首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
The role of costly commitment signals in assorting cooperators during intergroup conflict 在群体间冲突中,代价高昂的承诺信号在同化合作者中的作用
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.003
Martin Lang , Radim Chvaja , Benjamin G. Purzycki

A reliable assortment of committed individuals is crucial for success in intergroup conflict due to the danger of shirking. Theory predicts that reliable communication of commitment is afforded by costly signals that track cooperative intent. Across four pre-registered studies (total N = 1440, general US population), we used the public goods game where groups competed for resources to investigate whether and how costly signals function to assort cooperators. We found that costly signals assorted more cooperative participants, creating groups that would win most of the between-group clashes. The same effects were not observed when participants were assigned to signal, implying that signaling tracks but does not create cooperative intent. However, contrary to costly signaling theory, we found that low cost signals were more effective in cooperator assortment compared to high cost signals and suggest that future studies need to focus on signaler perception of cost/benefit trade-off of signaling.

由于推卸责任的危险,一批可靠的有承诺的人对于在群体间冲突中取得成功至关重要。根据理论预测,可靠的承诺交流可以通过追踪合作意向的高成本信号来实现。在四项预先登记的研究中(总人数 = 1440,美国普通人群),我们利用公共物品游戏(群体间竞争资源)来研究代价高昂的信号是否以及如何起到分类合作者的作用。我们发现,代价高昂的信号会分化出更多的合作参与者,从而形成在大多数群体间冲突中获胜的群体。当参与者被分配到信号组时,我们没有观察到同样的效果,这意味着信号组会跟踪但不会产生合作意向。然而,与高成本信号理论相反,我们发现与高成本信号相比,低成本信号在合作者分群中更为有效,并建议未来的研究需要关注信号发出者对信号发出的成本/收益权衡的看法。
{"title":"The role of costly commitment signals in assorting cooperators during intergroup conflict","authors":"Martin Lang ,&nbsp;Radim Chvaja ,&nbsp;Benjamin G. Purzycki","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.003","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>A reliable assortment of committed individuals is crucial for success in intergroup conflict due to the danger of shirking. Theory predicts that reliable communication of commitment is afforded by costly signals that track cooperative intent. Across four pre-registered studies (total </span><em>N</em> = 1440, general US population), we used the public goods game where groups competed for resources to investigate whether and how costly signals function to assort cooperators. We found that costly signals assorted more cooperative participants, creating groups that would win most of the between-group clashes. The same effects were not observed when participants were assigned to signal, implying that signaling tracks but does not create cooperative intent. However, contrary to costly signaling theory, we found that low cost signals were more effective in cooperator assortment compared to high cost signals and suggest that future studies need to focus on signaler perception of cost/benefit trade-off of signaling.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 131-143"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139586982","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing disgust sensitivity in women in early pregnancy and non-pregnant women in the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle 比较处于月经周期卵泡期和黄体期的早期妊娠妇女和非妊娠妇女的厌恶敏感性
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.006
Daniela Dlouhá , Jana Ullmann , Lea Takács , Kamila Nouzová , Hana Hrbáčková , Jan Šeda , Šárka Kaňková

Considered a part of the behavioral immune system, disgust functions as a protective mechanism against potential pathogen threat. There is evidence that disgust sensitivity varies depending on immunological and hormonal changes, including those occurring during the menstrual cycle or pregnancy. Although some studies indicate that disgust is elevated in early pregnancy, no study has yet compared disgust sensitivity in pregnant and non-pregnant women. This study aimed to examine differences in disgust sensitivity in pregnant versus non-pregnant women, while investigating whether disgust sensitivity differs depending on the phase of the menstrual cycle in non-pregnant women. The sample included 172 women (aged 21–40) in the first trimester of pregnancy and 354 non-pregnant, naturally cycling women (aged 20–40), out of whom 218 (61.6%) were in the luteal phase. All women filled out the Disgust Scale-Revised and the Three Domains of Disgust Scale. Non-pregnant women also completed the Culpepper Disgust Image Set. We observed that pregnant women had significantly higher pathogen-related and sexual disgust sensitivity than their non-pregnant counterparts (in both the follicular and luteal cycle phases). In non-pregnant women, there was no difference in disgust sensitivity between women in the follicular and luteal phases. When comparing pregnant women, women in the follicular phase, and those in the luteal phase, pathogen-related disgust sensitivity was lowest in the follicular, then in the luteal phase, and the highest in early pregnancy, although the difference between women in the follicular and luteal phase was not significant. Our results provide further evidence for the hypothesis that disgust is elevated when there is a need for increased protection, such as in the critical period of organogenesis in the first trimester of pregnancy.

