首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Genetic markers of cousin marriage and honour cultures 表亲婚姻和荣誉文化的遗传标记
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106636
Olympia L.K. Campbell , Cecilia Padilla-Iglesias , Grégory Fiorio , Ruth Mace
Honour cultures, characterized by violent responses to perceived threats to personal or family honour, are widespread. Honour killings, one of the manifestations of honour cultures, claims the lives of thousands of women each year, often at the hands of close relatives, representing not only a social problem but also an evolutionary puzzle. They typically follow accusations of sexual impropriety and are the most extreme manifestation of a range of punishments that control the sexual and marital choices of women. The origins of such practises remain unclear, though honour cultures frequently occur where cousin marriage is common. We propose that cousin marriage offers kin benefits through wealth consolidation yet may also generate parent-offspring conflict over marriage choices. In response, norms and punitive measures, including aspects of honour codes, may have evolved to enforce cousin marriage. To test this, we use the average genomic inbreeding coefficient of an ethnic group, as a measure of the historical practice of cousin marriage, to show that this is associated with the likelihood of endorsing honour killings across 52 ethnic groups. We interpret our findings within the context of parent-offspring conflict over consanguineous marriage and we contribute to the growing body of research exploring the relationship between intensive kinship and cultural traits.
名誉文化的特点是对个人或家庭名誉受到的威胁做出暴力反应,这种文化非常普遍。名誉杀人是名誉文化的表现形式之一,每年有成千上万的妇女因此丧生,而且往往是死于近亲之手,这不仅是一个社会问题,也是一个进化难题。这种做法通常是在被指控性行为不端之后发生的,是控制妇女性选择和婚姻选择的一系列惩罚措施中最极端的表现形式。尽管在表兄妹婚姻盛行的地方经常出现荣誉文化,但这种习俗的起源仍不清楚。我们认为,表亲婚姻通过巩固财富为亲属带来利益,但也可能因婚姻选择而引发父母与后代之间的冲突。作为回应,规范和惩罚措施(包括荣誉法典的某些方面)可能已经演变为强制表亲婚姻。为了验证这一点,我们使用一个族群的平均基因组近亲繁殖系数来衡量历史上的表亲婚姻习俗,结果表明,在 52 个族群中,近亲繁殖系数与认可名誉杀人的可能性相关。我们在近亲结婚引发的父母与后代冲突的背景下解释了我们的研究结果,并为探索密集亲缘关系与文化特征之间关系的越来越多的研究做出了贡献。
{"title":"Genetic markers of cousin marriage and honour cultures","authors":"Olympia L.K. Campbell ,&nbsp;Cecilia Padilla-Iglesias ,&nbsp;Grégory Fiorio ,&nbsp;Ruth Mace","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106636","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106636","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Honour cultures, characterized by violent responses to perceived threats to personal or family honour, are widespread. Honour killings, one of the manifestations of honour cultures, claims the lives of thousands of women each year, often at the hands of close relatives, representing not only a social problem but also an evolutionary puzzle. They typically follow accusations of sexual impropriety and are the most extreme manifestation of a range of punishments that control the sexual and marital choices of women. The origins of such practises remain unclear, though honour cultures frequently occur where cousin marriage is common. We propose that cousin marriage offers kin benefits through wealth consolidation yet may also generate parent-offspring conflict over marriage choices. In response, norms and punitive measures, including aspects of honour codes, may have evolved to enforce cousin marriage. To test this, we use the average genomic inbreeding coefficient of an ethnic group, as a measure of the historical practice of cousin marriage, to show that this is associated with the likelihood of endorsing honour killings across 52 ethnic groups. We interpret our findings within the context of parent-offspring conflict over consanguineous marriage and we contribute to the growing body of research exploring the relationship between intensive kinship and cultural traits.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106636"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142650751","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Even small differences in attractiveness and formidability affect the probability and speed of selection: An online study and an offline replication 即使吸引力和可塑造性的微小差异也会影响选择的概率和速度:在线研究和离线复制
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106634
Vít Třebický , Petr Tureček , Jitka Třebická Fialová , Žaneta Pátková , Dominika Grygarová , Jan Havlíček
Facial and bodily features represent salient visual stimuli upon which people spontaneously attribute various fitness-relevant characteristics such as attractiveness or formidability. While existing evidence predominantly relies on sequential stimuli presentation tasks, real-world social comparisons often involve assessing two or multiple individuals. In studies using two-alternative forced-choice tasks, participants usually perform at rates above the chance to select the expected option. However, these tasks use dichotomized and artificially manipulated stimuli that lack generalizability in situations where the differences between individuals are less likely to be ‘clear-cut’. We tested whether the probability of selection will proportionally increase with increasing degrees of difference between the stimuli or whether there is a discrimination threshold if the stimuli are perceived as too similar. In two registered studies comprising online (N = 446) and onsite (N = 56) participants, we explored the influence of the degree of difference in attractiveness and formidability ratings between stimuli pairs on both the probability of selection and selection speed. Participants were presented with randomly selected pairs of men (30 pairs of faces, 30 pairs of bodies) and tasked with choosing the more attractive or formidable target. Applying Bayesian inference, our findings reveal a systematic impact of the degree of difference on both the selection probability and speed. As differences in attractiveness or formidability increased, both men and women exhibited a heightened propensity and speed in selecting the higher-scoring stimuli. Our study demonstrates that people discriminate even slight differences in attractiveness and formidability, indicating that cognitive processes underlying the perception of these characteristics had undergone natural selection for a high level of discrimination.
