首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Robust evidence that mobile hunter-gatherers participated in war: Comment on Fry (2025)
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106658
Luke Glowacki
{"title":"Robust evidence that mobile hunter-gatherers participated in war: Comment on Fry (2025)","authors":"Luke Glowacki","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106658","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106658","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106658"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143512153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infrastructure of mother-infant interactions across development in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the wild
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106671
Bas van Boekholt, Simone Pika
The infrastructure underlying human social interaction can be described by several characteristics, such as the exchange of signals and actions, specific temporal relationships, and the use of directed gaze and body direction. These characteristics are remarkably uniform across several different languages, cultures with some of them emerging in mother-infant interactions early in development before the onset of words. It has been suggested that distinct features underlying human social action might have preceded the evolution of language and are shared across the whole primate lineage. However, despite decades of research on nonhuman primate communication, our understanding of general characteristics underlying communicative interactions, and, more specifically, the role they play in the development of communication, remains surprisingly limited. Hence, here we aimed to gain a more comprehensive overview, by studying mother-infant interactions of one of our closest living relatives, the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), living in their natural environments. Specifically, we addressed the following two research questions: 1) Which characteristics built the main infrastructure of mother-infant interactions? 2) Which factors influence the infrastructure of mother-infant interactions? To answer these questions, we observed communicative interactions of a total of 17 chimpanzee mother-infant dyads (0–5 years) in the Ngogo community, Kibale National Park, Uganda between February 2021 and February 2023 (N = 1295 observation hours). We specifically focused on four different contexts where interactions frequently occurred, food sharing, nursing, grooming and joint-travel, and investigated the role of demographic factors (age and sex of the infant, interactant class) and interactional factors (context, unit type, turn transition). The results showed that mother-infant interactions were characterized by an equally distributed exchange of signals and actions, showed response times ranging from zero to two seconds, and involved the establishment and maintenance of participation frameworks through high frequencies of directed gaze and body direction. There was little to no effect of age and sex of the infant, interactant class, unit type and turn transition on these characteristics. However, context had a strong influence with relative lower frequencies of signals, quicker response times, and lower frequencies of directed gaze and body direction observed in the joint-travel context. By taking a comparative developmental approach, this study highlights commonalities in the infrastructure of mother-infant interactions between humans and chimpanzees, which contribute to uncover how extinct humans might have socially interacted.
{"title":"Infrastructure of mother-infant interactions across development in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the wild","authors":"Bas van Boekholt,&nbsp;Simone Pika","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106671","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106671","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The infrastructure underlying human social interaction can be described by several characteristics, such as the exchange of signals and actions, specific temporal relationships, and the use of directed gaze and body direction. These characteristics are remarkably uniform across several different languages, cultures with some of them emerging in mother-infant interactions early in development before the onset of words. It has been suggested that distinct features underlying human social action might have preceded the evolution of language and are shared across the whole primate lineage. However, despite decades of research on nonhuman primate communication, our understanding of general characteristics underlying communicative interactions, and, more specifically, the role they play in the development of communication, remains surprisingly limited. Hence, here we aimed to gain a more comprehensive overview, by studying mother-infant interactions of one of our closest living relatives, the chimpanzee (<em>Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii</em>), living in their natural environments. Specifically, we addressed the following two research questions: 1) Which characteristics built the main infrastructure of mother-infant interactions? 