恶心被认为是行为免疫系统的一部分,是一种抵御潜在病原体威胁的保护机制。有证据表明,恶心敏感度会随着免疫和荷尔蒙的变化而变化,包括月经周期或怀孕期间发生的变化。尽管一些研究表明,恶心感在孕早期会升高,但还没有研究对孕妇和非孕妇的恶心敏感性进行比较。本研究旨在探讨孕妇与非孕妇在厌恶敏感度上的差异,同时探讨非孕妇的厌恶敏感度是否会因月经周期的不同阶段而有所差异。样本包括 172 名怀孕前三个月的妇女(21-40 岁)和 354 名未怀孕的自然周期妇女(20-40 岁),其中 218 人(61.6%)处于黄体期。所有妇女都填写了恶心量表(修订版)和恶心三域量表。非孕妇也填写了库尔佩珀厌恶形象集。我们发现,孕妇对病原体和性厌恶的敏感度明显高于非孕妇(在卵泡期和黄体期)。在非孕期妇女中,卵泡期和黄体期妇女的厌恶敏感度没有差异。在比较孕妇、卵泡期妇女和黄体期妇女时,与病原体相关的厌恶敏感度在卵泡期最低,然后是黄体期,在妊娠早期最高,但卵泡期妇女和黄体期妇女之间的差异并不显著。我们的研究结果进一步证明了这样一个假设:当需要加强保护时,如在妊娠头三个月器官形成的关键时期,厌恶感会升高。
{"title":"Comparing disgust sensitivity in women in early pregnancy and non-pregnant women in the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle","authors":"Daniela Dlouhá ,&nbsp;Jana Ullmann ,&nbsp;Lea Takács ,&nbsp;Kamila Nouzová ,&nbsp;Hana Hrbáčková ,&nbsp;Jan Šeda ,&nbsp;Šárka Kaňková","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.006","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.006","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Considered a part of the behavioral immune system<span>, disgust functions as a protective mechanism against potential pathogen threat. There is evidence that disgust sensitivity varies depending on immunological and hormonal changes, including those occurring during the menstrual cycle<span> or pregnancy. Although some studies indicate that disgust is elevated in early pregnancy, no study has yet compared disgust sensitivity in pregnant and non-pregnant women. This study aimed to examine differences in disgust sensitivity in pregnant versus non-pregnant women, while investigating whether disgust sensitivity differs depending on the phase of the menstrual cycle in non-pregnant women. The sample included 172 women (aged 21–40) in the first trimester of pregnancy and 354 non-pregnant, naturally cycling women (aged 20–40), out of whom 218 (61.6%) were in the luteal phase. All women filled out the Disgust Scale-Revised and the Three Domains of Disgust Scale. Non-pregnant women also completed the Culpepper Disgust Image Set. We observed that pregnant women<span> had significantly higher pathogen-related and sexual disgust sensitivity than their non-pregnant counterparts (in both the follicular and luteal cycle phases). In non-pregnant women, there was no difference in disgust sensitivity between women in the follicular and luteal phases. When comparing pregnant women, women in the follicular phase, and those in the luteal phase, pathogen-related disgust sensitivity was lowest in the follicular, then in the luteal phase, and the highest in early pregnancy, although the difference between women in the follicular and luteal phase was not significant. Our results provide further evidence for the hypothesis that disgust is elevated when there is a need for increased protection, such as in the critical period of organogenesis in the first trimester of pregnancy.</span></span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 164-174"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139882378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sibling aggression is surprisingly common and sexually egalitarian 兄妹间的侵犯行为出奇地普遍,而且在性方面是平等的
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.03.001
Amanda P. Kirsch , Douglas T. Kenrick , Ahra Ko , Cari M. Pick , Michael E.W. Varnum

Two well-supported generalizations from aggression research are that: a) people are less likely to commit homicide against close kin compared to non-kin, and b) females are less likely to engage in direct aggression than are males. Aggression between siblings, however, is somewhat more complicated than one might surmise from those two generalizations. Data from 3 studies collected using undergraduate and Prolific samples (N = 1640) reveal classic sex differences in direct aggression between non-relatives, but not between sisters and brothers. Whereas only a small minority of females have hit a friend or an acquaintance, the majority of females, like the majority of males, have hit a sibling. Although reputational aggression is substantially less likely between siblings than between friends or acquaintances, mild forms of direct aggression (such as hitting) are quite frequent between siblings. Discussion considers several possible limitations of the findings reported here and considers results in light of Trivers' theory of parent-offspring conflict.