面部和身体特征是一种显著的视觉刺激,人们会自发地将吸引力或可塑性等各种与适配性相关的特征归因于这些刺激。现有证据主要依赖于连续刺激呈现任务,而现实世界中的社会比较通常涉及对两个或多个个体进行评估。在使用双项强制选择任务的研究中,参与者的表现通常高于选择预期选项的几率。然而,这些任务使用的是二分法和人为操纵的刺激物,在个体间差异不太可能 "一目了然 "的情况下缺乏普遍性。我们测试了选择概率是否会随着刺激物之间差异程度的增加而成正比地增加,或者如果刺激物被认为过于相似,是否会存在一个分辨阈值。在由在线参与者(446 人)和现场参与者(56 人)组成的两项注册研究中,我们探讨了刺激对之间吸引力和可形成性评级的差异程度对选择概率和选择速度的影响。我们向受试者随机展示了一对男性刺激物(30 对脸部刺激物和 30 对身体刺激物),并要求受试者选择更具吸引力或更可怕的目标。通过贝叶斯推理,我们的研究结果表明,差异程度对选择概率和速度都有系统性的影响。随着吸引力或可塑造性差异的增加,男性和女性在选择得分较高的刺激物时都表现出更高的倾向和速度。我们的研究表明,即使吸引力和可塑性存在微小差异,人们也能分辨出来,这说明人们对这些特征的认知过程是经过自然选择的,具有较高的分辨能力。
{"title":"Even small differences in attractiveness and formidability affect the probability and speed of selection: An online study and an offline replication","authors":"Vít Třebický ,&nbsp;Petr Tureček ,&nbsp;Jitka Třebická Fialová ,&nbsp;Žaneta Pátková ,&nbsp;Dominika Grygarová ,&nbsp;Jan Havlíček","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106634","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106634","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Facial and bodily features represent salient visual stimuli upon which people spontaneously attribute various fitness-relevant characteristics such as attractiveness or formidability. While existing evidence predominantly relies on sequential stimuli presentation tasks, real-world social comparisons often involve assessing two or multiple individuals. In studies using two-alternative forced-choice tasks, participants usually perform at rates above the chance to select the expected option. However, these tasks use dichotomized and artificially manipulated stimuli that lack generalizability in situations where the differences between individuals are less likely to be ‘clear-cut’. We tested whether the probability of selection will proportionally increase with increasing degrees of difference between the stimuli or whether there is a discrimination threshold if the stimuli are perceived as too similar. In two registered studies comprising online (<em>N</em> = 446) and onsite (<em>N</em> = 56) participants, we explored the influence of the degree of difference in attractiveness and formidability ratings between stimuli pairs on both the probability of selection and selection speed. Participants were presented with randomly selected pairs of men (30 pairs of faces, 30 pairs of bodies) and tasked with choosing the more attractive or formidable target. Applying Bayesian inference, our findings reveal a systematic impact of the degree of difference on both the selection probability and speed. As differences in attractiveness or formidability increased, both men and women exhibited a heightened propensity and speed in selecting the higher-scoring stimuli. Our study demonstrates that people discriminate even slight differences in attractiveness and formidability, indicating that cognitive processes underlying the perception of these characteristics had undergone natural selection for a high level of discrimination.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106634"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142531747","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Deep neural networks generate facial metrics that overcome limitations of previous methods and predict in-person attraction 深度神经网络生成的面部指标克服了以往方法的局限性,并能预测人与人之间的吸引力
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106632
Amy A.Z. Zhao, Brendan P. Zietsch
Here we introduce deep neural networks (a form of artificial intelligence) as a novel method for quantifying facial characteristics such as averageness, masculinity, and similarity. Previous methods have quantified facial characteristics using subjective ratings, or objective landmark methods which ignore much of the information we use to perceive faces (e.g. skin colour and contrast, hair, eye colour). We obtained facial images and in-person ratings of facial attractiveness and kindness from 682 speed-dating participants. We find that facial measures derived from neural networks similarly predict in-person ratings compared to facial measures derived from both manual and automatic landmarks. Using neural network-derived measures, we find robust evidence for the attractiveness of masculinity in males, as well as some evidence for assortative preferences for masculinity. Past findings were supported regarding facial similarity as a cue of prosociality. Correlations between neural network and landmark measures were significant but small, and we found that neural network measures captured information beyond face shape. Neural network measures of masculinity had little to no correlation with facial pitch (head tilt) on measures of masculinity, overcoming a major limitation of landmark measures, which were substantially correlated with facial pitch.
