首页 > 最新文献

Language and cognitive processes最新文献

英文 中文
Response exclusion in word–word tasks: A comment on Roelofs, Piai and Schriefers 词-词任务中的反应排斥:对Roelofs、Piai和Schriefers的评析
Pub Date : 2013-05-22 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.746715
Niels Janssen
Roelofs, Piai, and Schriefers discuss a series of results obtained from various word reading tasks in the context of word and picture distractors. They argue that these results support WEAVER++, a computational model of word production that assumes a competitive lexical selection mechanism, and challenge the response exclusion hypothesis, a model that assumes a non-competitive lexical selection mechanism. At odds with this claim, I argue here that these data do not pose problems for the response exclusion hypothesis. I also discuss new avenues of research to advance the debate on the mechanism of lexical selection.
Roelofs, Piai和Schriefers讨论了在文字和图片干扰的情况下,从各种单词阅读任务中获得的一系列结果。他们认为,这些结果支持WEAVER++,一个假设竞争性词汇选择机制的单词生成计算模型,并挑战了反应排斥假说,一个假设非竞争性词汇选择机制的模型。与这种说法不一致的是,我在这里认为,这些数据并没有对反应排除假说构成问题。本文还讨论了新的研究途径,以促进对词汇选择机制的争论。
{"title":"Response exclusion in word–word tasks: A comment on Roelofs, Piai and Schriefers","authors":"Niels Janssen","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.746715","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.746715","url":null,"abstract":"Roelofs, Piai, and Schriefers discuss a series of results obtained from various word reading tasks in the context of word and picture distractors. They argue that these results support WEAVER++, a computational model of word production that assumes a competitive lexical selection mechanism, and challenge the response exclusion hypothesis, a model that assumes a non-competitive lexical selection mechanism. At odds with this claim, I argue here that these data do not pose problems for the response exclusion hypothesis. I also discuss new avenues of research to advance the debate on the mechanism of lexical selection.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.746715","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135653","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Context effects and selective attention in picture naming and word reading: Competition versus response exclusion 情境效应与图片命名和单词阅读中的选择性注意:竞争与反应排斥
Pub Date : 2013-05-22 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.615663
A. Roelofs, Vitória Piai, H. Schriefers
For several decades, context effects in picture naming and word reading have been extensively investigated. However, researchers have found no agreement on the explanation of the effects. Whereas it has long been assumed that several types of effect reflect competition in word selection, recently it has been argued that these effects reflect the exclusion of articulatory responses from an output buffer. Here, we first critically evaluate the findings on context effects in picture naming that have been taken as evidence against the competition account, and we argue that the findings are, in fact, compatible with the competition account. Moreover, some of the findings appear to challenge rather than support the response exclusion account. Next, we compare the response exclusion and competition accounts with respect to their ability to explain data on word reading. It appears that response exclusion does not account well for context effects on word reading times, whereas computer simulations reveal that a competition model like WEAVER++ accounts for the findings.