从攻击研究中得到支持的两个概括是:a) 与非亲属相比,人们不太可能对近亲实施凶杀;b) 与男性相比,女性不太可能实施直接攻击。然而,兄弟姐妹之间的攻击行为比人们从这两个概括中推测出的情况要复杂得多。利用本科生和 Prolific 样本(= 1640)收集的 3 项研究数据显示,非亲属间的直接攻击存在典型的性别差异,但兄弟姐妹间却没有。只有少数女性打过朋友或熟人,而大多数女性和大多数男性一样,打过兄弟姐妹。虽然兄弟姐妹之间发生名誉攻击的可能性大大低于朋友或熟人之间,但兄弟姐妹之间却经常发生轻微的直接攻击行为(如打人)。讨论考虑了本文报告的研究结果可能存在的一些局限性,并根据特里弗斯的父母-后代冲突理论对结果进行了分析。
{"title":"Sibling aggression is surprisingly common and sexually egalitarian","authors":"Amanda P. Kirsch ,&nbsp;Douglas T. Kenrick ,&nbsp;Ahra Ko ,&nbsp;Cari M. Pick ,&nbsp;Michael E.W. Varnum","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.03.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.03.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Two well-supported generalizations from aggression research are that: a) people are less likely to commit homicide against close kin compared to non-kin, and b) females are less likely to engage in direct aggression than are males. Aggression between siblings, however, is somewhat more complicated than one might surmise from those two generalizations. Data from 3 studies collected using undergraduate and Prolific samples (<em>N</em> = 1640) reveal classic sex differences in direct aggression between non-relatives, but not between sisters and brothers. Whereas only a small minority of females have hit a friend or an acquaintance, the majority of females, like the majority of males, have hit a sibling. Although reputational aggression is substantially less likely between siblings than between friends or acquaintances, mild forms of direct aggression (such as hitting) are quite frequent between siblings. Discussion considers several possible limitations of the findings reported here and considers results in light of Trivers' theory of parent-offspring conflict.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 214-227"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140129797","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing to help others at a cost to oneself elevates preschoolers' body posture 选择以牺牲自己为代价来帮助他人会提升学龄前儿童的身体姿态
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.001
Sveinung Sundfør Sivertsen , Daniel Haun , Robert Hepach

Young children sometimes help others at a cost to themselves, but little is known about the emotional mechanisms underlying this behaviour. Here, 5-year-old children (n = 96, 45 girls, mean age = 5.57 years, SD = 1.79 months, range = 5.19 years to 5.9 years, families recruited from a local database based in a medium-sized German city) were engaged in one task and then asked either to help (child-helps) or watch (child-watches) an adult complete another task. Children would lose (cost) or not lose (no-cost) the progress they had made on their own task if they engaged with the adult. Children were more likely to interrupt their own task in the helping condition and were overall faster to do so when helping was not costly. Children who chose to incur a cost to help showed more positive emotions after helping—as measured via changes in their postural elevation—compared to helping at no cost. This pattern was not found in the child-watches condition. This suggests that costly helping holds emotional rewards for children in ways that non-costly helping does not.

幼儿有时会以牺牲自己为代价帮助他人,但人们对这种行为背后的情感机制知之甚少。在这里,5 岁的儿童(= 96,45 名女孩,平均年龄 = 5.57 岁,SD = 1.79 个月,范围 = 5.19 岁至 5.9 岁,从德国一个中等城市的本地数据库中招募的家庭)在完成一项任务后,会被要求帮助(儿童帮助)或观看(儿童观看)成人完成另一项任务。如果儿童与成人一起完成任务,他们将失去(付出代价)或不失去(无代价)在自己任务上取得的进展。在提供帮助的条件下,儿童更有可能中断自己的任务,而且在提供帮助不需要付出代价的情况下,儿童中断自己任务的速度更快。与无偿帮助相比,选择有偿帮助的儿童在帮助后会表现出更多的积极情绪,这可以通过他们的姿势升降变化来衡量。这种模式在儿童观察条件下没有发现。这表明,付出代价的帮助会给儿童带来情绪上的回报,而不付出代价的帮助则不会。
{"title":"Choosing to help others at a cost to oneself elevates preschoolers' body posture","authors":"Sveinung Sundfør Sivertsen ,&nbsp;Daniel Haun ,&nbsp;Robert Hepach","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Young children sometimes help others at a cost to themselves, but little is known about the emotional mechanisms underlying this behaviour. Here, 5-year-old children (<em>n</em> = 96, 45 girls, mean age = 5.57 years, SD = 1.79 months, range = 5.19 years to 5.9 years, families recruited from a local database based in a medium-sized German city) were engaged in one task and then asked either to help (child-helps) or watch (child-watches) an adult complete another task. Children would lose (cost) or not lose (no-cost) the progress they had made on their own task if they engaged with the adult. Children were more likely to interrupt their own task in the helping condition and were overall faster to do so when helping was not costly. Children who chose to incur a cost to help showed more positive emotions after helping—as measured via changes in their postural elevation—compared to helping at no cost. This pattern was not found in the child-watches condition. This suggests that costly helping holds emotional rewards for children in ways that non-costly helping does not.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 175-182"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000205/pdfft?md5=c329d6e77c1585731bc702b7103955c5&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000205-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140129868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dyads in networks: We (dis)like our partners' partners based on their anticipated indirect effects on us 网络中的二人组:我们(不)喜欢我们伙伴的伙伴,是基于他们对我们的预期间接影响
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.004
Laureon A. Merrie , Jaimie Arona Krems , Daniel Sznycer