在这里,我们将深度神经网络(人工智能的一种形式)作为量化面部特征(如平均值、男性气质和相似度)的一种新方法。以前的方法都是通过主观评分或客观地标方法来量化面部特征,这些方法忽略了我们用来感知面部的许多信息(如肤色和对比度、头发、眼睛颜色等)。我们从 682 名快速约会参与者那里获得了面部图像以及对面部吸引力和亲切程度的亲身评价。我们发现,与通过手动和自动地标得出的面部测量结果相比,通过神经网络得出的面部测量结果同样可以预测当面评分。利用神经网络得出的测量结果,我们发现了男性阳刚之气具有吸引力的有力证据,以及男性阳刚之气具有同类偏好的一些证据。过去关于面部相似性作为亲社会性线索的研究结果也得到了支持。神经网络和地标测量之间的相关性显著但较小,我们发现神经网络测量捕捉到了脸型以外的信息。在测量男性气质时,神经网络测量与面部俯仰(头部倾斜)几乎没有相关性,这克服了地标测量的一个主要局限性,地标测量与面部俯仰有很大的相关性。
{"title":"Deep neural networks generate facial metrics that overcome limitations of previous methods and predict in-person attraction","authors":"Amy A.Z. Zhao,&nbsp;Brendan P. Zietsch","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106632","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106632","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Here we introduce deep neural networks (a form of artificial intelligence) as a novel method for quantifying facial characteristics such as averageness, masculinity, and similarity. Previous methods have quantified facial characteristics using subjective ratings, or objective landmark methods which ignore much of the information we use to perceive faces (e.g. skin colour and contrast, hair, eye colour). We obtained facial images and in-person ratings of facial attractiveness and kindness from 682 speed-dating participants. We find that facial measures derived from neural networks similarly predict in-person ratings compared to facial measures derived from both manual and automatic landmarks. Using neural network-derived measures, we find robust evidence for the attractiveness of masculinity in males, as well as some evidence for assortative preferences for masculinity. Past findings were supported regarding facial similarity as a cue of prosociality. Correlations between neural network and landmark measures were significant but small, and we found that neural network measures captured information beyond face shape. Neural network measures of masculinity had little to no correlation with facial pitch (head tilt) on measures of masculinity, overcoming a major limitation of landmark measures, which were substantially correlated with facial pitch.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106632"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142531746","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex differences in close friendships and social style 亲密友谊和社交风格的性别差异
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106631
R.I.M. Dunbar , Eiluned Pearce , Rafael Wlodarski , Anna Machin
Friendships play a central role in human sociality, and are a major influence, both directly and indirectly, on our fitness. The two most important forms of friendship are the support clique and the best friend. Although the basis on which we choose friends and romantic partners have been studied in considerable detail, we know a great deal less about how individuals' own psychological traits affect whom they form relationships with. Here, we use an ethnically homogenous UK sample of 757 adults (aged 18–75 years; 56 % female) attending national science festivals to show that there are striking differences between men and women in both the structure of friendship groups and the psychological mechanisms that underpin their capacity to hold and maintain close friendships. Individual differences in the size and structure of women's cliques, and their likelihood of having a best friend, are underpinned mainly by prosocial tendencies, whereas in men they correlate negatively with anti-social, rather than prosocial, tendencies. These findings add to the evidence that male and female social worlds are organised in very different ways. This begs the evolutionary question as to why this is so.
友谊在人类社会中起着核心作用,对我们的健康有着直接或间接的重要影响。最重要的两种友谊形式是支持小团体和最好的朋友。尽管我们已经对选择朋友和恋爱伙伴的基础进行了相当详细的研究,但对于个人自身的心理特征如何影响他们与谁建立关系,我们却知之甚少。在这里,我们使用一个英国同种族的样本,对参加全国科学节的 757 名成年人(年龄在 18-75 岁之间;56% 为女性)进行了研究,结果表明,男性和女性在友谊群体的结构以及支持他们建立和维持亲密友谊的心理机制方面都存在显著差异。女性小团体的规模和结构的个体差异,以及她们拥有最好朋友的可能性,主要由亲社会倾向支撑,而男性则与反社会倾向而非亲社会倾向呈负相关。这些发现进一步证明,男性和女性的社会世界是以非常不同的方式组织起来的。这就引出了一个进化问题:为什么会这样?
{"title":"Sex differences in close friendships and social style","authors":"R.I.M. Dunbar ,&nbsp;Eiluned Pearce ,&nbsp;Rafael Wlodarski ,&nbsp;Anna Machin","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106631","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106631","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Friendships play a central role in human sociality, and are a major influence, both directly and indirectly, on our fitness. The two most important forms of friendship are the support clique and the best friend. Although the basis on which we choose friends and romantic partners have been studied in considerable detail, we know a great deal less about how individuals' own psychological traits affect whom they form relationships with. Here, we use an ethnically homogenous UK sample of 757 adults (aged 18–75 years; 56 % female) attending national science festivals to show that there are striking differences between men and women in both the structure of friendship groups and the psychological mechanisms that underpin their capacity to hold and maintain close friendships. Individual differences in the size and structure of women's cliques, and their likelihood of having a best friend, are underpinned mainly by prosocial tendencies, whereas in men they correlate negatively with anti-social, rather than prosocial, tendencies. These findings add to the evidence that male and female social worlds are organised in very different ways. This begs the evolutionary question as to why this is so.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106631"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142444842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Men (but not women) prefer to live in economically equal societies when it comes to mating: A five-study investigation 在交配方面,男性(而非女性)更喜欢生活在经济平等的社会中:五项研究调查
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106633
Xijing Wang , Hao Chen , Khandis R. Blake
It is generally believed that people prefer societies with economic equality. No studies thus far have systematically examined sex differences in this preference specifically concerning mating—an important life stage. Building upon theoretical frameworks of mating strategies (i.e., hypergyny), we hypothesized that men, in comparison to women, are less inclined to prefer and reside in a highly unequal society when it comes to mating. This could be because economically unequal environments lead men to expect poorer life quality after marriage than women. These hypotheses were confirmed across five studies using a mixed-method approach. In particular, we first provided evidence by focusing on fertile age populations and employing the panel data across 50 states of the USA from 2006 to 2019 (Study 1A), the most recent cross-sectional data at the county level of the USA (Study 1B), and a large-scale survey data on the individual migration records of American residents (N = 4,746,718, Study 2). In addition, we conducted two controlled experiments by manipulating mating motivation (Study 3) and economic inequality level (high versus low, Studies 3 and 4, N = 812, N = 418). Our studies, employing both archival data high in ecological validity and experimental evidence allowing causal inferences, show that men exhibit a stronger aversion than women toward economic inequality. Our findings contribute to a deeper understanding of how evolutionary mating strategies and sex differences jointly influence the economic inequality preference.