2) Which factors influence the infrastructure of mother-infant interactions? To answer these questions, we observed communicative interactions of a total of 17 chimpanzee mother-infant dyads (0–5 years) in the Ngogo community, Kibale National Park, Uganda between February 2021 and February 2023 (<em>N</em> = 1295 observation hours). We specifically focused on four different contexts where interactions frequently occurred, food sharing, nursing, grooming and joint-travel, and investigated the role of demographic factors (age and sex of the infant, interactant class) and interactional factors (context, unit type, turn transition). The results showed that mother-infant interactions were characterized by an equally distributed exchange of signals and actions, showed response times ranging from zero to two seconds, and involved the establishment and maintenance of participation frameworks through high frequencies of directed gaze and body direction. There was little to no effect of age and sex of the infant, interactant class, unit type and turn transition on these characteristics. However, context had a strong influence with relative lower frequencies of signals, quicker response times, and lower frequencies of directed gaze and body direction observed in the joint-travel context. By taking a comparative developmental approach, this study highlights commonalities in the infrastructure of mother-infant interactions between humans and chimpanzees, which contribute to uncover how extinct humans might have socially interacted.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106671"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143561693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Live fast, die young, leave a good-looking offspring: longevity and reproduction trade-offs in rural Spain (1536–1965)
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106673
Francisco J. Marco-Gracia , Ángel Luis González-Esteban
One of the most debated evolutionary theories is the trade-off between fertility and longevity. Specifically, women who experience more childbirths may have a shorter lifespan due to reduced opportunities for their bodies to recover from the associated costs. Previous studies have produced varied results, largely due to the challenge of distinguishing evolutionary factors from genetic and environmental influences in human societies. Since both longevity and fertility patterns tend to be inherited across generations, long-term historical longitudinal population samples are particularly valuable for exploring this trade-off, as they not only provide large population samples across different temporal contexts but also allow for the control of intergenerational transmission of behavior. In this study, we use a sample of 5097 women from rural Spain (based on parish records from 17 villages) who died between the sixteenth and twenty-first centuries to investigate whether a trade-off between fertility and longevity has existed. In all cases, we can control for the experiences of their parents. Our findings reveal a substantial negative impact of parity on women's longevity, as well as the adverse effect of having an early birth before fertility control was established. Additionally, we observed a significant negative effect of a prolonged reproductive cycle on natural fertility.
{"title":"Live fast, die young, leave a good-looking offspring: longevity and reproduction trade-offs in rural Spain (1536–1965)","authors":"Francisco J. Marco-Gracia ,&nbsp;Ángel Luis González-Esteban","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106673","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106673","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>One of the most debated evolutionary theories is the trade-off between fertility and longevity. Specifically, women who experience more childbirths may have a shorter lifespan due to reduced opportunities for their bodies to recover from the associated costs. Previous studies have produced varied results, largely due to the challenge of distinguishing evolutionary factors from genetic and environmental influences in human societies. Since both longevity and fertility patterns tend to be inherited across generations, long-term historical longitudinal population samples are particularly valuable for exploring this trade-off, as they not only provide large population samples across different temporal contexts but also allow for the control of intergenerational transmission of behavior. In this study, we use a sample of 5097 women from rural Spain (based on parish records from 17 villages) who died between the sixteenth and twenty-first centuries to investigate whether a trade-off between fertility and longevity has existed. In all cases, we can control for the experiences of their parents. Our findings reveal a substantial negative impact of parity on women's longevity, as well as the adverse effect of having an early birth before fertility control was established. Additionally, we observed a significant negative effect of a prolonged reproductive cycle on natural fertility.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106673"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143561691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating perceptions of fetal resemblance
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106670
Carlota Batres , Amy Mullen , Sonya Krofl , Lauren Trainor
Previous research has found that mothers are more likely to ascribe paternal resemblance to newborns. Moreover, studies have found that fathers who perceive that their children resemble them invest more in those children. In this study, we aimed to examine if maternal claims of paternal resemblance exist even with very limited visual information by asking parents whom they believed the fetus looked like during an ultrasound. We found that mothers, but not fathers, were more likely to say that the fetus resembled the father. Additionally, we found that women who were not married were even more likely to say that the fetus resembled the father. By claiming phenotypic similarity with the father, mothers are reducing paternity uncertainty and, consequently, securing investment for their offspring from when they are in utero.
