首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Children's evolved cues to promote caregiving: Are voices more powerful than thoughts in signaling young children's attributes and needs to adults? 儿童进化出的促进照料的线索:在向成人传递幼儿的属性和需求时,声音是否比想法更有力?
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609
Carlos Hernández Blasi , David F. Bjorklund , Sonia Agut , Francisco Lozano Nomdedeu , Miguel Ángel Martínez

Children have evolved “psychological weapons” to endear them to adults, enhancing their chances of surviving. Earlier research has shown that, during early childhood, caregivers feel positively attracted by children's vocal and cognitive cues of immaturity, which in turn provide adults with information about children's attributes and needs. The purpose of this study was to disentangle which of these two cues (vocal or cognitive), if either, might be more relevant for adults in assessing children's attributes and needs. College students (n = 273) listened to four pairs of children reasoning either in a mature or an immature manner about two types of thinking, one we labeled supernatural that reflects “magical thinking” (e.g., “The sun's not out because it's mad”) and the other we labeled natural that reflects abilities such as estimating one's cognition abilities or inhibition (e.g., “I can remember all the words you showed me”). In one condition (Consistent), the immaturity/maturity of children's reasoning matched the immaturity/maturity of their voices, whereas in the other condition (Inconsistent) they did not. Results revealed that, regardless of the type of reasoning, children's vocal cues prevailed over cognitive cues for assessing attributes of positive affect and helplessness. Conversely, children's cognitive cues prevailed over vocal cues for assessing intelligence (but only for supernatural thinking), and negative affect (but only for natural thinking). The results reveal natural selection's use of different cues of immaturity to promote caregiving during early childhood and reflect the complexity of multimodal features when adults evaluate young children.

儿童进化出了让成人喜爱的 "心理武器",从而增加了他们生存的机会。早先的研究表明,在幼儿期,照料者会被儿童的声音和认知上的不成熟暗示所吸引,这反过来又为成人提供了有关儿童属性和需求的信息。本研究的目的是要弄清这两种线索(声音线索和认知线索)中,哪一种(如果有的话)对成人评估儿童的属性和需求更有意义。大学生(n = 273)聆听了四对儿童以成熟或不成熟的方式就两种思维类型进行的推理,其中一种我们称为超自然思维,反映了 "神奇思维"(如 "太阳没出来是因为它疯了"),另一种我们称为自然思维,反映了估计自己的认知能力或抑制能力(如 "我能记住你给我看的所有单词")。在一种情况下(一致),儿童推理的不成熟/成熟度与他们声音的不成熟/成熟度相匹配,而在另一种情况下(不一致)则不然。结果显示,无论推理类型如何,在评估积极情感和无助感属性时,儿童的声音线索都优先于认知线索。相反,在评估智力(但仅限于超自然思维)和负面情绪(但仅限于自然思维)时,儿童的认知线索优先于声音线索。这些结果揭示了自然选择使用不同的不成熟线索来促进幼儿期的照顾,并反映了成人评价幼儿时多模态特征的复杂性。
{"title":"Children's evolved cues to promote caregiving: Are voices more powerful than thoughts in signaling young children's attributes and needs to adults?","authors":"Carlos Hernández Blasi ,&nbsp;David F. Bjorklund ,&nbsp;Sonia Agut ,&nbsp;Francisco Lozano Nomdedeu ,&nbsp;Miguel Ángel Martínez","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children have evolved “psychological weapons” to endear them to adults, enhancing their chances of surviving. Earlier research has shown that, during early childhood, caregivers feel positively attracted by children's vocal and cognitive cues of immaturity, which in turn provide adults with information about children's attributes and needs. The purpose of this study was to disentangle which of these two cues (vocal or cognitive), if either, might be more relevant for adults in assessing children's attributes and needs. College students (<em>n</em> = 273) listened to four pairs of children reasoning either in a mature or an immature manner about two types of thinking, one we labeled <em>supernatural</em> that reflects “magical thinking” (e.g., “The sun's not out because it's mad”) and the other we labeled <em>natural</em> that reflects abilities such as estimating one's cognition abilities or inhibition (e.g., “I can remember all the words you showed me”). In one condition (<em>Consistent</em>), the immaturity/maturity of children's reasoning matched the immaturity/maturity of their voices, whereas in the other condition (<em>Inconsistent</em>) they did not. Results revealed that, regardless of the type of reasoning, children's vocal cues prevailed over cognitive cues for assessing attributes of positive affect and helplessness. Conversely, children's cognitive cues prevailed over vocal cues for assessing intelligence (but only for supernatural thinking), and negative affect (but only for natural thinking). The results reveal natural selection's use of different cues of immaturity to promote caregiving during early childhood and reflect the complexity of multimodal features when adults evaluate young children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106609"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000850/pdfft?md5=f1efee7fcf5b8676c619e7bbb95045b7&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000850-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Religious signaling and prosociality: A review of the literature 宗教信号与亲社会性:文献综述
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002
Stefanie B. Northover , Tadeg Quillien , Daniel Conroy-Beam , Adam B. Cohen

