad & Thornhill, 1998). The mate-switching hypothesis instead argues infidelity helps women obtain a new mate without a period of deprivation (Buss et al., 2017). To test these hypotheses, we conducted a pre-registered survey of 254 individuals from 19 countries and 6 continents who were previously or currently engaged in infidelity. We measured individuals' perception of their primary partner and their affair partner across four domains: physical attractiveness, personal attractiveness, attractiveness as a co-parent, and overall desirability (mate value). We also asked participants to report their motivations for the affair. Consistent with a dual mating strategy, women experienced stronger physical attraction to their affair partners and stronger parental attraction to their primary partners. Contrary to the mate-switching hypothesis, women did not prefer their affair partners overall, parentally, or personally. There were no significant gender differences in these findings, suggesting strategic dualism in men as well. Our qualitative data revealed a more nuanced story at the individual level, with participants reporting motives consistent with a variety of evolutionarily coherent strategies. While our quantitative results speak to the relevance of the dual-mating hypothesis to understanding infidelity, our findings also suggest that seeking infidelity's primary explanation in either gender is, perhaps, too simple an approach to the issue.

尽管学者们普遍认为男性的不忠行为是通过增加后代数量进化而来的,但对女性不忠行为的进化动因仍存在争议。好基因"(双重交配策略)假说认为,不忠行为可以让女性将外遇伴侣的偏好基因与其主要伴侣的偏好投资配对(Gangstad & Thornhill, 1998)。伴侣转换假说则认为,不忠行为有助于女性在没有剥夺期的情况下获得新伴侣(Buss 等人,2017 年)。为了验证这些假设,我们对来自 6 大洲 19 个国家的 254 名曾经或目前有不忠行为的人进行了预先登记的调查。我们测量了个人对其主要伴侣和外遇伴侣在以下四个方面的看法:外貌吸引力、个人吸引力、作为共同父母的吸引力以及总体可取性(伴侣价值)。我们还要求参与者报告他们外遇的动机。与双重交配策略一致的是,女性对外遇伴侣的身体吸引力更强,而对主要伴侣的父母吸引力更强。与伴侣转换假说相反,女性并不更喜欢外遇伴侣的整体、父母或个人。这些研究结果没有明显的性别差异,这表明男性也存在策略双重性。我们的定性数据揭示了个人层面上更细微的故事,参与者报告的动机与各种进化上一致的策略相一致。虽然我们的定量结果说明了双重交配假说与理解不忠行为的相关性,但我们的研究结果也表明,在两性中寻找不忠行为的主要解释也许是一种过于简单的方法。
{"title":"Why women cheat: testing evolutionary hypotheses for female infidelity in a multinational sample","authors":"Macken Murphy ,&nbsp;Caroline A. Phillips ,&nbsp;Khandis R. Blake","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>While scholars largely agree men's infidelity evolved by increasing offspring quantity, the evolutionary drivers of women's infidelity remain debated. The “good genes” (dual mating strategy) hypothesis posits infidelity allows women to pair the preferred genes of an affair partner with the preferred investment of their primary partner (Gangstad &amp; Thornhill, 1998). The mate-switching hypothesis instead argues infidelity helps women obtain a new mate without a period of deprivation (Buss et al., 2017). To test these hypotheses, we conducted a pre-registered survey of 254 individuals from 19 countries and 6 continents who were previously or currently engaged in infidelity. We measured individuals' perception of their primary partner and their affair partner across four domains: physical attractiveness, personal attractiveness, attractiveness as a co-parent, and overall desirability (mate value). We also asked participants to report their motivations for the affair. Consistent with a dual mating strategy, women experienced stronger physical attraction to their affair partners and stronger parental attraction to their primary partners. Contrary to the mate-switching hypothesis, women did not prefer their affair partners overall, parentally, or personally. There were no significant gender differences in these findings, suggesting strategic dualism in men as well. Our qualitative data revealed a more nuanced story at the individual level, with participants reporting motives consistent with a variety of evolutionarily coherent strategies. While our quantitative results speak to the relevance of the dual-mating hypothesis to understanding infidelity, our findings also suggest that seeking infidelity's primary explanation in either gender is, perhaps, too simple an approach to the issue.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106595"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000710/pdfft?md5=17fac63b8dd7bb1c5f853c1541025986&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000710-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141539191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genomic findings and their implications for the evolutionary social sciences 基因组研究结果及其对进化社会科学的影响
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596
Brendan P. Zietsch