Research on close relationships often focuses on the dyad (e.g., dyads of romantic partners) and on how dyad members affect each other's welfare. But dyads exist embedded in broader, densely-interconnected social networks, and less research attention has been paid to the myriad ways in which people outside the dyad impact one's welfare through their interactions with, or even their attitudes about, the other member of the dyad. What drives our feelings toward such extra-dyadic individuals? Balance Theory, an influential formalist theory in social psychology, suggests that our feelings are driven by the need for affective balance, achieved by, for example, liking strangers who share our feelings toward our existing partners or by disliking strangers who do not. We propose an alternative theory, the Embedded Dyad Framework, which foregrounds the substantive effects that strangers can have on our welfare through their interactions with our dyadic partners. Across four experiments (N = 1589) with U.S.-residing participants we predict and find, consistent with the Embedded Dyad Framework, that we like strangers who share our hatred for our rivals and our love for our friends (consistent with Balance Theory); but we dislike strangers who share our love for our spouses (contradicting Balance Theory). Further supporting predictions from an Embedded Dyad Framework, (a) greater perceived exclusivity in welfare-enhancing dyadic relationships (e.g., friendships) drives our lesser liking of strangers who share our love for our partners, and (b) greater perceived welfare suppression by our antagonistic partners (e.g., rivals) drives our liking of strangers who share our hatred of our antagonists. This framework outpredicts cognitive consistency views by emphasizing the real threats and opportunities that dyadic relationships afford people when dyads are embedded in social networks.

对亲密关系的研究通常集中于二人关系(如恋爱伴侣二人关系)以及二人关系成员如何影响彼此的福利。但是,二人关系存在于范围更广、联系更紧密的社会网络之中,而对于二人关系之外的人通过与二人关系成员的互动,甚至是他们对二人关系成员的态度,以何种方式影响二人关系成员的福利,研究关注较少。是什么驱使我们对这种关系外的人产生感情呢?平衡理论(Balance Theory)是社会心理学中一种颇具影响力的形式主义理论,它认为我们的情感是由情感平衡的需求驱动的,例如,通过喜欢那些与我们对现有伴侣的情感相同的陌生人或不喜欢那些与我们对现有伴侣的情感不同的陌生人来实现情感平衡。我们提出了另一种理论,即 "嵌入式关系框架"(Embedded Dyad Framework),它强调了陌生人通过与我们的关系伙伴的互动对我们的福利产生的实质性影响。通过对居住在美国的参与者进行的四次实验(= 1589 人),我们预测并发现,与 "嵌入式关系框架 "相一致的是,我们喜欢与我们同样憎恨对手、同样热爱朋友的陌生人(与 "平衡理论 "相一致);但我们不喜欢与我们同样热爱配偶的陌生人(与 "平衡理论 "相矛盾)。进一步支持嵌入式双向关系框架预测的是:(a) 在福利增强型双向关系(如友谊)中,感知到的排他性越强,我们就越不喜欢与我们分享对伴侣的爱的陌生人;(b) 感知到的对立伴侣(如对手)对福利的压制越强,我们就越喜欢与我们分享对对手的恨的陌生人。这一框架强调了当二人关系嵌入社会网络时,二人关系给人们带来的真正威胁和机会,从而超越了认知一致性观点。
{"title":"Dyads in networks: We (dis)like our partners' partners based on their anticipated indirect effects on us","authors":"Laureon A. Merrie ,&nbsp;Jaimie Arona Krems ,&nbsp;Daniel Sznycer","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research on close relationships often focuses on the dyad (e.g., dyads of romantic partners) and on how dyad members affect each other's welfare. But dyads exist embedded in broader, densely-interconnected social networks, and less research attention has been paid to the myriad ways in which people outside the dyad impact one's welfare through their interactions with, or even their attitudes about, <em>the other</em> member of the dyad. What drives our feelings toward such extra-dyadic individuals? Balance Theory, an influential formalist theory in social psychology, suggests that our feelings are driven by the need for affective balance, achieved by, for example, liking strangers who share our feelings toward our existing partners or by disliking strangers who do not. We propose an alternative theory, the Embedded Dyad Framework, which foregrounds the substantive effects that strangers can have on our welfare through their interactions with our dyadic partners. Across four experiments (<em>N</em> = 1589) with U.S.-residing participants we predict and find, consistent with the Embedded Dyad Framework, that we like strangers who share our hatred for our rivals and our love for our friends (consistent with Balance Theory); but we dislike strangers who share our love for our spouses (contradicting Balance Theory). Further supporting predictions from an Embedded Dyad Framework, (a) greater perceived exclusivity in welfare-enhancing dyadic relationships (e.g., friendships) drives our lesser liking of strangers who share our love for our partners, and (b) greater perceived welfare suppression by our antagonistic partners (e.g., rivals) drives our liking of strangers who share our hatred of our antagonists. This framework outpredicts cognitive consistency views by emphasizing the real threats and opportunities that dyadic relationships afford people when dyads are embedded in social networks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 203-213"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140129639","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why do people make noises in bed? 为什么人们在床上会发出声音?
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.002
Andrey Anikin