The costly signaling theory of religion states that costly religious behaviors, badges, and bans (“religious practice” for short) are signals of commitment to the ingroup and its moral code. Such signals are proposed to increase cooperation. Here we review the empirical literature, which suggests that religious actors are often perceived as especially trustworthy and may be more likely recipients of help and cooperation. The evidence does not present a clear picture regarding the actual trustworthiness nor prosocial tendencies of religious actors. Limited available evidence suggests that routine forms of religious behavior are associated with ingroup favoritism. High-cost, infrequent, highly social forms of religious practice are associated with an increase in religious identity, but also an expanded social identity and greater tolerance for outgroup members. Following the literature review, we provide a discussion of proposed future research directions pertaining to the costs and benefits of religious practice, moderators, secular versus religious practice, and mediation of the relationship between observed religious practice and perceptions of religious actors' trustworthiness.

宗教的代价信号理论认为,代价高昂的宗教行为、徽章和禁令(简称 "宗教实践")是对内聚群体及其道德准则做出承诺的信号。这种信号被认为可以增加合作。在此,我们回顾了实证文献,这些文献表明,宗教行为者通常被认为特别值得信赖,可能更容易获得帮助与合作。关于宗教行为者的实际可信度或亲社会倾向,证据并没有给出清晰的描述。有限的现有证据表明,常规形式的宗教行为与内群体偏袒有关。高成本、非经常性、高度社会化的宗教活动形式与宗教认同感的增强有关,但也与社会认同感的扩大和对外群体成员的更大容忍度有关。在文献综述之后,我们就宗教实践的成本与收益、调节因素、世俗实践与宗教实践、观察到的宗教实践与对宗教行为者可信度认知之间关系的调解等方面的未来研究方向进行了讨论。
{"title":"Religious signaling and prosociality: A review of the literature","authors":"Stefanie B. Northover ,&nbsp;Tadeg Quillien ,&nbsp;Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;Adam B. Cohen","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.06.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The costly signaling theory of religion states that costly religious behaviors, badges, and bans (“religious practice” for short) are signals of commitment to the ingroup and its moral code. Such signals are proposed to increase cooperation. Here we review the empirical literature, which suggests that religious actors are often perceived as especially trustworthy and may be more likely recipients of help and cooperation. The evidence does not present a clear picture regarding the actual trustworthiness nor prosocial tendencies of religious actors<em>.</em> Limited available evidence suggests that routine forms of religious behavior are associated with ingroup favoritism. High-cost, infrequent, highly social forms of religious practice are associated with an increase in religious identity, but also an expanded social identity and greater tolerance for outgroup members. Following the literature review, we provide a discussion of proposed future research directions pertaining to the costs and benefits of religious practice, moderators, secular versus religious practice, and mediation of the relationship between observed religious practice and perceptions of religious actors' trustworthiness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106593"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141606889","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why women cheat: testing evolutionary hypotheses for female infidelity in a multinational sample 女性为何出轨:在多国样本中检验女性不忠的进化假设
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595
Macken Murphy , Caroline A. Phillips , Khandis R. Blake

While scholars largely agree men's infidelity evolved by increasing offspring quantity, the evolutionary drivers of women's infidelity remain debated. The “good genes” (dual mating strategy) hypothesis posits infidelity allows women to pair the preferred genes of an affair partner with the preferred investment of their primary partner (Gangstad & Thornhill, 1998). The mate-switching hypothesis instead argues infidelity helps women obtain a new mate without a period of deprivation (Buss et al., 2017). To test these hypotheses, we conducted a pre-registered survey of 254 individuals from 19 countries and 6 continents who were previously or currently engaged in infidelity. We measured individuals' perception of their primary partner and their affair partner across four domains: physical attractiveness, personal attractiveness, attractiveness as a co-parent, and overall desirability (mate value). We also asked participants to report their motivations for the affair. Consistent with a dual mating strategy, women experienced stronger physical attraction to their affair partners and stronger parental attraction to their primary partners. Contrary to the mate-switching hypothesis, women did not prefer their affair partners overall, parentally, or personally. There were no significant gender differences in these findings, suggesting strategic dualism in men as well. Our qualitative data revealed a more nuanced story at the individual level, with participants reporting motives consistent with a variety of evolutionarily coherent strategies. While our quantitative results speak to the relevance of the dual-mating hypothesis to understanding infidelity, our findings also suggest that seeking infidelity's primary explanation in either gender is, perhaps, too simple an approach to the issue.