What past selection pressures have shaped human traits and their variation and covariation across individuals? These are key questions in the evolutionary social sciences. Recent advances in the field of human genomics have yielded a wealth of evidence that sheds light on these questions, yet the findings and their implications seem to be little known in the evolutionary social sciences. In this paper I aim to bring together these findings while explaining the conceptual and technical background that is often assumed knowledge for reading the primary reports. First, I outline the genomics methodologies that have enabled the relevant findings, such as genomewide association studies and DNA-based heritability estimation. I describe how these methodologies reveal the genetic architecture of traits, and then how this information in turn enables inferences about past selection. The findings show pervasive evidence that the genetic architecture of complex traits has been shaped by negative (purifying) selection, implying that the extant genetic variation in the traits has been maintained by mutation-selection-drift balance. On the other hand, there is no evidence that balancing selection has substantively shaped complex traits, and strong evidence that it has not. Finally, I discuss the implications of these findings for issues such as the dimensional structure of personality variation and the plausibility of psychological life history theory.

过去的选择压力塑造了人类的哪些特征及其在个体间的变异和共变?这些都是进化社会科学的关键问题。人类基因组学领域的最新进展提供了大量证据来揭示这些问题,但这些发现及其影响在进化社会科学领域似乎鲜为人知。在本文中,我旨在汇集这些研究成果,同时解释在阅读主要报告时通常被假定为知识的概念和技术背景。首先,我概述了促成相关发现的基因组学方法,如全基因组关联研究和基于 DNA 的遗传率估计。我将介绍这些方法如何揭示性状的遗传结构,以及这些信息如何反过来推断过去的选择。研究结果表明,有大量证据表明,复杂性状的遗传结构是由负向(净化)选择形成的,这意味着性状中现存的遗传变异是由突变-选择-漂移平衡维持的。另一方面,没有证据表明平衡选择实质上塑造了复杂性状,也有强有力的证据表明平衡选择没有塑造复杂性状。最后,我将讨论这些发现对人格变异的维度结构和心理生活史理论的合理性等问题的影响。
{"title":"Genomic findings and their implications for the evolutionary social sciences","authors":"Brendan P. Zietsch","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>What past selection pressures have shaped human traits and their variation and covariation across individuals? These are key questions in the evolutionary social sciences. Recent advances in the field of human genomics have yielded a wealth of evidence that sheds light on these questions, yet the findings and their implications seem to be little known in the evolutionary social sciences. In this paper I aim to bring together these findings while explaining the conceptual and technical background that is often assumed knowledge for reading the primary reports. First, I outline the genomics methodologies that have enabled the relevant findings, such as genomewide association studies and DNA-based heritability estimation. I describe how these methodologies reveal the genetic architecture of traits, and then how this information in turn enables inferences about past selection. The findings show pervasive evidence that the genetic architecture of complex traits has been shaped by negative (purifying) selection, implying that the extant genetic variation in the traits has been maintained by mutation-selection-drift balance. On the other hand, there is no evidence that balancing selection has substantively shaped complex traits, and strong evidence that it has not. Finally, I discuss the implications of these findings for issues such as the dimensional structure of personality variation and the plausibility of psychological life history theory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106596"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000722/pdfft?md5=645cc54ab3f014e3bc9ba1f3bac18fee&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000722-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the immediate effects of income inequality on homicide rates: A reply to Daly's critique 超越收入不平等对凶杀率的直接影响:回应戴利的批评
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597
Carlos Vilalta , Edel Cadena , Carlos Garrocho , Gustavo Fondevila

This study responds to Martin Daly's critique of our 2022 study on the correlation between income inequality and homicide rates in Mexican municipalities. Our updated analysis incorporates both immediate and lagged effects of income inequality, revealing significant non-linear relationships between past inequality and current homicide rates. We find that higher levels of past inequality interact with present inequality to increase homicide rates, particularly among currently average and highly unequal municipalities –not so much in less unequal municipalities. These findings support Daly's argument that economic inequality's influence on violent behavior accumulates over time, highlighting the need for a time dimension in homicide rate models. Our study emphasizes the importance of considering historical economic conditions when addressing socio-economic determinants of homicide, aiming to contribute constructively to ongoing attempts to effectively reduce homicidal violence.