人们普遍认为,人们更喜欢经济平等的社会。迄今为止,还没有研究系统地考察过这种偏好的性别差异,特别是在交配--一个重要的人生阶段--方面。基于交配策略的理论框架(即hypergyny),我们假设,在交配方面,男性比女性更不倾向于选择和居住在高度不平等的社会中。这可能是因为经济不平等的环境导致男性对婚后生活质量的预期低于女性。这些假设通过采用混合方法的五项研究得到了证实。具体而言,我们首先以育龄人口为研究对象,利用 2006 年至 2019 年美国 50 个州的面板数据(研究 1A)、美国县一级的最新横截面数据(研究 1B)以及美国居民个人移民记录的大规模调查数据(N = 4,746,718, 研究 2)提供了证据。此外,我们还通过操纵交配动机(研究 3)和经济不平等程度(高与低,研究 3 和研究 4,N = 812,N = 418)进行了两次对照实验。我们的研究既采用了生态有效性较高的档案数据,也采用了可进行因果推论的实验证据,结果表明男性比女性对经济不平等表现出更强烈的厌恶。我们的研究结果有助于加深对进化交配策略和性别差异如何共同影响经济不平等偏好的理解。
{"title":"Men (but not women) prefer to live in economically equal societies when it comes to mating: A five-study investigation","authors":"Xijing Wang ,&nbsp;Hao Chen ,&nbsp;Khandis R. Blake","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106633","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106633","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>It is generally believed that people prefer societies with economic equality. No studies thus far have systematically examined sex differences in this preference specifically concerning mating—an important life stage. Building upon theoretical frameworks of mating strategies (i.e., hypergyny), we hypothesized that men, in comparison to women, are less inclined to prefer and reside in a highly unequal society when it comes to mating. This could be because economically unequal environments lead men to expect poorer life quality after marriage than women. These hypotheses were confirmed across five studies using a mixed-method approach. In particular, we first provided evidence by focusing on fertile age populations and employing the panel data across 50 states of the USA from 2006 to 2019 (Study 1A), the most recent cross-sectional data at the county level of the USA (Study 1B), and a large-scale survey data on the individual migration records of American residents (<em>N</em> = 4,746,718, Study 2). In addition, we conducted two controlled experiments by manipulating mating motivation (Study 3) and economic inequality level (high versus low, Studies 3 and 4, <em>N</em> = 812, <em>N</em> = 418). Our studies, employing both archival data high in ecological validity and experimental evidence allowing causal inferences, show that men exhibit a stronger aversion than women toward economic inequality. Our findings contribute to a deeper understanding of how evolutionary mating strategies and sex differences jointly influence the economic inequality preference.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106633"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142419816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of facial trustworthiness and dominance modulate early neural responses to male facial sexual dimorphism 对面部可信度和主导地位的感知会调节对男性面部性别二形性的早期神经反应
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106629
Barnaby J.W. Dixson , Nicole L. Nelson , Eleanor Moses , Anthony J. Lee , Alan J. Pegna
Sexual selection may have shaped the evolution of cognitive mechanisms to assess dominance and trustworthiness among anonymous conspecifics. We tested the hypothesis that masculine facial morphology and beardedness modulate early P100, N170, P200 and N250 event related potentials (ERP) components using electroencephalography (EEG) during judgments of male facial dominance and trustworthiness. We found that facial hair drove early P100 neural effects while facial masculinity drove an N170 effect during perceptions of dominance. For perceptions of trustworthiness, there was a significant N170 peak for bearded over clean-shaven faces while no significant effects were observed when judging facial masculinity. Clean-shaven faces exerted significant effects over bearded faces for P200 amplitudes for dominance and trustworthiness perceptions. The only significant N250 amplitudes occurred for beardedness over clean-shaven faces when judging trustworthiness. There were no effects of facial masculinity on any ERPs when faces were bearded, supporting previous research demonstrating that facial hair may mask sexually dimorphic structural facial traits. Masculine faces augmented judgments of dominance and trustworthiness over less masculine faces. Likewise, bearded faces enhanced dominance and trustworthiness judgments over clean-shaven faces. Our findings suggest facial masculinity activates neural responses involved in face processing when judging assertiveness and status seeking involved in same-sex competition, but not socially affiliative attributes prioritised in more communal behaviours. In contrast, facial hair acts as a low-level visual feature that rapidly communicated dominance and latterly communicated trustworthiness, suggesting a role of competence for facial hair when assessing male sociosexual attributes.