{"title":"Investigating perceptions of fetal resemblance","authors":"Carlota Batres ,&nbsp;Amy Mullen ,&nbsp;Sonya Krofl ,&nbsp;Lauren Trainor","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106670","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106670","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Previous research has found that mothers are more likely to ascribe paternal resemblance to newborns. Moreover, studies have found that fathers who perceive that their children resemble them invest more in those children. In this study, we aimed to examine if maternal claims of paternal resemblance exist even with very limited visual information by asking parents whom they believed the fetus looked like during an ultrasound. We found that mothers, but not fathers, were more likely to say that the fetus resembled the father. Additionally, we found that women who were not married were even more likely to say that the fetus resembled the father. By claiming phenotypic similarity with the father, mothers are reducing paternity uncertainty and, consequently, securing investment for their offspring from when they are in utero.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106670"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143600617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The function of love: A signaling-to-alternatives account of the commitment device hypothesis
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106672
Benjamin Gelbart , Kathryn V. Walter , Daniel Conroy-Beam , Casey Estorque , David M. Buss , Kelly Asao , Agnieszka Sorokowska , Piotr Sorokowski , Toivo Aavik , Grace Akello , Mohammad Madallh Alhabahba , Charlotte Alm , Naumana Amjad , Afifa Anjum , Chiemezie S. Atama , Derya Atamtürk Duyar , Carlota Batres , Mons Bendixen , Aicha Bensafia , Boris Bizumic , Maja Zupancic
Love is commonly hypothesized to function as an evolved commitment device, disincentivizing the pursuit of romantic alternatives and signaling this motivational shift to a partner. Here, we test this possibility against a novel signaling-to-alternatives account, in which love instead operates by dissuading alternatives from pursuing oneself. Overall, we find stronger support for the latter account. In Studies 1 and 2, we find that partner quality relative to alternatives positively predicts feelings of love, and love fails to mitigate the negative effects of desirable alternatives on relationship satisfaction—contradicting the classic commitment device account. In Study 3, using a longitudinal design, we replicate these effects and find that changes in partner quality relative to alternatives predict changes in love over time. In Study 4, we replicate the relationship between love and relative partner quality across 44 countries. In Study 5, we find a nearly one-to-one correspondence between the extent to which partner-directed actions are diagnostic of love and reductions in romantic alternatives' attraction to the actor. These results suggest that love may not act as a commitment device in the classic sense by disincentivizing the pursuit of alternatives but by disincentivizing alternatives from pursuing oneself.
{"title":"The function of love: A signaling-to-alternatives account of the commitment device hypothesis","authors":"Benjamin Gelbart ,&nbsp;Kathryn V. Walter ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;Casey Estorque ,&nbsp;David M. Buss ,&nbsp;Kelly Asao ,&nbsp;Agnieszka Sorokowska ,&nbsp;Piotr Sorokowski ,&nbsp;Toivo Aavik ,&nbsp;Grace Akello ,&nbsp;Mohammad Madallh Alhabahba ,&nbsp;Charlotte Alm ,&nbsp;Naumana Amjad ,&nbsp;Afifa Anjum ,&nbsp;Chiemezie S. Atama ,&nbsp;Derya Atamtürk Duyar ,&nbsp;Carlota Batres ,&nbsp;Mons Bendixen ,&nbsp;Aicha Bensafia ,&nbsp;Boris Bizumic ,&nbsp;Maja Zupancic","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106672","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106672","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Love is commonly hypothesized to function as an evolved commitment device, disincentivizing the pursuit of romantic alternatives and signaling this motivational shift to a partner. Here, we test this possibility against a novel signaling-to-alternatives account, in which love instead operates by dissuading alternatives from pursuing oneself. Overall, we find stronger support for the latter account. In Studies 1 and 2, we find that partner quality relative to alternatives positively predicts feelings of love, and love fails to mitigate the negative effects of desirable alternatives on relationship satisfaction—contradicting the classic commitment device account. In Study 3, using a longitudinal design, we replicate these effects and find that changes in partner quality relative to alternatives predict changes in love over time. In Study 4, we replicate the relationship between love and relative partner quality across 44 countries. In Study 5, we find a nearly one-to-one correspondence between the extent to which partner-directed actions are diagnostic of love and reductions in romantic alternatives' attraction to the actor. These results suggest that love may not act as a commitment device in the classic sense by disincentivizing the pursuit of alternatives but by disincentivizing alternatives from pursuing oneself.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106672"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143611704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of synchronous chanting and identity fusion on perceived ingroup formidability, outgroup threat, and parochial altruism among soccer fans