尽管学者们普遍认为男性的不忠行为是通过增加后代数量进化而来的,但对女性不忠行为的进化动因仍存在争议。好基因"(双重交配策略)假说认为,不忠行为可以让女性将外遇伴侣的偏好基因与其主要伴侣的偏好投资配对(Gangstad & Thornhill, 1998)。伴侣转换假说则认为,不忠行为有助于女性在没有剥夺期的情况下获得新伴侣(Buss 等人,2017 年)。为了验证这些假设,我们对来自 6 大洲 19 个国家的 254 名曾经或目前有不忠行为的人进行了预先登记的调查。我们测量了个人对其主要伴侣和外遇伴侣在以下四个方面的看法:外貌吸引力、个人吸引力、作为共同父母的吸引力以及总体可取性(伴侣价值)。我们还要求参与者报告他们外遇的动机。与双重交配策略一致的是,女性对外遇伴侣的身体吸引力更强,而对主要伴侣的父母吸引力更强。与伴侣转换假说相反,女性并不更喜欢外遇伴侣的整体、父母或个人。这些研究结果没有明显的性别差异,这表明男性也存在策略双重性。我们的定性数据揭示了个人层面上更细微的故事,参与者报告的动机与各种进化上一致的策略相一致。虽然我们的定量结果说明了双重交配假说与理解不忠行为的相关性,但我们的研究结果也表明,在两性中寻找不忠行为的主要解释也许是一种过于简单的方法。
{"title":"Why women cheat: testing evolutionary hypotheses for female infidelity in a multinational sample","authors":"Macken Murphy ,&nbsp;Caroline A. Phillips ,&nbsp;Khandis R. Blake","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106595","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>While scholars largely agree men's infidelity evolved by increasing offspring quantity, the evolutionary drivers of women's infidelity remain debated. The “good genes” (dual mating strategy) hypothesis posits infidelity allows women to pair the preferred genes of an affair partner with the preferred investment of their primary partner (Gangstad &amp; Thornhill, 1998). The mate-switching hypothesis instead argues infidelity helps women obtain a new mate without a period of deprivation (Buss et al., 2017). To test these hypotheses, we conducted a pre-registered survey of 254 individuals from 19 countries and 6 continents who were previously or currently engaged in infidelity. We measured individuals' perception of their primary partner and their affair partner across four domains: physical attractiveness, personal attractiveness, attractiveness as a co-parent, and overall desirability (mate value). We also asked participants to report their motivations for the affair. Consistent with a dual mating strategy, women experienced stronger physical attraction to their affair partners and stronger parental attraction to their primary partners. Contrary to the mate-switching hypothesis, women did not prefer their affair partners overall, parentally, or personally. There were no significant gender differences in these findings, suggesting strategic dualism in men as well. Our qualitative data revealed a more nuanced story at the individual level, with participants reporting motives consistent with a variety of evolutionarily coherent strategies. While our quantitative results speak to the relevance of the dual-mating hypothesis to understanding infidelity, our findings also suggest that seeking infidelity's primary explanation in either gender is, perhaps, too simple an approach to the issue.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106595"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000710/pdfft?md5=17fac63b8dd7bb1c5f853c1541025986&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000710-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141539191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genomic findings and their implications for the evolutionary social sciences 基因组研究结果及其对进化社会科学的影响
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596
Brendan P. Zietsch

What past selection pressures have shaped human traits and their variation and covariation across individuals? These are key questions in the evolutionary social sciences. Recent advances in the field of human genomics have yielded a wealth of evidence that sheds light on these questions, yet the findings and their implications seem to be little known in the evolutionary social sciences. In this paper I aim to bring together these findings while explaining the conceptual and technical background that is often assumed knowledge for reading the primary reports. First, I outline the genomics methodologies that have enabled the relevant findings, such as genomewide association studies and DNA-based heritability estimation. I describe how these methodologies reveal the genetic architecture of traits, and then how this information in turn enables inferences about past selection. The findings show pervasive evidence that the genetic architecture of complex traits has been shaped by negative (purifying) selection, implying that the extant genetic variation in the traits has been maintained by mutation-selection-drift balance. On the other hand, there is no evidence that balancing selection has substantively shaped complex traits, and strong evidence that it has not. Finally, I discuss the implications of these findings for issues such as the dimensional structure of personality variation and the plausibility of psychological life history theory.