Many primates produce copulation calls, but we have surprisingly little data on what human sex sounds like. I present 34 h of audio recordings from 2239 authentic sexual episodes shared online. These include partnered sex or masturbation, but each recording has only one main vocalizer (1950 female, 289 male). Both acoustic features and arousal ratings from an online perceptual experiment with 109 listeners recruited on Prolific follow an inverted-U curve, revealing the likely time of orgasm. Sexual vocalizations become longer, louder, more high-pitched, voiced, and unpredictable at orgasm in both men and women. Men are not less vocal overall in this sample, but women start moaning at an earlier stage; speech or even minimally verbalized exclamations are uncommon. While excessive vocalizing sounds inauthentic to listeners, vocal bursts at peak arousal are ubiquitous and less verbalized than in the build-up phase, suggesting limited volitional control. Human sexual vocalizations likely include both consciously controlled and spontaneous moans of pleasure, which are perhaps best understood as sounds of liking rather than signals specific to copulation.

许多灵长类动物都会发出交配的叫声,但关于人类性爱声音的数据却少得令人吃惊。我介绍了 34 小时的录音,这些录音来自网上共享的 2239 个真实的性爱场景。其中包括伴侣性行为或手淫,但每段录音只有一个主要发声者(1950 个女性,289 个男性)。在 Prolific 网站上招募的 109 名听众进行了在线感知实验,实验结果显示,声音特征和唤醒评分均呈倒 U 型曲线,揭示了性高潮的可能时间。在性高潮时,男性和女性的性发声都会变得更长、更响亮、音调更高、声音更大且更难以预测。在这一样本中,男性的发声总体上并没有减少,但女性开始呻吟的时间更早;说话或甚至微弱的口头感叹都不常见。虽然过度发声对听众来说听起来不真实,但唤醒高峰期的爆发性发声无处不在,而且与酝酿阶段相比口头发声较少,这表明意志控制能力有限。人类的性发声可能包括有意识控制的愉悦呻吟和自发的愉悦呻吟,也许最好将其理解为交配的声音而不是特定的信号。
{"title":"Why do people make noises in bed?","authors":"Andrey Anikin","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.02.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many primates produce copulation calls, but we have surprisingly little data on what human sex sounds like. I present 34 h of audio recordings from 2239 authentic sexual episodes shared online. These include partnered sex or masturbation, but each recording has only one main vocalizer (1950 female, 289 male). Both acoustic features and arousal ratings from an online perceptual experiment with 109 listeners recruited on Prolific follow an inverted-U curve, revealing the likely time of orgasm. Sexual vocalizations become longer, louder, more high-pitched, voiced, and unpredictable at orgasm in both men and women. Men are not less vocal overall in this sample, but women start moaning at an earlier stage; speech or even minimally verbalized exclamations are uncommon. While excessive vocalizing sounds inauthentic to listeners, vocal bursts at peak arousal are ubiquitous and less verbalized than in the build-up phase, suggesting limited volitional control. Human sexual vocalizations likely include both consciously controlled and spontaneous moans of pleasure, which are perhaps best understood as sounds of <em>liking</em> rather than signals specific to copulation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 183-192"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000217/pdfft?md5=5fdcfb76da04ce19c481f7e16fcb7230&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000217-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140006061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Victims of misfortune may not “deserve” help: A possible factor in victim-devaluation 不幸的受害者可能并不 "值得 "帮助:评价受害者的一个可能因素
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.005
Pascal Boyer , Eric Chantland , Lou Safra

Why do people blame, devalue or derogate the victims of misfortune? The literature suggests general factors like a belief in a just world or a desire to distance oneself from misfortune, but the empirical results are often unclear. Here we suggest another potential factor in victim-devaluation in particular. Attitudes to victims should be seen in the context of human cooperation, as victims can be a source of costs for others and, therefore, may constitute poor potential cooperation partners. If that is the case, devaluation should be associated with a reluctance to offer help to victims. As predicted, across six pre-registered studies, we found that participants' reluctance to donate their own money (their bonus for participation), or allocate other people's money to a victim predicted the devaluation of the victim's character. Both devaluation and willingness to help were influenced by manipulating the victim's apparent competence, and the victim's concern for other people's possible costs, two crucial dimensions of cooperative potential. These results are consistent with the overall hypothesis that people's intuitions about a victim's cooperation potential are relevant to victim-devaluation.