性选择可能决定了评估匿名同类间支配地位和可信度的认知机制的进化。我们利用脑电图(EEG)对男性面部支配力和可信度判断过程中男性面部形态和胡须调节早期 P100、N170、P200 和 N250 事件相关电位(ERP)成分的假设进行了检验。我们发现,面部毛发会驱动早期的 P100 神经效应,而面部的阳刚之气则会在支配感知中驱动 N170 效应。在对可信度的感知中,留胡子的面孔比胡子刮得干净的面孔有明显的 N170 峰值,而在判断面部男性气质时则没有观察到明显的效应。在支配力和可信度感知的 P200 波幅上,胡子拉碴的面孔比胡子拉碴的面孔有显著影响。在判断可信度时,胡须面孔的 N250 振幅比胡子刮得干净的面孔的 N250 振幅大,而胡子刮得干净的面孔的 N250 振幅比胡子刮得干净的面孔小。当面部留有胡须时,面部的男性气质对任何 ERPs 都没有影响,这支持了之前的研究,即面部毛发可能会掩盖面部结构上的性别二态性特征。相对于男性气质较弱的面孔,男性气质较强的面孔会增强对支配力和可信度的判断。同样,留胡子的面孔比胡子刮得干净的面孔更能增强支配力和可信度判断。我们的研究结果表明,在判断同性竞争中的自信和地位追求时,面部的男性气质会激活面部加工的神经反应,但在更多的集体行为中,面部的社会从属属性并不会被优先考虑。与此相反,面部毛发作为一种低级视觉特征,能迅速传达支配性,并在随后传达可信性,这表明在评估男性社会性属性时,面部毛发起着胜任的作用。
{"title":"Perceptions of facial trustworthiness and dominance modulate early neural responses to male facial sexual dimorphism","authors":"Barnaby J.W. Dixson ,&nbsp;Nicole L. Nelson ,&nbsp;Eleanor Moses ,&nbsp;Anthony J. Lee ,&nbsp;Alan J. Pegna","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106629","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106629","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Sexual selection may have shaped the evolution of cognitive mechanisms to assess dominance and trustworthiness among anonymous conspecifics. We tested the hypothesis that masculine facial morphology and beardedness modulate early P100, N170, P200 and N250 event related potentials (ERP) components using electroencephalography (EEG) during judgments of male facial dominance and trustworthiness. We found that facial hair drove early P100 neural effects while facial masculinity drove an N170 effect during perceptions of dominance. For perceptions of trustworthiness, there was a significant N170 peak for bearded over clean-shaven faces while no significant effects were observed when judging facial masculinity. Clean-shaven faces exerted significant effects over bearded faces for P200 amplitudes for dominance and trustworthiness perceptions. The only significant N250 amplitudes occurred for beardedness over clean-shaven faces when judging trustworthiness. There were no effects of facial masculinity on any ERPs when faces were bearded, supporting previous research demonstrating that facial hair may mask sexually dimorphic structural facial traits. Masculine faces augmented judgments of dominance and trustworthiness over less masculine faces. Likewise, bearded faces enhanced dominance and trustworthiness judgments over clean-shaven faces. Our findings suggest facial masculinity activates neural responses involved in face processing when judging assertiveness and status seeking involved in same-sex competition, but not socially affiliative attributes prioritised in more communal behaviours. In contrast, facial hair acts as a low-level visual feature that rapidly communicated dominance and latterly communicated trustworthiness, suggesting a role of competence for facial hair when assessing male sociosexual attributes.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106629"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142419815","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can race be replaced? Ecology and race categorization 种族可以被取代吗?生态学与种族分类
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106630
Oliver Sng , Krystina A. Boyd-Frenkel , Keelah E.G. Williams

Why do people categorize others by race? Building on recent work integrating affordance management with a life history perspective, we propose that one reason perceivers categorize others by race is because race is a cue to the environments/ecologies in which groups live. In the U.S., because Black and White individuals differentially live in environments that vary in ecological harshness/unpredictability, race may be used as a cue of a person's home ecology. In three experiments (undergraduate and online U.S. samples; N = 1260) with the memory confusion paradigm (“who-said-what”), when American perceivers are presented with information on both a person's race (i.e., Black/White) and the ecologies in which they live (i.e., harsh/hopeful), racial categorization decreases, and ecology categorization emerges (Studies 1–3). Hence, in the minds of perceivers, the ecologies that others come from “replaces” others' race. However, counter to expectations, instructing perceivers to form social impressions of others on traits linked to ecological harshness (i.e., “planfulness”) led to ecology categorization disappearing (Study 3). We discuss implications of our findings for race perception and for social perception at large.