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106666
Tiago Bortolini , Martha Newson , Francisco Tovar-Moll , Sofia Latgé-Tovar , Harvey Whitehouse , Jorge Moll , Ronald Fischer
Previous research indicates that social synchrony simultaneously increases ingroup bonding and prosociality, whilst also having the potential to make groups appear more formidable or threatening. However, many such studies have been carried out in artificial settings or in minimal groups which lack preexisting bonds or histories of prosocial action or intergroup rivalry. Here we investigate: 1) whether a synchronous display is perceived as more formidable by ingroups and outgroups, and more threatening by outgroups, in a real-world context involving football (soccer) fans; 2) whether these perceptions extend to behavioral decisions towards ingroups and outgroups; and 3) whether ‘identity fusion’ with the ingroup moderates these relationships. A sample of 771 Brazilian soccer fans took part in a pre-registered conceptual replication online study in which they randomly listened to either a synchronous or asynchronous soccer chant. Both ingroups and outgroups judged the synchronous stimuli to make the performers seem more formidable, but not more threatening. At the behavioral level, we found no effect for synchrony on pro-group behaviors or outgroup derogation, but there was a significant interaction indicating that highly ‘fused’ participants who perceived rival fans to be more threatening tended to engage in more outgroup hostility, even at a cost to self. These results parallel previous studies showing synchrony's effects on perceived ingroup formidability and provide novel insights into the role of social bonding and perceived outgroup threat as contributing factors in the evolution of intergroup conflict in real world situations.
{"title":"Effects of synchronous chanting and identity fusion on perceived ingroup formidability, outgroup threat, and parochial altruism among soccer fans","authors":"Tiago Bortolini ,&nbsp;Martha Newson ,&nbsp;Francisco Tovar-Moll ,&nbsp;Sofia Latgé-Tovar ,&nbsp;Harvey Whitehouse ,&nbsp;Jorge Moll ,&nbsp;Ronald Fischer","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106666","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106666","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Previous research indicates that social synchrony simultaneously increases ingroup bonding and prosociality, whilst also having the potential to make groups appear more formidable or threatening. However, many such studies have been carried out in artificial settings or in minimal groups which lack preexisting bonds or histories of prosocial action or intergroup rivalry. Here we investigate: 1) whether a synchronous display is perceived as more formidable by ingroups and outgroups, and more threatening by outgroups, in a real-world context involving football (soccer) fans; 2) whether these perceptions extend to behavioral decisions towards ingroups and outgroups; and 3) whether ‘identity fusion’ with the ingroup moderates these relationships. A sample of 771 Brazilian soccer fans took part in a pre-registered conceptual replication online study in which they randomly listened to either a synchronous or asynchronous soccer chant. Both ingroups and outgroups judged the synchronous stimuli to make the performers seem more formidable, but not more threatening. At the behavioral level, we found no effect for synchrony on pro-group behaviors or outgroup derogation, but there was a significant interaction indicating that highly ‘fused’ participants who perceived rival fans to be more threatening tended to engage in more outgroup hostility, even at a cost to self. These results parallel previous studies showing synchrony's effects on perceived ingroup formidability and provide novel insights into the role of social bonding and perceived outgroup threat as contributing factors in the evolution of intergroup conflict in real world situations.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"Article 106666"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143403476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why honor heroes? The emergence of extreme altruistic behavior as a by-product of praisers' self-promotion
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106656
Jean-Louis Dessalles
Heroes are people who perform costly altruistic acts. Few people turn out to be heroes, but many spontaneously honor heroes by commenting, applauding, or enthusiastically celebrating their deeds. The existence of a praising audience leads individuals to compete to attract the crowd's admiration. The outcome is a winner-take-all situation in which only one or a few individuals engage in extreme altruistic behavior. The more difficult part is to explain the crowd's propensity to pay tribute from an individual fitness optimization perspective. The model proposed here shows how heroic behavior and its celebration by a large audience may emerge together. This situation is possible if admirers use public praise as a social signal to promote their own commitment to the values displayed by the hero.