为什么人们会指责、贬低或贬损不幸的受害者?文献中提出了一些一般性的因素,如人们相信世界是公正的,或希望远离不幸,但实证结果往往并不明确。在此,我们特别提出了受害者评价的另一个潜在因素。我们应该从人类合作的角度来看待对受害者的态度,因为受害者可能是他人付出代价的来源,因此可能构成不良的潜在合作伙伴。如果是这样的话,对受害者的贬低应该与不愿意向受害者提供帮助有关。正如我们所预测的那样,在六项预先登记的研究中,我们发现参与者不愿意捐出自己的钱(他们参与研究的奖金)或将其他人的钱分配给受害者,这预示着对受害者人格的贬低。通过操纵受害者的表面能力和受害者对他人可能付出的代价的关心程度,这两个合作潜力的关键维度都会对贬低和帮助意愿产生影响。这些结果与总体假设一致,即人们对受害者合作潜力的直觉与受害者评价相关。
{"title":"Victims of misfortune may not “deserve” help: A possible factor in victim-devaluation","authors":"Pascal Boyer ,&nbsp;Eric Chantland ,&nbsp;Lou Safra","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.01.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Why do people blame, devalue or derogate the victims of misfortune? The literature suggests general factors like a belief in a just world or a desire to distance oneself from misfortune, but the empirical results are often unclear. Here we suggest another potential factor in victim-devaluation in particular. Attitudes to victims should be seen in the context of human cooperation, as victims can be a source of costs for others and, therefore, may constitute poor potential cooperation partners. If that is the case, devaluation should be associated with a reluctance to offer help to victims. As predicted, across six pre-registered studies, we found that participants' reluctance to donate their own money (their bonus for participation), or allocate other people's money to a victim predicted the devaluation of the victim's character. Both devaluation and willingness to help were influenced by manipulating the victim's apparent competence, and the victim's concern for other people's possible costs, two crucial dimensions of cooperative potential. These results are consistent with the overall hypothesis that people's intuitions about a victim's cooperation potential are relevant to victim-devaluation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 2","pages":"Pages 153-163"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139668709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parochial altruism: What it is and why it varies 狭隘利他主义:它是什么以及它为什么会变化
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.06.005
Anne C. Pisor , Cody T. Ross

Parochial altruism (PA), or ingroup favoritism paired with outgroup hostility, is sometimes treated as a synonym for human intergroup relations. However, empirical data suggest that PA is highly variable—across individuals, across situations, and across groups. Here, we review theory and data on PA to explore the candidate sources for this variability. Along the way, we unpack assumptions (e.g., what constitutes a group?), identify precursors to PA behavior (e.g., context and internal states), and review evidence for the pairing of ingroup favoritism with outgroup hostility. We discuss phenomena with measurable impact on downstream behavior, including resource access and cultural institutions, but also flag how researcher expectations and methodological design impact reported variability in PA. We close by making recommendations for how researchers can reduce noise in the study of PA by checking assumptions and being deliberate in research design; this is key, as the PA literature is part of sensitive public discourse.

狭隘利他主义(Parochial altruism,PA),或内群体偏袒与外群体敌意的搭配,有时被视为人类群体间关系的同义词。然而,经验数据表明,PA 在不同个体、不同情境和不同群体之间存在很大差异。在此,我们回顾了有关 PA 的理论和数据,以探索这种可变性的候选来源。在此过程中,我们将解开各种假设(例如,什么是群体?我们讨论了对下游行为有可测量影响的现象,包括资源获取和文化制度,同时也指出了研究者的期望和方法设计如何影响 PA 的报告变异性。最后,我们就研究人员如何通过检查假设和深思熟虑的研究设计来减少 PA 研究中的噪音提出了建议;这一点非常关键,因为 PA 文献是敏感的公共讨论的一部分。
{"title":"Parochial altruism: What it is and why it varies","authors":"Anne C. Pisor ,&nbsp;Cody T. Ross","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.06.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.06.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Parochial altruism<span> (PA), or ingroup favoritism paired with outgroup hostility, is sometimes treated as a synonym for human intergroup relations. However, empirical data suggest that PA is highly variable—across individuals, across situations, and across groups. Here, we review theory and data on PA to explore the candidate sources for this variability. Along the way, we unpack assumptions (e.g., what constitutes a group?), identify precursors to PA behavior (e.g., context and internal states), and review evidence for the pairing of ingroup favoritism with outgroup hostility. We discuss phenomena with measurable impact on downstream behavior, including resource access and cultural institutions, but also flag how researcher expectations and methodological design impact reported variability in PA. We close by making recommendations for how researchers can reduce noise in the study of PA by checking assumptions and being deliberate in research design; this is key, as the PA literature is part of sensitive public discourse.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 1","pages":"Pages 2-12"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42965107","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reference frames for spatial navigation and declarative memory: Individual differences in performance support the phylogenetic continuity hypothesis 空间导航和陈述性记忆的参考框架:表现的个体差异支持系统发育连续性假说
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.08.001
A. Fragueiro , A. Tosoni , M. Boccia , R. Di Matteo , C. Sestieri , G. Committeri