几十年来,语境效应在图片命名和单词阅读中的作用得到了广泛的研究。然而,研究人员对这种影响的解释没有达成一致。长期以来,人们一直认为有几种类型的效应反映了词语选择中的竞争,但最近有人认为,这些效应反映了输出缓冲中发音反应的排除。在这里,我们首先批判性地评估了关于图片命名中的上下文效应的发现,这些发现被作为反对竞争说的证据,我们认为这些发现实际上与竞争说是相容的。此外,一些研究结果似乎挑战而不是支持反应排除说。接下来,我们比较了反应排除和竞争帐户在解释单词阅读数据方面的能力。反应排斥似乎不能很好地解释上下文对单词阅读时间的影响,而计算机模拟显示,像WEAVER++这样的竞争模型解释了这一发现。
{"title":"Context effects and selective attention in picture naming and word reading: Competition versus response exclusion","authors":"A. Roelofs, Vitória Piai, H. Schriefers","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.615663","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.615663","url":null,"abstract":"For several decades, context effects in picture naming and word reading have been extensively investigated. However, researchers have found no agreement on the explanation of the effects. Whereas it has long been assumed that several types of effect reflect competition in word selection, recently it has been argued that these effects reflect the exclusion of articulatory responses from an output buffer. Here, we first critically evaluate the findings on context effects in picture naming that have been taken as evidence against the competition account, and we argue that the findings are, in fact, compatible with the competition account. Moreover, some of the findings appear to challenge rather than support the response exclusion account. Next, we compare the response exclusion and competition accounts with respect to their ability to explain data on word reading. It appears that response exclusion does not account well for context effects on word reading times, whereas computer simulations reveal that a competition model like WEAVER++ accounts for the findings.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.615663","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135003","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 51
Explaining semantic facilitation and interference effects in the picture–word interference task—A rejoinder to Navarrete and Mahon (2013) 图词干扰任务中语义促进和干扰效应的解释——对Navarrete and Mahon(2013)的回应
Pub Date : 2013-05-22 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2013.770891
A. Mädebach, A. Hantsch
In our original article, we presented a picture–word interference experiment in which pictures had to be named with basic level names, although their naming preference was at the subordinate level (e.g., picture: rose, response: flower). We observed semantic interference for distractor words denoting the subordinate name of the picture, but no effect for distractor words denoting another subordinate name corresponding to the same basic level. We argued that this pattern is inconsistent with the assumption that semantic interference effects in picture–word interference tasks are caused by a post-lexical response-exclusion mechanism which operates over coarse semantic properties of distractor words, but that lexical competition models can account for these results. In their commentary on our article, Navarrete and Mahon come to the opposite conclusion. Here we are addressing the principal objections raised by Navarrete and Mahon, regarding the interpretation of our results. We elaborate on our view that lexical competition models can account for our data and agree that a response-exclusion mechanism, which is part of a general monitoring system and operates over fine-grained semantic information, would be compatible with our data.
在我们最初的文章中,我们提出了一个图片-单词干扰实验,在这个实验中,尽管图片的命名偏好是在从属层次(例如,图片:玫瑰,反应:花),但必须用基本层次的名称来命名图片。我们观察到,表示图片从属名称的干扰词存在语义干扰,而表示同一基本水平对应的另一个从属名称的干扰词不存在语义干扰。我们认为,这种模式与图词干扰任务中的语义干扰效应是由对干扰词的粗糙语义特性起作用的词汇后反应排斥机制引起的假设不一致,但词汇竞争模型可以解释这些结果。在他们对我们文章的评论中,Navarrete和Mahon得出了相反的结论。在这里,我们将讨论Navarrete和Mahon对我们的结果的解释提出的主要反对意见。我们详细阐述了我们的观点,即词汇竞争模型可以解释我们的数据,并同意响应排除机制(作为一般监控系统的一部分,在细粒度语义信息上运行)将与我们的数据兼容。
{"title":"Explaining semantic facilitation and interference effects in the picture–word interference task—A rejoinder to Navarrete and Mahon (2013)","authors":"A. Mädebach, A. Hantsch","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2013.770891","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.770891","url":null,"abstract":"In our original article, we presented a picture–word interference experiment in which pictures had to be named with basic level names, although their naming preference was at the subordinate level (e.g., picture: rose, response: flower). We observed semantic interference for distractor words denoting the subordinate name of the picture, but no effect for distractor words denoting another subordinate name corresponding to the same basic level. We argued that this pattern is inconsistent with the assumption that semantic interference effects in picture–word interference tasks are caused by a post-lexical response-exclusion mechanism which operates over coarse semantic properties of distractor words, but that lexical competition models can account for these results. In their commentary on our article, Navarrete and Mahon come to the opposite conclusion. Here we are addressing the principal objections raised by Navarrete and Mahon, regarding the interpretation of our results. We elaborate on our view that lexical competition models can account for our data and agree that a response-exclusion mechanism, which is part of a general monitoring system and operates over fine-grained semantic information, would be compatible with our data.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2013.770891","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Is lexical selection in spoken word production competitive? Introduction to the special issue on lexical competition in language production 口语词汇生成中的词汇选择是否具有竞争性?语言生产中的词汇竞争专题导论
Pub Date : 2013-05-22 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.718088
K. Spalek, M. Damian, Jens Bölte
A common assumption in research on spoken word production is that lexical selection is a competition-based process among co-activated lexical representations. This assumption of competitive lexical selection has been challenged by an alternative account, which places competition at a postlexical response selection stage. A complex pattern of empirical findings from picture-word interference and other tasks has emerged which constrains current thinking about word production. In this article we provide an overview over the main positions, empirical findings, and put the various contributions to this Special Issue into a wider context. The theoretical debate is far from closed, but it has drawn attention to some critical points that we emphasise in this Editorial: The speech production process needs an element of competition, but this competition need not necessarily take place during lexical selection. Behavioural interference effects are caused by a combination of facilitation and interference, but there is discord about the processing levels at which these mechanisms are located. Finally, we stress the necessity to use findings from different experimental paradigms for theory-building and advocate a shift from the narrow focus on the picture-word paradigm that has dominated the theoretical discussion in this Special Issue and elsewhere.