本研究回应了马丁-戴利对我们 2022 年关于墨西哥城市收入不平等与凶杀率之间相关性研究的批评。我们的最新分析纳入了收入不平等的即时和滞后效应,揭示了过去的不平等与当前凶杀率之间的显著非线性关系。我们发现,过去较高水平的不平等与当前的不平等相互作用,增加了凶杀率,尤其是在目前处于平均水平和高度不平等的城市中,而在不太平等的城市中则没有这么严重。这些研究结果支持了 Daly 的观点,即经济不平等对暴力行为的影响会随着时间的推移而不断累积,从而突出了在凶杀率模型中加入时间维度的必要性。我们的研究强调了在处理杀人案的社会经济决定因素时考虑历史经济条件的重要性,旨在为目前有效减少杀人暴力的尝试做出建设性贡献。
{"title":"Beyond the immediate effects of income inequality on homicide rates: A reply to Daly's critique","authors":"Carlos Vilalta ,&nbsp;Edel Cadena ,&nbsp;Carlos Garrocho ,&nbsp;Gustavo Fondevila","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study responds to Martin Daly's critique of our 2022 study on the correlation between income inequality and homicide rates in Mexican municipalities. Our updated analysis incorporates both immediate and lagged effects of income inequality, revealing significant non-linear relationships between past inequality and current homicide rates. We find that higher levels of past inequality interact with present inequality to increase homicide rates, particularly among currently average and highly unequal municipalities –not so much in less unequal municipalities. These findings support Daly's argument that economic inequality's influence on violent behavior accumulates over time, highlighting the need for a time dimension in homicide rate models. Our study emphasizes the importance of considering historical economic conditions when addressing socio-economic determinants of homicide, aiming to contribute constructively to ongoing attempts to effectively reduce homicidal violence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106597"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prestige and dominance in egalitarian and hierarchical societies: Children in Finland favor prestige more than children in Colombia or the USA 平等社会和等级社会中的声望和支配地位:芬兰儿童比哥伦比亚或美国儿童更喜欢威望
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005
Maija-Eliina Sequeira , Narges Afshordi , Anni Kajanus

We examined how children reason about dominance and prestige in Colombia, Finland, and the USA, contexts that vary in terms of societal inequality and hierarchical organization. We tested 496 children aged 4–11 years old to determine whether they: i) recognized and discriminated between dominance and prestige, ii) preferred to learn from a dominant or prestigious character, iii) assigned leadership to a dominant or prestigious character, and iv) self-identified more with a dominant or subordinate character. Older children were more likely to recognize, prefer, learn from, and assign leadership to the prestigious character, and to identify with the subordinate. There were no cross-cultural differences in learning preferences, supporting evolutionary theories that posit a universal bias towards social learning from prestigious individuals. There was variation in leadership preferences; children were the most likely to assign leadership to a prestigious character in more egalitarian Finland, and least likely in more unequal Colombia. We argue that societal factors including levels of inequality and hierarchical social organization shape an underlying propensity for children to learn to reason about rank and to broadly favor prestige in leaders and models for learning from.

我们研究了哥伦比亚、芬兰和美国儿童如何推理支配地位和威望,这些国家的社会不平等和等级组织各不相同。我们对 496 名 4-11 岁的儿童进行了测试,以确定他们是否:i) 认识并区分支配地位和威望;ii) 喜欢向支配地位或威望高的角色学习;iii) 将领导权赋予支配地位或威望高的角色;以及 iv) 更认同支配地位或从属地位的角色。年龄较大的儿童更有可能认识到、更喜欢、更愿意向有威望的角色学习,并将领导权分配给有威望的角色,也更有可能认同从属角色。在学习偏好方面没有跨文化差异,这支持了进化论的观点,即人们普遍倾向于向有威望的人学习社会知识。在领导力偏好方面存在差异;在较为平等的芬兰,儿童最有可能将领导力赋予有威望的人物,而在较为不平等的哥伦比亚,儿童最不可能将领导力赋予有威望的人物。我们认为,包括不平等程度和等级社会组织在内的社会因素形成了一种潜在的倾向,即儿童学会推理等级,并普遍倾向于选择有威望的领导者和学习榜样。
{"title":"Prestige and dominance in egalitarian and hierarchical societies: Children in Finland favor prestige more than children in Colombia or the USA","authors":"Maija-Eliina Sequeira ,&nbsp;Narges Afshordi ,&nbsp;Anni Kajanus","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined how children reason about dominance and prestige in Colombia, Finland, and the USA, contexts that vary in terms of societal inequality and hierarchical organization. We tested 496 children aged 4–11 years old to determine whether they: i) recognized and discriminated between dominance and prestige, ii) preferred to learn from a dominant or prestigious character, iii) assigned leadership to a dominant or prestigious character, and iv) self-identified more with a dominant or subordinate character. Older children were more likely to recognize, prefer, learn from, and assign leadership to the prestigious character, and to identify with the subordinate. There were no cross-cultural differences in learning preferences, supporting evolutionary theories that posit a universal bias towards social learning from prestigious individuals. There was variation in leadership preferences; children were the most likely to assign leadership to a prestigious character in more egalitarian Finland, and least likely in more unequal Colombia. We argue that societal factors including levels of inequality and hierarchical social organization shape an underlying propensity for children to learn to reason about rank and to broadly favor prestige in leaders and models for learning from.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106591"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000679/pdfft?md5=4bf084df9e4709d3f8c1a4f77125e9dc&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000679-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1