为什么人们会根据种族对他人进行分类?在最近将承受力管理与生活史视角相结合的研究基础上,我们提出,感知者根据种族对他人进行分类的原因之一是种族是群体生活环境/生态的线索。在美国,由于黑人和白人的生活环境不同,生态环境的恶劣程度/不可预测性也不同,因此种族可能被用作一个人家庭生态环境的线索。在使用记忆混淆范式("谁说了什么")进行的三项实验(本科生和在线美国样本;N = 1260)中,当向美国感知者展示一个人的种族(即黑人/白人)和生活生态(即恶劣/充满希望)信息时,种族分类减少,生态分类出现(研究 1-3)。因此,在感知者的心目中,他人来自的生态环境 "取代 "了他人的种族。然而,与预期相反的是,指示感知者根据与生态严酷性相关的特征(即 "有计划性")来形成对他人的社会印象会导致生态分类消失(研究 3)。我们将讨论我们的发现对种族感知和整个社会感知的影响。
{"title":"Can race be replaced? Ecology and race categorization","authors":"Oliver Sng ,&nbsp;Krystina A. Boyd-Frenkel ,&nbsp;Keelah E.G. Williams","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106630","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106630","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Why do people categorize others by race? Building on recent work integrating affordance management with a life history perspective, we propose that one reason perceivers categorize others by race is because race is a cue to the environments/ecologies in which groups live. In the U.S., because Black and White individuals differentially live in environments that vary in ecological harshness/unpredictability, race may be used as a cue of a person's home ecology. In three experiments (undergraduate and online U.S. samples; <em>N</em> = 1260) with the memory confusion paradigm (“who-said-what”), when American perceivers are presented with information on both a person's race (i.e., Black/White) and the ecologies in which they live (i.e., harsh/hopeful), racial categorization decreases, and ecology categorization emerges (Studies 1–3). Hence, in the minds of perceivers, the ecologies that others come from “replaces” others' race. However, counter to expectations, instructing perceivers to form social impressions of others on traits linked to ecological harshness (i.e., “planfulness”) led to ecology categorization disappearing (Study 3). We discuss implications of our findings for race perception and for social perception at large.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106630"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241837","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The evolution of between-sex bonds in primates 灵长类动物性间联系的进化
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106628
Elisabeth H.M. Sterck , Catherine Crockford , Julia Fischer , Jorg J.M. Massen , Barbara Tiddi , Susan Perry , Cédric Sueur , Oliver Schülke , Julia Ostner

Social bonds can be a way for individuals to gain access to crucial resources and services that cannot be taken by force and are therefore subject to leverage. Bonds between the sexes can provide access to services that are specific to the other sex. Females exert leverage over males in terms of mating access, males have leverage over females in terms of the service protection, and both sexes exert leverage over the other sex in terms of tolerance and agonistic support. While mating access can be coerced in some circumstances, most services cannot be forced. Here, we use theoretical considerations to explore when sources of leverage over the opposite sex lead to between-sex bonds. Focussing on primates living in multi-male multi-female groups, we predict that leverage over the other sex will be higher, when 1) the receiver benefits on average more than the provider, 2) receivers cannot share the resource, and 3) the resource is rare and valuable. If these conditions are fulfilled, and given the mutual nature of a social bond, we expect bonds to be found, 4) when long-term targeting of the same partner yields benefits. We argue that a female's main source of leverage is mating access, whereas males mainly exert leverage over females in terms of protection of females and offspring. The combination of female mate choice with male protection and care for young is expected to promote between-sex bonds; reduced female cohesion and/or secondary female dispersal are expected to further increase the strength of between-sex bonds. The investment in shared offspring results in interdependency between male and female strategies, but the different services provided by females and males indicate that affiliative exchanges associated with bonds between the sexes will be typically asymmetric and vary over time. Thus, bonds between the sexes are expected to form in a limited number of circumstances where both sexes have leverage over the other sex in terms of their respective sex-specific services. While a systematic test of this proposal is hampered by the dearth of data on species lacking social bonds between the sexes, the data currently available are consistent with our hypothesis.