口语词汇产生研究中的一个普遍假设是,词汇选择是协同激活的词汇表征之间基于竞争的过程。这种竞争性词汇选择的假设受到了另一种说法的挑战,该说法认为竞争处于词汇后反应选择阶段。从图片-单词干扰和其他任务中得出的经验发现的复杂模式已经出现,这限制了当前对单词生成的思考。在这篇文章中,我们概述了主要立场,实证研究结果,并将本期特刊的各种贡献置于更广泛的背景下。理论辩论远未结束,但它引起了我们在这篇社论中强调的一些关键点的注意:语音产生过程需要竞争的元素,但这种竞争不一定发生在词汇选择过程中。行为干扰效应是由促进和干扰的结合引起的,但在这些机制所在的处理水平上存在不一致。最后,我们强调有必要使用来自不同实验范式的发现来构建理论,并主张从狭隘地关注图片-单词范式的转变,这种范式在本期特刊和其他地方主导了理论讨论。
{"title":"Is lexical selection in spoken word production competitive? Introduction to the special issue on lexical competition in language production","authors":"K. Spalek, M. Damian, Jens Bölte","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.718088","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.718088","url":null,"abstract":"A common assumption in research on spoken word production is that lexical selection is a competition-based process among co-activated lexical representations. This assumption of competitive lexical selection has been challenged by an alternative account, which places competition at a postlexical response selection stage. A complex pattern of empirical findings from picture-word interference and other tasks has emerged which constrains current thinking about word production. In this article we provide an overview over the main positions, empirical findings, and put the various contributions to this Special Issue into a wider context. The theoretical debate is far from closed, but it has drawn attention to some critical points that we emphasise in this Editorial: The speech production process needs an element of competition, but this competition need not necessarily take place during lexical selection. Behavioural interference effects are caused by a combination of facilitation and interference, but there is discord about the processing levels at which these mechanisms are located. Finally, we stress the necessity to use findings from different experimental paradigms for theory-building and advocate a shift from the narrow focus on the picture-word paradigm that has dominated the theoretical discussion in this Special Issue and elsewhere.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.718088","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 41
What does the articulatory output buffer know about alternative picture names? Evidence against the response-exclusion hypothesis 发音输出缓冲区对备选图片名称了解多少?反对反应排斥假说的证据
Pub Date : 2013-05-22 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.595725
A. Hantsch, A. Mädebach
When participants name pictures in the presence of a distractor word, a semantic relation between distractor word and picture name interferes with the naming response. Some models take this to reflect a lexical-competition process, while other models assume it to result from a postlexical response-exclusion mechanism. According to the latter view, the distractor word has privileged access to an articulatory output buffer and has to be purged from it before the picture name can be produced. This buffer is assumed to have access to information that is relevant within a given task such as gross semantic category information. Any (semantic) similarity between the picture name and the distractor word then should render removal of the distractor more difficult and thus prolong naming latencies. However, more fine-grained semantic information is not accessible to the articulatory output buffer and, thus, should not affect naming performance. We tested this assumption by comparing the effect of two semantic distractor conditions keeping the semantic relation between distractor words and the to-be-produced (basic-level) picture names constant, while manipulating only the relation between the distractor and the pictures' subordinate-level name. Contrary to the predictions of the response-exclusion hypothesis, this manipulation determined whether or not semantic interference was obtained.