Recent experimental evidence has led to the idea that the neural mechanisms supporting spatial navigation have been flexibly adapted to organize concepts and memories through spatial codes. The “phylogenetic continuity hypothesis” (Buszáki & Moser, 2013) further proposes that the mechanisms supporting episodic and semantic memory would have respectively evolved from self-based (i.e. egocentric) and map-based (i.e. allocentric) spatial navigation mechanisms. Recent studies have observed traces of this phylogenetic continuity in human behavior, but the full original model has not yet been tested. Here, we evaluated the relationships between the four model components by using two sets of tasks in the spatial navigation and declarative memory domains based on complex materials and emphasizing the self vs. map-based processing (i.e. route vs. survey component for spatial navigation and episodic vs. semantic component for declarative memory). Consistent with the model predictions, the results of a multiple multivariate regression analysis revealed a specific across-domain relationship, such that route-based navigation performance specifically predicted episodic memory performance (self-based, egocentric components), while survey navigation performance specifically predicted the semantic memory one (map-based, allocentric components). The results of an additional regression analysis on the within-domain transformation process from self-based to map-based representations confirmed that route-based navigation specifically predicted survey navigation, while episodic memory specifically predicted semantic memory. Our results provide further behavioral evidence in support of the general hypothesis that the neural machinery evolved to map the physical world might have been recycled to organize memory and conceptual knowledge. Crucially, they also support the more specific hypothesis that the organizational principles involved in higher-level processing of information have inherited the fundamental distinction between different reference frames (egocentric vs. allocentric) for navigation in the physical world.

最近的实验证据表明,支持空间导航的神经机制已被灵活地调整为通过空间代码来组织概念和记忆。系统发育连续性假说"(Buszáki & Moser, 2013)进一步提出,支持情节记忆和语义记忆的机制将分别从基于自我(即以自我为中心)和基于地图(即以分配为中心)的空间导航机制进化而来。最近的研究在人类行为中观察到了这种系统发育连续性的痕迹,但完整的原始模型尚未得到验证。在此,我们使用两组基于复杂材料的空间导航和声明性记忆领域的任务,评估了模型四个组成部分之间的关系,并强调了自我与基于地图的处理(即空间导航的路线与调查部分,声明性记忆的情节与语义部分)。与模型预测一致的是,多重多元回归分析的结果显示了一种特定的跨领域关系,即基于路线的导航成绩特别预测了外显记忆成绩(以自我为基础、以自我为中心的成分),而调查导航成绩特别预测了语义记忆成绩(以地图为基础、以分配为中心的成分)。对从自我表征到地图表征的域内转换过程进行的额外回归分析结果证实,路线导航可具体预测调查导航,而外显记忆可具体预测语义记忆。我们的研究结果为一般假设提供了进一步的行为证据,即用于绘制物理世界地图的神经机制可能被用于组织记忆和概念知识。最重要的是,这些结果还支持了一个更具体的假设,即高层次信息处理的组织原则继承了物理世界中不同参照系(以自我为中心与以分配为中心)导航的基本区别。
{"title":"Reference frames for spatial navigation and declarative memory: Individual differences in performance support the phylogenetic continuity hypothesis","authors":"A. Fragueiro ,&nbsp;A. Tosoni ,&nbsp;M. Boccia ,&nbsp;R. Di Matteo ,&nbsp;C. Sestieri ,&nbsp;G. Committeri","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.08.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.08.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent experimental evidence has led to the idea that the neural mechanisms supporting spatial navigation have been flexibly adapted to organize concepts and memories through spatial codes. The “phylogenetic continuity hypothesis” (Buszáki &amp; Moser, 2013) further proposes that the mechanisms supporting episodic and semantic memory would have respectively evolved from self-based (i.e. egocentric) and map-based (i.e. allocentric) spatial navigation mechanisms. Recent studies have observed traces of this phylogenetic continuity in human behavior, but the full original model has not yet been tested. Here, we evaluated the relationships between the four model components by using two sets of tasks in the spatial navigation and declarative memory domains based on complex materials and emphasizing the self vs. map-based processing (i.e. route vs. survey component for spatial navigation and episodic vs. semantic component for declarative memory). Consistent with the model predictions, the results of a multiple multivariate regression analysis revealed a specific across-domain relationship, such that route-based navigation performance specifically predicted episodic memory performance (self-based, egocentric components), while survey navigation performance specifically predicted the semantic memory one (map-based, allocentric components). The results of an additional regression analysis on the within-domain transformation process from self-based to map-based representations confirmed that route-based navigation specifically predicted survey navigation, while episodic memory specifically predicted semantic memory. Our results provide further behavioral evidence in support of the general hypothesis that the neural machinery evolved to map the physical world might have been recycled to organize memory and conceptual knowledge. Crucially, they also support the more specific hypothesis that the organizational principles involved in higher-level processing of information have inherited the fundamental distinction between different reference frames (egocentric vs. allocentric) for navigation in the physical world.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 1","pages":"Pages 20-26"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513823000685/pdfft?md5=a6582493f0e34c61cf277c883e45a706&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513823000685-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42302671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinctiveness and femininity, rather than symmetry and masculinity, affect facial attractiveness across the world 影响全球面部吸引力的是独特性和女性气质,而不是对称性和男性气质
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.001
Karel Kleisner , Petr Tureček , S. Adil Saribay , Ondřej Pavlovič , Juan David Leongómez , S. Craig Roberts , Jan Havlíček , Jaroslava Varella Valentova , Silviu Apostol , Robert Mbe Akoko , Marco A.C. Varella