社会纽带是个人获得重要资源和服务的一种方式,而这些资源和服务是无法通过武力夺取的,因此需要借助杠杆作用。两性之间的纽带可以提供获得另一性别特有服务的机会。在交配机会方面,雌性对雄性施加杠杆作用;在服务保护方面,雄性对雌性施加杠杆作用;在宽容和对抗性支持方面,两性对另一性别施加杠杆作用。虽然交配机会在某些情况下是可以强迫的,但大多数服务是不能强迫的。在这里,我们从理论上探讨了对异性的影响力何时会导致两性之间的联系。以生活在多雄性多雌性群体中的灵长类动物为研究对象,我们预测,当出现以下情况时,对异性的影响力会更大 1) 平均而言,接受者比提供者获益更多,2) 接受者无法分享资源,3) 资源稀有且珍贵。如果满足了这些条件,并且考虑到社会纽带的相互性,我们预计纽带会在以下情况下出现:4)长期针对同一伴侣产生收益。我们认为,雌性杠杆作用的主要来源是交配机会,而雄性对雌性的杠杆作用主要体现在对雌性和后代的保护上。雌性的配偶选择与雄性的保护和对幼崽的照顾相结合,有望促进两性之间的联系;雌性凝聚力的降低和/或雌性的二次分散有望进一步增加两性之间联系的强度。对共同后代的投资导致了雄性和雌性策略之间的相互依赖,但雌性和雄性提供的不同服务表明,与性别间联系相关的附属交换通常是不对称的,并随时间而变化。因此,在少数情况下,两性之间的联系会形成,在这些情况下,两性在各自的特定性别服务方面都比另一性别具有优势。由于缺乏有关缺乏两性之间社会纽带的物种的数据,对这一建议的系统性检验受到了阻碍,但目前可用的数据与我们的假设是一致的。
{"title":"The evolution of between-sex bonds in primates","authors":"Elisabeth H.M. Sterck ,&nbsp;Catherine Crockford ,&nbsp;Julia Fischer ,&nbsp;Jorg J.M. Massen ,&nbsp;Barbara Tiddi ,&nbsp;Susan Perry ,&nbsp;Cédric Sueur ,&nbsp;Oliver Schülke ,&nbsp;Julia Ostner","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106628","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106628","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Social bonds can be a way for individuals to gain access to crucial resources and services that cannot be taken by force and are therefore subject to leverage. Bonds between the sexes can provide access to services that are specific to the other sex. Females exert leverage over males in terms of mating access, males have leverage over females in terms of the service protection, and both sexes exert leverage over the other sex in terms of tolerance and agonistic support. While mating access can be coerced in some circumstances, most services cannot be forced. Here, we use theoretical considerations to explore when sources of leverage over the opposite sex lead to between-sex bonds. Focussing on primates living in multi-male multi-female groups, we predict that leverage over the other sex will be higher, when 1) the receiver benefits on average more than the provider, 2) receivers cannot share the resource, and 3) the resource is rare and valuable. If these conditions are fulfilled, and given the mutual nature of a social bond, we expect bonds to be found, 4) when long-term targeting of the same partner yields benefits. We argue that a female's main source of leverage is mating access, whereas males mainly exert leverage over females in terms of protection of females and offspring. The combination of female mate choice with male protection and care for young is expected to promote between-sex bonds; reduced female cohesion and/or secondary female dispersal are expected to further increase the strength of between-sex bonds. The investment in shared offspring results in interdependency between male and female strategies, but the different services provided by females and males indicate that affiliative exchanges associated with bonds between the sexes will be typically asymmetric and vary over time. Thus, bonds between the sexes are expected to form in a limited number of circumstances where both sexes have leverage over the other sex in terms of their respective sex-specific services. While a systematic test of this proposal is hampered by the dearth of data on species lacking social bonds between the sexes, the data currently available are consistent with our hypothesis.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106628"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824001041/pdfft?md5=d27cea174fb7b445c10efc3ea4c0111f&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824001041-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142228639","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex and cross-cultural comparison of self-enhancement practices: Data from four distinct societies 自我提升做法的性别和跨文化比较:来自四个不同社会的数据
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106627
Marta Kowal , Piotr Sorokowski , Sayra M. Cardona , Andrea Castañeda , C.M. Nadeem Faisal

People worldwide invest substantial resources in improving their physical attractiveness. We employed a qualitative approach to investigate the ‘what’, ‘how intensely’, and ‘why’ behind this phenomenon. We conducted semi-structured in-depth interviews with 96 members of four distinct societies, including the Cook Islands, Guatemala, Pakistan, and Poland. The thematic analysis, guided by the four Tinbergen's questions, provided valuable insight on the beauty-enhancing practices across the studied societies. Commonly shared practices included maintaining hygiene, using make-up, cosmetics, accessories, perfumes, exercising, and caring for clothing and hair. Additionally, interviewees from different countries placed particular importance on some distinctive characteristics, such as flowers in the Cook Islands, fashionable clothing in Guatemala, beards and golden jewelry in Pakistan, and body shape in Poland. Conforming with evolutionary theories, all interviewees unanimously agreed that women devote more time than men to the pursuit of beauty, albeit with variations across the four societies. Furthermore, aligning with basic principles of mate choice, the present research delineated fundamental motives driving appearance enhancement, that is, inter-sexual and intra-sexual competition. We also shed more light on other motives, seldomly investigated in the past, including, for instance, religious reasoning. By juxtaposing perspectives from culturally and geographically diverse societies, we offer a nuanced understanding of the multifaceted nature of self-enhancement practices.