当被试在有干扰词的情况下命名图片时,干扰词和图片名称之间的语义关系干扰了命名反应。一些模型认为这反映了词汇竞争过程,而其他模型则认为这是词汇后反应排斥机制的结果。根据后一种观点,干扰词对发音输出缓冲器有特权访问,必须在产生图片名称之前从缓冲器中清除。假定该缓冲区可以访问给定任务中相关的信息,例如总体语义类别信息。图片名称和干扰词之间的任何(语义)相似性都应该使移除干扰词变得更加困难,从而延长命名延迟。但是,发音输出缓冲区无法访问更细粒度的语义信息,因此不应影响命名性能。我们通过比较两种语义干扰条件的效果来验证这一假设,即保持干扰词与待生成(基本级)图片名称之间的语义关系不变,而只操纵干扰词与图片的从属级名称之间的关系。与反应排斥假说的预测相反,这种操作决定了是否获得语义干扰。
{"title":"What does the articulatory output buffer know about alternative picture names? Evidence against the response-exclusion hypothesis","authors":"A. Hantsch, A. Mädebach","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.595725","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.595725","url":null,"abstract":"When participants name pictures in the presence of a distractor word, a semantic relation between distractor word and picture name interferes with the naming response. Some models take this to reflect a lexical-competition process, while other models assume it to result from a postlexical response-exclusion mechanism. According to the latter view, the distractor word has privileged access to an articulatory output buffer and has to be purged from it before the picture name can be produced. This buffer is assumed to have access to information that is relevant within a given task such as gross semantic category information. Any (semantic) similarity between the picture name and the distractor word then should render removal of the distractor more difficult and thus prolong naming latencies. However, more fine-grained semantic information is not accessible to the articulatory output buffer and, thus, should not affect naming performance. We tested this assumption by comparing the effect of two semantic distractor conditions keeping the semantic relation between distractor words and the to-be-produced (basic-level) picture names constant, while manipulating only the relation between the distractor and the pictures' subordinate-level name. Contrary to the predictions of the response-exclusion hypothesis, this manipulation determined whether or not semantic interference was obtained.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.595725","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59134513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Event-related brain potential evidence that local nouns affect subject–verb agreement processing 局部名词影响主谓一致处理的事件相关脑电位证据
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.650900
Erica Y. Shen, A. Staub, Lisa D. Sanders
The conditions under which speakers make syntactic errors, and the manner in which listeners respond to them, provide insight into how complex computational problems related to the rules of language are solved. One of the important syntactic rules of English is that a subject and its corresponding verb must agree in number. However, the presence of a number-bearing element between the subject and verb results in frequent production errors and has also been shown to complicate comprehension. When asked to press a button in response to anomalies in several narrated short stories, participants in the current study were better able to detect subject–verb agreement violations when there were no intervening words. In a separate event-related potential (ERP) experiment in which participants listened to the same stories for comprehension, simple subject–verb agreement violations elicited the predicted anterior negativity and later posterior positivity (P600). In contrast, when a singular noun phrase appeared between a singular subject and the corresponding verb, agreement violations elicited an early negativity with a distinctly posterior distribution. When a singular subject was followed by a plural noun phrase, there were no differences evident in ERPs elicited by singular and plural verbs. These results indicate that during comprehension of natural speech, the computation of subject–verb agreement is affected by the presence of number-bearing elements other than the subject itself.