过去的选择压力塑造了人类的哪些特征及其在个体间的变异和共变?这些都是进化社会科学的关键问题。人类基因组学领域的最新进展提供了大量证据来揭示这些问题,但这些发现及其影响在进化社会科学领域似乎鲜为人知。在本文中,我旨在汇集这些研究成果,同时解释在阅读主要报告时通常被假定为知识的概念和技术背景。首先,我概述了促成相关发现的基因组学方法,如全基因组关联研究和基于 DNA 的遗传率估计。我将介绍这些方法如何揭示性状的遗传结构,以及这些信息如何反过来推断过去的选择。研究结果表明,有大量证据表明,复杂性状的遗传结构是由负向(净化)选择形成的,这意味着性状中现存的遗传变异是由突变-选择-漂移平衡维持的。另一方面,没有证据表明平衡选择实质上塑造了复杂性状,也有强有力的证据表明平衡选择没有塑造复杂性状。最后,我将讨论这些发现对人格变异的维度结构和心理生活史理论的合理性等问题的影响。
{"title":"Genomic findings and their implications for the evolutionary social sciences","authors":"Brendan P. Zietsch","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106596","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>What past selection pressures have shaped human traits and their variation and covariation across individuals? These are key questions in the evolutionary social sciences. Recent advances in the field of human genomics have yielded a wealth of evidence that sheds light on these questions, yet the findings and their implications seem to be little known in the evolutionary social sciences. In this paper I aim to bring together these findings while explaining the conceptual and technical background that is often assumed knowledge for reading the primary reports. First, I outline the genomics methodologies that have enabled the relevant findings, such as genomewide association studies and DNA-based heritability estimation. I describe how these methodologies reveal the genetic architecture of traits, and then how this information in turn enables inferences about past selection. The findings show pervasive evidence that the genetic architecture of complex traits has been shaped by negative (purifying) selection, implying that the extant genetic variation in the traits has been maintained by mutation-selection-drift balance. On the other hand, there is no evidence that balancing selection has substantively shaped complex traits, and strong evidence that it has not. Finally, I discuss the implications of these findings for issues such as the dimensional structure of personality variation and the plausibility of psychological life history theory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106596"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000722/pdfft?md5=645cc54ab3f014e3bc9ba1f3bac18fee&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000722-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the immediate effects of income inequality on homicide rates: A reply to Daly's critique 超越收入不平等对凶杀率的直接影响:回应戴利的批评
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597
Carlos Vilalta , Edel Cadena , Carlos Garrocho , Gustavo Fondevila

This study responds to Martin Daly's critique of our 2022 study on the correlation between income inequality and homicide rates in Mexican municipalities. Our updated analysis incorporates both immediate and lagged effects of income inequality, revealing significant non-linear relationships between past inequality and current homicide rates. We find that higher levels of past inequality interact with present inequality to increase homicide rates, particularly among currently average and highly unequal municipalities –not so much in less unequal municipalities. These findings support Daly's argument that economic inequality's influence on violent behavior accumulates over time, highlighting the need for a time dimension in homicide rate models. Our study emphasizes the importance of considering historical economic conditions when addressing socio-economic determinants of homicide, aiming to contribute constructively to ongoing attempts to effectively reduce homicidal violence.

本研究回应了马丁-戴利对我们 2022 年关于墨西哥城市收入不平等与凶杀率之间相关性研究的批评。我们的最新分析纳入了收入不平等的即时和滞后效应,揭示了过去的不平等与当前凶杀率之间的显著非线性关系。我们发现,过去较高水平的不平等与当前的不平等相互作用,增加了凶杀率,尤其是在目前处于平均水平和高度不平等的城市中,而在不太平等的城市中则没有这么严重。这些研究结果支持了 Daly 的观点,即经济不平等对暴力行为的影响会随着时间的推移而不断累积,从而突出了在凶杀率模型中加入时间维度的必要性。我们的研究强调了在处理杀人案的社会经济决定因素时考虑历史经济条件的重要性,旨在为目前有效减少杀人暴力的尝试做出建设性贡献。
{"title":"Beyond the immediate effects of income inequality on homicide rates: A reply to Daly's critique","authors":"Carlos Vilalta ,&nbsp;Edel Cadena ,&nbsp;Carlos Garrocho ,&nbsp;Gustavo Fondevila","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106597","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study responds to Martin Daly's critique of our 2022 study on the correlation between income inequality and homicide rates in Mexican municipalities. Our updated analysis incorporates both immediate and lagged effects of income inequality, revealing significant non-linear relationships between past inequality and current homicide rates. We find that higher levels of past inequality interact with present inequality to increase homicide rates, particularly among currently average and highly unequal municipalities –not so much in less unequal municipalities. These findings support Daly's argument that economic inequality's influence on violent behavior accumulates over time, highlighting the need for a time dimension in homicide rate models. Our study emphasizes the importance of considering historical economic conditions when addressing socio-economic determinants of homicide, aiming to contribute constructively to ongoing attempts to effectively reduce homicidal violence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106597"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prestige and dominance in egalitarian and hierarchical societies: Children in Finland favor prestige more than children in Colombia or the USA 平等社会和等级社会中的声望和支配地位:芬兰儿童比哥伦比亚或美国儿童更喜欢威望
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005
Maija-Eliina Sequeira , Narges Afshordi , Anni Kajanus

We examined how children reason about dominance and prestige in Colombia, Finland, and the USA, contexts that vary in terms of societal inequality and hierarchical organization. We tested 496 children aged 4–11 years old to determine whether they: i) recognized and discriminated between dominance and prestige, ii) preferred to learn from a dominant or prestigious character, iii) assigned leadership to a dominant or prestigious character, and iv) self-identified more with a dominant or subordinate character. Older children were more likely to recognize, prefer, learn from, and assign leadership to the prestigious character, and to identify with the subordinate. There were no cross-cultural differences in learning preferences, supporting evolutionary theories that posit a universal bias towards social learning from prestigious individuals. There was variation in leadership preferences; children were the most likely to assign leadership to a prestigious character in more egalitarian Finland, and least likely in more unequal Colombia. We argue that societal factors including levels of inequality and hierarchical social organization shape an underlying propensity for children to learn to reason about rank and to broadly favor prestige in leaders and models for learning from.