{"title":"Why honor heroes? The emergence of extreme altruistic behavior as a by-product of praisers' self-promotion","authors":"Jean-Louis Dessalles","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106656","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106656","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Heroes are people who perform costly altruistic acts. Few people turn out to be heroes, but many spontaneously honor heroes by commenting, applauding, or enthusiastically celebrating their deeds. The existence of a praising audience leads individuals to compete to attract the crowd's admiration. The outcome is a winner-take-all situation in which only one or a few individuals engage in extreme altruistic behavior. The more difficult part is to explain the crowd's propensity to pay tribute from an individual fitness optimization perspective. The model proposed here shows how heroic behavior and its celebration by a large audience may emerge together. This situation is possible if admirers use public praise as a social signal to promote their own commitment to the values displayed by the hero.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"Article 106656"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143152205","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The social benefits of “anti-social” punishment
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106655
Alain Schläpfer
Punishment of defectors is believed to be a key factor in sustaining cooperation among unrelated individuals. However, several studies have shown that humans also frequently punish those that act cooperatively, a phenomenon termed anti-social punishment. While subsequent work has suggested reasons why anti-social punishment may be individually optimal, it is universally considered to be detrimental to cooperation and thus indeed “anti-social”. This study contradicts this view, showing that punishment of cooperators can be a positive factor in sustaining cooperation rates when used by conditional cooperators against those who cooperate unconditionally. This suggests that judging whether a punitive act is beneficial or detrimental to cooperation is more complex than previously thought.
{"title":"The social benefits of “anti-social” punishment","authors":"Alain Schläpfer","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106655","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106655","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Punishment of defectors is believed to be a key factor in sustaining cooperation among unrelated individuals. However, several studies have shown that humans also frequently punish those that act cooperatively, a phenomenon termed anti-social punishment. While subsequent work has suggested reasons why anti-social punishment may be individually optimal, it is universally considered to be detrimental to cooperation and thus indeed “anti-social”. This study contradicts this view, showing that punishment of cooperators can be a positive factor in sustaining cooperation rates when used by conditional cooperators against those who cooperate unconditionally. This suggests that judging whether a punitive act is beneficial or detrimental to cooperation is more complex than previously thought.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"Article 106655"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143152206","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional tears: What they are and how they work
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106652
Daniel Sznycer , Asmir Gračanin , Debra Lieberman
Although much is known about emotional tears from the perspectives of neurobiology and behavior, the production of emotional tears and the responses they elicit depend sensitively on a rich set of computations—one which has received little attention to date. This review article aims to close this gap. Emotional tearing occurs during negative events (e.g., injuries) and positive events (e.g., achievements). Episodes of tearing appear to be united by tearers' subjective imputation of negative or positive value to certain internal or external phenomena. Knowing the degree to which objects, organisms, events, and states of affairs enhance or diminish one's prospects—the value of things—is a pressing matter for humans and other organisms. Value information is produced for internal consumption, to be used by behavior-regulating mechanisms in the focal individual. But some evaluations are made available, in addition, to other people, through tearing and other forms of verbal and non-verbal communication. Tearing may function as an implicit plea for receivers (the tear targets) to minimize the costs imposed on the tearer by nature, by third-parties, or by the tear targets themselves—common when the tearer has lower formidability or wherewithal than tear targets do. In addition, tearing may exhort tear targets to infer and register which things the tearer values, positively or negatively. Here, we characterize tears, describe the game-theoretic logic of bargaining from a position of weakness, outline the computational systems that regulate the production of and responses to emotional tears, and review findings about emotional tearing that are relevant to the signaling hypothesis.