说话者犯句法错误的情况,以及听者对句法错误的反应方式,让我们了解与语言规则相关的复杂计算问题是如何解决的。英语中一个重要的句法规则是主语和动词在数量上必须一致。然而,在主语和动词之间存在数词元素会导致频繁的生产错误,也会使理解变得复杂。当被要求按下一个按钮来回应几个叙述短篇故事中的异常情况时,在目前的研究中,当没有干预词时,参与者能够更好地发现主-动词一致的违反。在一个单独的事件相关电位(ERP)实验中,参与者听同样的故事来理解,简单的主-动词一致违反引发了预测的前负性和后正性(P600)。相反,当一个单数名词短语出现在一个单数主语和相应的动词之间时,违背协议引发了一个明显后向分布的早期否定。当单主语后接复数名词短语时,单动词和复数动词引发的erp无明显差异。这些结果表明,在自然语音理解过程中,主谓一致性的计算受主语本身以外的数词元素的影响。
{"title":"Event-related brain potential evidence that local nouns affect subject–verb agreement processing","authors":"Erica Y. Shen, A. Staub, Lisa D. Sanders","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.650900","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.650900","url":null,"abstract":"The conditions under which speakers make syntactic errors, and the manner in which listeners respond to them, provide insight into how complex computational problems related to the rules of language are solved. One of the important syntactic rules of English is that a subject and its corresponding verb must agree in number. However, the presence of a number-bearing element between the subject and verb results in frequent production errors and has also been shown to complicate comprehension. When asked to press a button in response to anomalies in several narrated short stories, participants in the current study were better able to detect subject–verb agreement violations when there were no intervening words. In a separate event-related potential (ERP) experiment in which participants listened to the same stories for comprehension, simple subject–verb agreement violations elicited the predicted anterior negativity and later posterior positivity (P600). In contrast, when a singular noun phrase appeared between a singular subject and the corresponding verb, agreement violations elicited an early negativity with a distinctly posterior distribution. When a singular subject was followed by a plural noun phrase, there were no differences evident in ERPs elicited by singular and plural verbs. These results indicate that during comprehension of natural speech, the computation of subject–verb agreement is affected by the presence of number-bearing elements other than the subject itself.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.650900","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59134961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 29
Semantic category moderates phonological priming of proper name retrieval during tip-of-the-tongue states 语义范畴调节舌尖状态下专有名称检索的语音启动
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.658408
Katherine K. White, L. Abrams, Elizabeth A. Frame
Despite evidence that the majority of tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states occur for proper names, little research has investigated factors that influence their resolution. Although phonological primes typically increase TOT resolution, the present experiment investigated whether priming effects are mitigated by semantic competition. Participants read questions whose answers were proper name targets (e.g., Helen Hunt, Elton John) from various semantic categories (e.g., actor, musician). Following a TOT, another question was presented that either included a prime name that varied in phonological overlap with the target (full first name or first syllable) and semantic category (same profession, different profession) or was phonologically and semantically unrelated to the target. After presenting the target question a second time, participants were more likely to resolve TOTs following first-name primes than unrelated names, independent of semantic category. In contrast, first-syllable primes marginally facilitated TOT resolution when the prime was in a different semantic category but not when the prime was in the same semantic category. These results demonstrate that semantic overlap increases competition from phonologically related names when there is incomplete phonological input, allowing an alternative name to prevent TOT resolution.
尽管有证据表明,大多数的舌尖状态(TOT)发生在专有名称上,但很少有研究调查影响其解决的因素。虽然语音启动通常会增加TOT分辨率,但本实验研究了语义竞争是否会减轻启动效应。参与者从不同的语义类别(如演员、音乐家)中阅读答案为专有名称目标(如海伦·亨特、埃尔顿·约翰)的问题。在TOT之后,提出另一个问题,其中包括一个与目标(全名或第一个音节)和语义类别(相同职业,不同职业)在语音和语义上不同的启动名,或者与目标在语音和语义上不相关。在第二次呈现目标问题后,参与者更有可能解决名字后的tot,而不是不相关的名字,独立于语义类别。相比之下,当启动词在不同的语义范畴时,首音节启动词略微促进了TOT的解决,而当启动词在同一语义范畴时,则没有。这些结果表明,当存在不完整的语音输入时,语义重叠会增加语音相关名称的竞争,从而允许替代名称阻止TOT解析。
{"title":"Semantic category moderates phonological priming of proper name retrieval during tip-of-the-tongue states","authors":"Katherine K. White, L. Abrams, Elizabeth A. Frame","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.658408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.658408","url":null,"abstract":"Despite evidence that the majority of tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states occur for proper names, little research has investigated factors that influence their resolution. Although phonological primes typically increase TOT resolution, the present experiment investigated whether priming effects are mitigated by semantic competition. Participants read questions whose answers were proper name targets (e.g., Helen Hunt, Elton John) from various semantic categories (e.g., actor, musician). Following a TOT, another question was presented that either included a prime name that varied in phonological overlap with the target (full first name or first syllable) and semantic category (same profession, different profession) or was phonologically and semantically unrelated to the target. After presenting the target question a second time, participants were more likely to resolve TOTs following first-name primes than unrelated names, independent of semantic category. In contrast, first-syllable primes marginally facilitated TOT resolution when the prime was in a different semantic category but not when the prime was in the same semantic category. These results demonstrate that semantic overlap increases competition from phonologically related names when there is incomplete phonological input, allowing an alternative name to prevent TOT resolution.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.658408","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135080","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 20