Studies investigating facial attractiveness in humans have frequently been limited to studying the effect of individual morphological factors in isolation from other facial shape components in the same population. In this study, we go beyond this approach by focusing on multiple components and populations while combining geometric morphometrics of 72 standardized frontal facial landmarks and a Bayesian statistical framework. We investigate preferences in both sexes for three structural components of other sex facial beauty that are traditionally considered indicators of biological quality: symmetry, sexual dimorphism, and distinctiveness (i.e., the opposite of averageness). Based on a large sample of faces (n = 1550) from 10 populations across the world (Brazil, Cameroon, Czechia, Colombia, India, Namibia, Romania, Turkey, UK, and Vietnam), we found that distinctiveness negatively affects the perception of attractiveness in both sexes and that this association is stable across all studied populations. We corroborated some previous results indicating both a positive effect of femininity on male assessment of female facial beauty and a null or weak effect of masculinity on female evaluation of male facial attractiveness. Facial symmetry had no effect on facial attractiveness. In concert with other recent studies, our results support the importance of facial prototypicality but cast doubt on the role of symmetry as one of the key constituents of attractiveness in the human face.

对人类面部吸引力的研究往往局限于研究单个形态因素对同一人群中其他面部形状成分的影响。在本研究中,我们超越了这一研究方法,将 72 个标准化额面部地标的几何形态计量学与贝叶斯统计框架相结合,重点研究了多个组成部分和人群。我们研究了两性对其他性别面部美的三个结构成分的偏好,这些成分传统上被认为是生物质量的指标:对称性、性别二形性和独特性(即与平均性相反)。基于来自全球 10 个国家(巴西、喀麦隆、捷克、哥伦比亚、印度、纳米比亚、罗马尼亚、土耳其、英国和越南)的大量人脸样本(n = 1550),我们发现,独特性对两性的吸引力感知都有负面影响,而且这种关联在所有研究人群中都是稳定的。我们证实了之前的一些研究结果,即女性特质对男性评价女性面部美有积极影响,而男性特质对女性评价男性面部吸引力无影响或影响较弱。面部对称性对面部吸引力没有影响。我们的研究结果与最近的其他研究结果一致,支持了面部原型性的重要性,但对对称性作为人类面部吸引力的主要构成因素之一的作用表示怀疑。
{"title":"Distinctiveness and femininity, rather than symmetry and masculinity, affect facial attractiveness across the world","authors":"Karel Kleisner ,&nbsp;Petr Tureček ,&nbsp;S. Adil Saribay ,&nbsp;Ondřej Pavlovič ,&nbsp;Juan David Leongómez ,&nbsp;S. Craig Roberts ,&nbsp;Jan Havlíček ,&nbsp;Jaroslava Varella Valentova ,&nbsp;Silviu Apostol ,&nbsp;Robert Mbe Akoko ,&nbsp;Marco A.C. Varella","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Studies investigating facial attractiveness in humans have frequently been limited to studying the effect of individual morphological factors in isolation from other facial shape components in the same population. In this study, we go beyond this approach by focusing on multiple components and populations while combining geometric morphometrics of 72 standardized frontal facial landmarks and a Bayesian statistical framework. We investigate preferences in both sexes for three structural components of other sex facial beauty that are traditionally considered indicators of biological quality: symmetry, sexual dimorphism, and distinctiveness (i.e., the opposite of averageness). Based on a large sample of faces (<em>n</em> = 1550) from 10 populations across the world (Brazil, Cameroon, Czechia, Colombia, India, Namibia, Romania, Turkey, UK, and Vietnam), we found that distinctiveness negatively affects the perception of attractiveness in both sexes and that this association is stable across all studied populations. We corroborated some previous results indicating both a positive effect of femininity on male assessment of female facial beauty and a null or weak effect of masculinity on female evaluation of male facial attractiveness. Facial symmetry had no effect on facial attractiveness. In concert with other recent studies, our results support the importance of facial prototypicality but cast doubt on the role of symmetry as one of the key constituents of attractiveness in the human face.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 1","pages":"Pages 82-90"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136009387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1