全世界的人们都在为提高自己的身体吸引力而投入大量资源。我们采用了一种定性方法来研究这一现象背后的 "是什么"、"有多强烈 "以及 "为什么"。我们对库克群岛、危地马拉、巴基斯坦和波兰等四个不同社会的 96 名成员进行了半结构化深度访谈。以丁伯根的四个问题为指导进行的专题分析,为我们了解所研究社会中的美容习俗提供了宝贵的资料。共同的做法包括保持卫生、使用化妆品、饰品、香水、锻炼以及保养衣物和头发。此外,不同国家的受访者都特别重视一些与众不同的特征,如库克群岛的鲜花、危地马拉的时尚服装、巴基斯坦的胡须和金饰以及波兰的体型。根据进化理论,所有受访者一致认为,女性比男性花更多的时间追求美,尽管在四个社会中存在差异。此外,根据择偶的基本原则,本研究还界定了促使外表提升的基本动机,即两性之间和两性内部的竞争。我们还进一步揭示了过去很少研究的其他动机,例如宗教原因。通过并列不同文化和地域社会的观点,我们对自我提升行为的多面性有了细致入微的了解。
{"title":"Sex and cross-cultural comparison of self-enhancement practices: Data from four distinct societies","authors":"Marta Kowal ,&nbsp;Piotr Sorokowski ,&nbsp;Sayra M. Cardona ,&nbsp;Andrea Castañeda ,&nbsp;C.M. Nadeem Faisal","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106627","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106627","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People worldwide invest substantial resources in improving their physical attractiveness. We employed a qualitative approach to investigate the ‘what’, ‘how intensely’, and ‘why’ behind this phenomenon. We conducted semi-structured in-depth interviews with 96 members of four distinct societies, including the Cook Islands, Guatemala, Pakistan, and Poland. The thematic analysis, guided by the four Tinbergen's questions, provided valuable insight on the beauty-enhancing practices across the studied societies. Commonly shared practices included maintaining hygiene, using make-up, cosmetics, accessories, perfumes, exercising, and caring for clothing and hair. Additionally, interviewees from different countries placed particular importance on some distinctive characteristics, such as flowers in the Cook Islands, fashionable clothing in Guatemala, beards and golden jewelry in Pakistan, and body shape in Poland. Conforming with evolutionary theories, all interviewees unanimously agreed that women devote more time than men to the pursuit of beauty, albeit with variations across the four societies. Furthermore, aligning with basic principles of mate choice, the present research delineated fundamental motives driving appearance enhancement, that is, inter-sexual and intra-sexual competition. We also shed more light on other motives, seldomly investigated in the past, including, for instance, religious reasoning. By juxtaposing perspectives from culturally and geographically diverse societies, we offer a nuanced understanding of the multifaceted nature of self-enhancement practices.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106627"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142158073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disqualifiers or preferences? How humans incorporate dealbreakers into mate choice 取消资格还是偏好?人类如何在择偶时考虑到破坏因素
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106617
Kathryn V. Walter , Jared Kliszewski , Krystal Duarte , Daniel Conroy-Beam

Interest in dealbreakers has blossomed over the last decade, but we do not know how dealbreakers are used in mate choice. Here, we propose that some dealbreaker traits may act as disqualifiers, traits that we use to recognize others as potential mates. Alternatively, other dealbreaker traits may act like mate preferences and influence how attractive we find a potential mate relative to other potential mates. We use agent-based modeling and two samples of real-life couples, n = 1044, and n = 518, collected through Qualtrics' panel service, to test between these two possibilities. We find evidence that many of the traits colloquially considered to be dealbreakers, such as smoking status, height, and religion, are not used by the mind as disqualifiers, rather they act like preferences and are integrated into overall assessments of mate value. However, we find evidence that a person's sex is incorporated into mate choice like a disqualifier. Overall, considering and testing the evolved function of dealbreakers in the long-term mating process illuminates new directions for future research.

在过去十年中,人们对 "破坏者 "的兴趣日益浓厚,但我们并不知道 "破坏者 "是如何被用于择偶的。在这里,我们提出,一些破坏者的特征可能是我们用来识别他人潜在配偶的不合格特征。另一种情况是,其他的破坏性特征可能会像配偶偏好一样,影响我们认为潜在配偶相对于其他潜在配偶的吸引力。我们使用基于代理的建模和通过 Qualtrics 小组服务收集的两个真实情侣样本(n = 1044 和 n = 518)来检验这两种可能性。我们发现有证据表明,许多被俗称为破坏交易的特征,如吸烟状况、身高和宗教信仰等,并没有被大脑用作取消资格的条件,相反,它们就像偏好一样,被纳入了对配偶价值的整体评估中。然而,我们发现有证据表明,一个人的性别在择偶时就像一个剥夺资格的因素。总之,考虑和测试破坏因素在长期交配过程中的进化功能为未来的研究指明了新的方向。
{"title":"Disqualifiers or preferences? How humans incorporate dealbreakers into mate choice","authors":"Kathryn V. Walter ,&nbsp;Jared Kliszewski ,&nbsp;Krystal Duarte ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106617","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106617","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Interest in dealbreakers has blossomed over the last decade, but we do not know how dealbreakers are used in mate choice. Here, we propose that some dealbreaker traits may act as disqualifiers, traits that we use to recognize others as potential mates. Alternatively, other dealbreaker traits may act like mate preferences and influence how attractive we find a potential mate relative to other potential mates. We use agent-based modeling and two samples of real-life couples, <em>n</em> = 1044, and <em>n</em> = 518, collected through Qualtrics' panel service, to test between these two possibilities. We find evidence that many of the traits colloquially considered to be dealbreakers, such as smoking status, height, and religion, are not used by the mind as disqualifiers, rather they act like preferences and are integrated into overall assessments of mate value. However, we find evidence that a person's sex is incorporated into mate choice like a disqualifier. Overall, considering and testing the evolved function of dealbreakers in the long-term mating process illuminates new directions for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 6","pages":"Article 106617"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142149493","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1