Investigating the timecourse of accessing conversational implicatures during incremental sentence interpretation 渐进式句子解释中会话含义获取的时间进程研究
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.649040
R. Breheny, H. Ferguson, N. Katsos
Many contextual inferences in utterance interpretation are explained as following from the nature of conversation and the assumption that participants are rational. Recent psycholinguistic research has focused on certain of these “Gricean” inferences and have revealed that comprehenders can access them in online interpretation. However, there have been mixed results as to the time course of access. Some results show that Gricean inferences can be accessed very rapidly, as rapidly as any other contextually specified information; while other studies looking at the same kind of inference suggest that access to Gricean inferences are delayed relative to other aspects of semantic interpretation. While previous timecourse research has focused on Gricean inferences that support the online assignment of reference to definite expressions, the study reported here examines the timecourse of access to scalar implicatures, which enrich the meaning of an utterance beyond the semantic interpretation. Even if access to Gricean inference in support of reference assignment may be rapid, it is still unknown whether genuinely enriching scalar implicatures are delayed. Our results indicate that scalar implicatures are accessed as rapidly as other contextual inferences. The implications of our results are discussed in reference to the architecture of language comprehension.
话语解释中的许多语境推理可以从对话的性质和参与者是理性的假设中得到解释。最近的心理语言学研究集中在这些“格赖斯”推理上,并揭示了理解者可以在在线口译中使用它们。然而,关于进入的时间进程,结果好坏参半。一些结果表明,Gricean推理可以非常快速地访问,与任何其他上下文指定的信息一样快;而其他研究同样类型推理的研究表明,相对于语义解释的其他方面,对Gricean推理的获取是延迟的。虽然之前的时间过程研究主要集中在支持对确定表达的在线引用分配的Gricean推理上,但本文的研究考察了对标量含义的访问时间过程,它丰富了语义解释之外的话语意义。即使支持引用赋值的Gricean推理的访问可能很快,但真正丰富的标量隐含是否被延迟仍然是未知的。我们的结果表明,标量蕴涵的访问速度与其他上下文推断一样快。我们的研究结果在语言理解结构的基础上进行了讨论。
{"title":"Investigating the timecourse of accessing conversational implicatures during incremental sentence interpretation","authors":"R. Breheny, H. Ferguson, N. Katsos","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.649040","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.649040","url":null,"abstract":"Many contextual inferences in utterance interpretation are explained as following from the nature of conversation and the assumption that participants are rational. Recent psycholinguistic research has focused on certain of these “Gricean” inferences and have revealed that comprehenders can access them in online interpretation. However, there have been mixed results as to the time course of access. Some results show that Gricean inferences can be accessed very rapidly, as rapidly as any other contextually specified information; while other studies looking at the same kind of inference suggest that access to Gricean inferences are delayed relative to other aspects of semantic interpretation. While previous timecourse research has focused on Gricean inferences that support the online assignment of reference to definite expressions, the study reported here examines the timecourse of access to scalar implicatures, which enrich the meaning of an utterance beyond the semantic interpretation. Even if access to Gricean inference in support of reference assignment may be rapid, it is still unknown whether genuinely enriching scalar implicatures are delayed. Our results indicate that scalar implicatures are accessed as rapidly as other contextual inferences. The implications of our results are discussed in reference to the architecture of language comprehension.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.649040","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135403","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 78
The use of verb-specific information for prediction in sentence processing 动词特定信息在句子加工预测中的应用
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.658072
Manabu Arai, Frank Keller
Recent research has shown that language comprehenders make predictions about upcoming linguistic information. These studies demonstrate that the processor not only analyses the input that it received but also predicts upcoming unseen elements. Two visual world experiments were conducted to examine the type of syntactic information this prediction process has access to. Experiment 1 examined whether the verb's subcategorisation information is used for predicting a direct object, by comparing transitive verbs (e.g., punish) to intransitive verbs (e.g., disagree). Experiment 2 examined whether verb frequency information is used for predicting a reduced relative clause by contrasting verbs that are infrequent in the past participle form (e.g., watch) with ones that are frequent in that form (e.g., record). Both experiments showed that comprehenders used lexically specific syntactic information to predict upcoming syntactic structure; this information can be used to avoid garden paths in certain cases, as Experiment 2 demonstrated.