我们研究了哥伦比亚、芬兰和美国儿童如何推理支配地位和威望,这些国家的社会不平等和等级组织各不相同。我们对 496 名 4-11 岁的儿童进行了测试,以确定他们是否:i) 认识并区分支配地位和威望;ii) 喜欢向支配地位或威望高的角色学习;iii) 将领导权赋予支配地位或威望高的角色;以及 iv) 更认同支配地位或从属地位的角色。年龄较大的儿童更有可能认识到、更喜欢、更愿意向有威望的角色学习,并将领导权分配给有威望的角色,也更有可能认同从属角色。在学习偏好方面没有跨文化差异,这支持了进化论的观点,即人们普遍倾向于向有威望的人学习社会知识。在领导力偏好方面存在差异;在较为平等的芬兰,儿童最有可能将领导力赋予有威望的人物,而在较为不平等的哥伦比亚,儿童最不可能将领导力赋予有威望的人物。我们认为,包括不平等程度和等级社会组织在内的社会因素形成了一种潜在的倾向,即儿童学会推理等级,并普遍倾向于选择有威望的领导者和学习榜样。
{"title":"Prestige and dominance in egalitarian and hierarchical societies: Children in Finland favor prestige more than children in Colombia or the USA","authors":"Maija-Eliina Sequeira ,&nbsp;Narges Afshordi ,&nbsp;Anni Kajanus","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.05.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined how children reason about dominance and prestige in Colombia, Finland, and the USA, contexts that vary in terms of societal inequality and hierarchical organization. We tested 496 children aged 4–11 years old to determine whether they: i) recognized and discriminated between dominance and prestige, ii) preferred to learn from a dominant or prestigious character, iii) assigned leadership to a dominant or prestigious character, and iv) self-identified more with a dominant or subordinate character. Older children were more likely to recognize, prefer, learn from, and assign leadership to the prestigious character, and to identify with the subordinate. There were no cross-cultural differences in learning preferences, supporting evolutionary theories that posit a universal bias towards social learning from prestigious individuals. There was variation in leadership preferences; children were the most likely to assign leadership to a prestigious character in more egalitarian Finland, and least likely in more unequal Colombia. We argue that societal factors including levels of inequality and hierarchical social organization shape an underlying propensity for children to learn to reason about rank and to broadly favor prestige in leaders and models for learning from.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106591"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000679/pdfft?md5=4bf084df9e4709d3f8c1a4f77125e9dc&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000679-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cash, crowds, and cooperation: The effects of population density and resource scarcity on cooperation in the dictator game 现金、人群与合作:独裁者游戏中人口密度和资源稀缺性对合作的影响
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.009
Lynn K.L. Tan, Norman P. Li, Kenneth Tan

The adaptive benefits of cooperation among humans have been widely studied. However, is being cooperative always adaptive across various combinations of ecological conditions? Existing work has focused on cultural, inter-, and intra-individual predictors of cooperation yet there is a lack of research on how an individual's ecology may come into play. In this work, we focus on the interaction of two ecological factors—population density and resource scarcity—on cooperation. Population density is often accompanied by social competition for limited resources. We hypothesise that in response to cues of high (versus low) population density, people facing resource-scarcity would adaptively lower their cooperativeness, more so than those with resource abundance. Results from two studies support our hypothesis—population density lowers cooperation, but only for people who perceive lower resources or social status. Our findings provide insights that cooperation varies adaptively as a function of interacting ecological factors.