{"title":"Emotional tears: What they are and how they work","authors":"Daniel Sznycer ,&nbsp;Asmir Gračanin ,&nbsp;Debra Lieberman","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106652","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2025.106652","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Although much is known about emotional tears from the perspectives of neurobiology and behavior, the production of emotional tears and the responses they elicit depend sensitively on a rich set of computations—one which has received little attention to date. This review article aims to close this gap. Emotional tearing occurs during negative events (e.g., injuries) and positive events (e.g., achievements). Episodes of tearing appear to be united by tearers' subjective imputation of negative or positive value to certain internal or external phenomena. Knowing the degree to which objects, organisms, events, and states of affairs enhance or diminish one's prospects—the value of things—is a pressing matter for humans and other organisms. Value information is produced for internal consumption, to be used by behavior-regulating mechanisms in the focal individual. But some evaluations are made available, in addition, to other people, through tearing and other forms of verbal and non-verbal communication. Tearing may function as an implicit plea for receivers (the tear targets) to minimize the costs imposed on the tearer by nature, by third-parties, or by the tear targets themselves—common when the tearer has lower formidability or wherewithal than tear targets do. In addition, tearing may exhort tear targets to infer and register which things the tearer values, positively or negatively. Here, we characterize tears, describe the game-theoretic logic of bargaining from a position of weakness, outline the computational systems that regulate the production of and responses to emotional tears, and review findings about emotional tearing that are relevant to the signaling hypothesis.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"Article 106652"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143152151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Headmen, shamans, and mothers: Natural and sexual selection for computational services
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106651
Edward H. Hagen , Zachary H. Garfield , Aaron D. Lightner
Computer engineers face a dilemma. They must build systems with sufficient resources to solve the most complex problems the systems are expected to solve, but the systems will only need to solve such problems intermittently, resulting in inefficient use of expensive computational resources. This dilemma is commonly resolved with timesharing, networking, multitasking, and other technologies that enable computational resources to be shared with multiple users. The human brain, which evolved to acquire, store, and process information to make beneficial decisions in situations that were periodically complex, is likewise energetically expensive to build and maintain yet plausibly has idle capacity much of the time. We propose that humans evolved to use advantages in information or computational resources to provide computational services to others via a language-based “network” in exchange for payments of various sorts that helped subsidize the energetic costs of the brain. Specifically, we argue that with the Pleistocene transition of Homo to a niche in open habitats with a more meat-based diet, four major selection pressures for knowledge specialists began to act on the human lineage: (1) the need to resolve conflicts and maintain cooperation in larger multilevel societies, which lead to the rise of knowledge-based leaders as decision-making and conflict resolution specialists who were “paid” with increased mating success or resources; (2) the need for greater defense against zoonotic pathogens, which lead to the rise of shamans as medical knowledge specialists, who were “paid” with increased mating success or resources; (3) the greater complexity of mothering with shorter interbirth intervals and longer periods of juvenile dependency, which led to mothers as both decision-making and medical specialists, who were “paid” with increased inclusive fitness; and (4) the need to make more efficient use of an increasingly large and energetically expensive brain.
{"title":"Headmen, shamans, and mothers: Natural and sexual selection for computational services","authors":"Edward H. Hagen ,&nbsp;Zachary H. Garfield ,&nbsp;Aaron D. Lightner","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106651","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106651","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Computer engineers face a dilemma. They must build systems with sufficient resources to solve the most complex problems the systems are expected to solve, but the systems will only need to solve such problems intermittently, resulting in inefficient use of expensive computational resources. This dilemma is commonly resolved with timesharing, networking, multitasking, and other technologies that enable computational resources to be shared with multiple users. The human brain, which evolved to acquire, store, and process information to make beneficial decisions in situations that were periodically complex, is likewise energetically expensive to build and maintain yet plausibly has idle capacity much of the time. We propose that humans evolved to use advantages in information or computational resources to provide computational services to others via a language-based “network” in exchange for payments of various sorts that helped subsidize the energetic costs of the brain. Specifically, we argue that with the Pleistocene transition of <em>Homo</em> to a niche in open habitats with a more meat-based diet, four major selection pressures for knowledge specialists began to act on the human lineage: (1) the need to resolve conflicts and maintain cooperation in larger multilevel societies, which lead to the rise of knowledge-based leaders as decision-making and conflict resolution specialists who were “paid” with increased mating success or resources; (2) the need for greater defense against zoonotic pathogens, which lead to the rise of shamans as medical knowledge specialists, who were “paid” with increased mating success or resources; (3) the greater complexity of mothering with shorter interbirth intervals and longer periods of juvenile dependency, which led to mothers as both decision-making and medical specialists, who were “paid” with increased inclusive fitness; and (4) the need to make more efficient use of an increasingly large and energetically expensive brain.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"Article 106651"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143152153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1