人类合作的适应性益处已被广泛研究。然而,在不同的生态条件组合下,合作是否总是适应性的?现有研究主要关注合作的文化、个体间和个体内预测因素,但缺乏对个体生态环境如何发挥作用的研究。在这项工作中,我们重点研究了两个生态因素--人口密度和资源稀缺性--对合作的相互作用。人口密度通常伴随着对有限资源的社会竞争。我们假设,面对高(相对于低)人口密度的提示,资源稀缺的人会适应性地降低合作性,比资源丰富的人更甚。两项研究的结果支持了我们的假设--种群密度会降低合作性,但只有那些认为资源或社会地位较低的人才会降低合作性。我们的研究结果为我们提供了启示,即合作会随着生态因素的相互作用而发生适应性变化。
{"title":"Cash, crowds, and cooperation: The effects of population density and resource scarcity on cooperation in the dictator game","authors":"Lynn K.L. Tan,&nbsp;Norman P. Li,&nbsp;Kenneth Tan","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.009","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The adaptive benefits of cooperation among humans have been widely studied. However, is being cooperative always adaptive across various combinations of ecological conditions? Existing work has focused on cultural, inter-, and intra-individual predictors of cooperation yet there is a lack of research on how an individual's ecology may come into play. In this work, we focus on the interaction of two ecological factors—population density and resource scarcity—on cooperation. Population density is often accompanied by social competition for limited resources. We hypothesise that in response to cues of high (versus low) population density, people facing resource-scarcity would adaptively lower their cooperativeness, more so than those with resource abundance. Results from two studies support our hypothesis—population density lowers cooperation, but only for people who perceive lower resources or social status. Our findings provide insights that cooperation varies adaptively as a function of interacting ecological factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106581"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Female foragers sometimes hunt, yet gendered divisions of labor are real: a comment on Anderson et al. (2023) The Myth of Man the Hunter 女性狩猎者有时会狩猎,但性别分工确实存在:对安德森等人(2023 年)《猎人的神话》的评论
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.014
Vivek V. Venkataraman , Jordie Hoffman , Kyle Farquharson , Helen Elizabeth Davis , Edward H. Hagen , Raymond B. Hames , Barry S. Hewlett , Luke Glowacki , Haneul Jang , Robert Kelly , Karen Kramer , Sheina Lew-Levy , Katie Starkweather , Kristen Syme , Duncan N.E. Stibbard-Hawkes

Gendered divisions of labor are a feature of every known contemporary hunter-gatherer (forager) society. While gender roles are certainly flexible, and prominent and well-studied cases of female hunting do exist, it is more often men who hunt. A new study (Anderson et al., 2023) surveyed ethnographically known foragers and found that women hunt in 79% of foraging societies, with big-game hunting occurring in 33%. Based on this single type of labor, which is one among dozens performed in foraging societies, the authors question the existence of gendered division of labor altogether. As a diverse group of hunter-gatherer experts, we find that claims that foraging societies lack or have weak gendered divisions of labor are contradicted by empirical evidence. We conducted an in-depth examination of the data and methods of Anderson et al. (2023), finding evidence of sample selection bias and numerous coding errors undermining the paper's conclusions. Anderson et al. (2023) have started a useful dialogue to ameliorate the potential misconception that women never hunt. However, their analysis does not contradict the wide body of empirical evidence for gendered divisions of labor in foraging societies. Furthermore, a myopic focus on hunting diminishes the value of contributions that take different forms and downplays the trade-offs foragers of both sexes routinely face. We caution against ethnographic revisionism that projects Westernized conceptions of labor and its value onto foraging societies.

性别分工是每一个已知的当代狩猎-采集(狩猎者)社会的特征。虽然性别角色肯定是灵活的,而且确实存在女性狩猎的突出案例和经过充分研究的案例,但更多时候是男性狩猎。一项新的研究(安德森等人,2023 年)对人种学上已知的狩猎者进行了调查,发现在 79% 的狩猎社会中都有女性狩猎,其中 33% 的狩猎者狩猎大型猎物。狩猎只是觅食社会中几十种劳动中的一种,基于这单一的劳动类型,作者对性别分工的存在提出了质疑。作为一个多元化的狩猎采集专家群体,我们发现,关于狩猎社会缺乏性别分工或性别分工薄弱的说法与经验证据相矛盾。我们对安德森等人(2023 年)的数据和方法进行了深入研究,发现了样本选择偏差和大量编码错误的证据,从而削弱了该论文的结论。安德森等人(2023 年)发起了一场有益的对话,以改善 "女性从不狩猎 "这一潜在误解。然而,他们的分析与大量关于狩猎社会中性别分工的经验证据并不矛盾。此外,只关注狩猎会降低不同形式贡献的价值,并淡化男女狩猎者经常面临的取舍。我们要警惕人种学修正主义,不要把西方化的劳动概念及其价值强加给狩猎社会。
{"title":"Female foragers sometimes hunt, yet gendered divisions of labor are real: a comment on Anderson et al. (2023) The Myth of Man the Hunter","authors":"Vivek V. Venkataraman ,&nbsp;Jordie Hoffman ,&nbsp;Kyle Farquharson ,&nbsp;Helen Elizabeth Davis ,&nbsp;Edward H. Hagen ,&nbsp;Raymond B. Hames ,&nbsp;Barry S. Hewlett ,&nbsp;Luke Glowacki ,&nbsp;Haneul Jang ,&nbsp;Robert Kelly ,&nbsp;Karen Kramer ,&nbsp;Sheina Lew-Levy ,&nbsp;Katie Starkweather ,&nbsp;Kristen Syme ,&nbsp;Duncan N.E. Stibbard-Hawkes","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.04.014","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Gendered divisions of labor are a feature of every known contemporary hunter-gatherer (forager) society. While gender roles are certainly flexible, and prominent and well-studied cases of female hunting do exist, it is more often men who hunt. A new study (<span>Anderson et al., 2023</span>) surveyed ethnographically known foragers and found that women hunt in 79% of foraging societies, with big-game hunting occurring in 33%. Based on this single type of labor, which is one among dozens performed in foraging societies, the authors question the existence of gendered division of labor altogether. As a diverse group of hunter-gatherer experts, we find that claims that foraging societies lack or have weak gendered divisions of labor are contradicted by empirical evidence. We conducted an in-depth examination of the data and methods of <span>Anderson et al. (2023)</span>, finding evidence of sample selection bias and numerous coding errors undermining the paper's conclusions. <span>Anderson et al. (2023)</span> have started a useful dialogue to ameliorate the potential misconception that women never hunt. However, their analysis does not contradict the wide body of empirical evidence for gendered divisions of labor in foraging societies. Furthermore, a myopic focus on hunting diminishes the value of contributions that take different forms and downplays the trade-offs foragers of both sexes routinely face. We caution against ethnographic revisionism that projects Westernized conceptions of labor and its value onto foraging societies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106586"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141030576","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
US adults accurately assess Hadza and Tsimane men's hunting ability from a single face photograph 美国成年人通过一张脸部照片准确评估哈扎人和齐马内人的狩猎能力
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106598
Adar B. Eisenbruch , Kristopher M. Smith , Clifford I. Workman , Christopher von Rueden , Coren L. Apicella

Trait inferences from faces are pervasive, but sometimes misleading. Past research indicates Americans infer hunting and gathering ability from others' faces, but the accuracy of these perceptions remains unknown. In three studies, we test whether Americans can accurately perceive foraging ability from faces. We used three datasets from two traditional subsistence societies (the Hadza and the Tsimane) in which individuals were photographed and evaluated by their peers on their ability to hunt or gather effectively (N = 175). US MTurkers (N = 579) then evaluated the photos for foraging ability. We found that MTurkers' perceptions of men consistently tracked peer-evaluated hunting ability (overall r = 0.25), suggesting that naïve perceptions of men's productivity from a face photo alone reflect actual hunting ability. MTurkers' perceptions of women's productivity inversely correlated with their peer-evaluated gathering ability, however. We discuss potential mechanisms and implications for research on social perception.

从面孔推断特质是一种普遍现象,但有时会产生误导。过去的研究表明,美国人可以从他人的面孔中推断出狩猎和采集能力,但这些推断的准确性仍然不得而知。在三项研究中,我们测试了美国人是否能从面孔中准确感知觅食能力。我们使用了来自两个传统自给自足社会(哈德扎人和齐马内人)的三个数据集,在这三个数据集中,个体被拍照并由同伴评价其有效狩猎或采集的能力(N = 175)。然后,美国 MTurkers(N = 579)对这些照片的觅食能力进行评估。我们发现, MTurkers 对男性的感知始终与同伴评价的狩猎能力相一致(总体 r = 0.25),这表明仅从脸部照片对男性生产力的天真感知反映了实际的狩猎能力。然而,MTurkers 对女性生产力的看法与其同伴评价的采集能力成反比。我们讨论了社会认知研究的潜在机制和意义。
{"title":"US adults accurately assess Hadza and Tsimane men's hunting ability from a single face photograph","authors":"Adar B. Eisenbruch ,&nbsp;Kristopher M. Smith ,&nbsp;Clifford I. Workman ,&nbsp;Christopher von Rueden ,&nbsp;Coren L. Apicella","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106598","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106598","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Trait inferences from faces are pervasive, but sometimes misleading. Past research indicates Americans infer hunting and gathering ability from others' faces, but the accuracy of these perceptions remains unknown. In three studies, we test whether Americans can accurately perceive foraging ability from faces. We used three datasets from two traditional subsistence societies (the Hadza and the Tsimane) in which individuals were photographed and evaluated by their peers on their ability to hunt or gather effectively (<em>N</em> = 175). US MTurkers (<em>N</em> = 579) then evaluated the photos for foraging ability. We found that MTurkers' perceptions of men consistently tracked peer-evaluated hunting ability (overall <em>r</em> = 0.25), suggesting that naïve perceptions of men's productivity from a face photo alone reflect actual hunting ability. MTurkers' perceptions of women's productivity inversely correlated with their peer-evaluated gathering ability, however. We discuss potential mechanisms and implications for research on social perception.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 4","pages":"Article 106598"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141480992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adult knowledge of wild plants associated with limited delayed health and nutritional benefits for children or adults in the face of external change: A yearly panel (2003−2010) study among Tsimane’, an indigenous Amazonian society in Bolivia 面对外部变化,成人对野生植物的了解与儿童或成人有限的延迟健康和营养益处有关:对玻利维亚亚马逊土著社会 Tsimane'的年度小组研究(2003-2010 年
IF 3 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106594
Ricardo Godoy , Tomás Huanca , William R. Leonard , Thomas McDade , Victoria Reyes-García , Asher Y. Rosinger , Susan Tanner

Cross-sectional studies suggest that local ecological knowledge (LEK) helps humans cope with their environment. Among the forms of LEK, adult knowledge of wild plants has been associated with better child and adult health. We assess if the concurrent links between i) LEK and ii) health and nutritional status last and examine if LEK yields delayed benefits when societies face large socioeconomic and environmental changes. We use a yearly panel (2002−2010) from Tsimane’, an Indigenous Amazonian society (Bolivia). All adults (∼440) and children (∼300) measured at baseline (2003) in 13 villages were followed yearly during 2004–2010 to estimate associations between a) baseline adult knowledge and skill about uses of wild plants and b) subsequent (2004–2010) anthropometric markers of nutritional status of themselves and the children (2y ≤ age ≤ 10y) living in the household at baseline. Among children, HAZ, BMI, and sum of four skinfolds were measured; among adults, BMI, sum of four skinfolds, and percent body fat with bioelectrical impedance were measured. Some skill losses increased by a large amount the likelihood of severe childhood stunting (HAZ < -3) for girls; the complete loss of these skills increased the share of severely stunted girls from 5% to 13%–20%. These are big numbers. The effects of LEK on other anthropometric indicators of children or adults were small. For example, if all adults in a household lost all their ethnobotanical knowledge, children's and women's BMI would decrease by only 3% and 11%, respectively.

横断面研究表明,地方生态知识(LEK)有助于人类应对环境。在各种形式的地方生态知识中,成人对野生植物的了解与儿童和成人健康状况的改善有关。我们评估了 i) 本地生态知识与 ii) 健康和营养状况之间的并发联系是否持续,并研究了当社会面临巨大的社会经济和环境变化时,本地生态知识是否会产生延迟效益。我们使用了来自亚马逊土著社会(玻利维亚)Tsimane'的年度面板数据(2002-2010 年)。在 2004-2010 年期间,我们每年对 13 个村庄的所有成年人(440 人)和儿童(300 人)进行基线(2003 年)测量,以估算 a) 基线成年人对野生植物利用的知识和技能与 b) 随后(2004-2010 年)基线时居住在该家庭中的成年人和儿童(2 岁 ≤ 10 岁)营养状况的人体测量指标之间的关系。在儿童中,测量了 HAZ、体重指数和四个皮褶的总和;在成人中,测量了体重指数、四个皮褶的总和以及生物电阻抗体脂百分比。一些技能的丧失使女孩出现严重儿童发育迟缓(HAZ < -3)的可能性大大增加;这些技能的完全丧失使严重发育迟缓女孩的比例从 5%增加到 13%-20%。这些都是很大的数字。低地生活技能对儿童或成人其他人体测量指标的影响很小。例如,如果一个家庭的所有成年人都失去了所有的民族植物学知识,那么儿童和妇女的体重指数将分别下降 3% 和 11%。
{"title":"Adult knowledge of wild plants associated with limited delayed health and nutritional benefits for children or adults in the face of external change: A yearly panel (2003−2010) study among Tsimane’, an indigenous Amazonian society in Bolivia","authors":"Ricardo Godoy ,&nbsp;Tomás Huanca ,&nbsp;William R. Leonard ,&nbsp;Thomas McDade ,&nbsp;Victoria Reyes-García ,&nbsp;Asher Y. Rosinger ,&nbsp;Susan Tanner","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106594","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106594","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cross-sectional studies suggest that local ecological knowledge (LEK) helps humans cope with their environment. Among the forms of LEK, adult knowledge of wild plants has been associated with better child and adult health. We assess if the concurrent links between i) LEK and ii) health and nutritional status last and examine if LEK yields delayed benefits when societies face large socioeconomic and environmental changes. We use a yearly panel (2002−2010) from Tsimane’, an Indigenous Amazonian society (Bolivia). All adults (∼440) and children (∼300) measured at baseline (2003) in 13 villages were followed yearly during 2004–2010 to estimate associations between a) baseline adult knowledge and skill about uses of wild plants and b) subsequent (2004–2010) anthropometric markers of nutritional status of themselves and the children (2y ≤ age ≤ 10y) living in the household at baseline. Among children, HAZ, BMI, and sum of four skinfolds were measured; among adults, BMI, sum of four skinfolds, and percent body fat with bioelectrical impedance were measured. Some skill losses increased by a large amount the likelihood of severe childhood stunting (HAZ &lt; -3) for girls; the complete loss of these skills increased the share of severely stunted girls from 5% to 13%–20%. These are big numbers. The effects of LEK on other anthropometric indicators of children or adults were small. For example, if all adults in a household lost all their ethnobotanical knowledge, children's and women's BMI would decrease by only 3% and 11%, respectively.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"45 5","pages":"Article 106594"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513824000709/pdfft?md5=a696e41f117ecaf6044cc34f2ed607ee&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513824000709-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141689810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1