最近的研究表明,语言理解者会对即将到来的语言信息做出预测。这些研究表明,处理器不仅分析它收到的输入,而且还预测即将到来的未见元素。我们进行了两个视觉世界实验来检验这种预测过程所获得的句法信息的类型。实验1通过比较及物动词(如:penalty)和不及物动词(如:disagree),考察动词的子范畴信息是否用于预测直接宾语。实验2通过对比过去分词形式中不常见的动词(如watch)和过去分词形式中经常出现的动词(如record),来检验动词频率信息是否用于预测关系从句。两个实验都表明,理解者使用词汇特异性句法信息来预测即将到来的句法结构;如实验2所示,在某些情况下,可以使用此信息来避开花园路径。
{"title":"The use of verb-specific information for prediction in sentence processing","authors":"Manabu Arai, Frank Keller","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.658072","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.658072","url":null,"abstract":"Recent research has shown that language comprehenders make predictions about upcoming linguistic information. These studies demonstrate that the processor not only analyses the input that it received but also predicts upcoming unseen elements. Two visual world experiments were conducted to examine the type of syntactic information this prediction process has access to. Experiment 1 examined whether the verb's subcategorisation information is used for predicting a direct object, by comparing transitive verbs (e.g., punish) to intransitive verbs (e.g., disagree). Experiment 2 examined whether verb frequency information is used for predicting a reduced relative clause by contrasting verbs that are infrequent in the past participle form (e.g., watch) with ones that are frequent in that form (e.g., record). Both experiments showed that comprehenders used lexically specific syntactic information to predict upcoming syntactic structure; this information can be used to avoid garden paths in certain cases, as Experiment 2 demonstrated.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.658072","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 87
When the fly flied and when the fly flew: How semantics affect the processing of inflected verbs 当苍蝇飞的时候和当苍蝇飞的时候:语义如何影响屈折动词的加工
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.649041
Michael Ramscar, Melody Dye
Although psychological theories of inflectional morphology have traditionally considered phonological and grammatical information to be the only factors affecting inflection, there is ample evidence indicating that semantic information can play a vital role in determining the past-tense forms of homophone verb stems. In this paper, we present two experiments that use on-line measures to test the prediction that semantic context shapes readers' expectations about the past-tense form of an upcoming verb. Consistent with the predictions of “single-route” accounts that model inflection using a uniform process of comparison to stored forms in memory, and contrary to the predictions of theories that posit context-independent rules, semantics are found to strongly influence reaction time data for both irregular and regular verbs, and for both existing and nonce verb forms. At the same time, no dissociation between regular and irregulars is observed, a finding which undercuts “dual-route” arguments for a grammatical constraint on denominal verb inflection. We discuss how these results may be understood in terms of discrimination learning.
虽然屈折形态的心理学理论传统上认为语音和语法信息是影响屈折的唯一因素,但有充分的证据表明,语义信息在决定同音动词词干的过去时形式方面起着至关重要的作用。在本文中,我们提出了两个实验,使用在线测量来测试语义上下文塑造读者对即将到来的动词的过去时形式的期望的预测。与“单一路径”理论的预测一致,即使用统一的过程与记忆中的存储形式进行比较来模拟屈折,与假设上下文独立规则的理论预测相反,语义被发现强烈影响不规则和规则动词的反应时间数据,以及现有和临时动词形式。同时,没有观察到规则和不规则之间的分离,这一发现削弱了对名动词屈折的语法限制的“双重路线”论点。我们讨论了如何从辨别学习的角度来理解这些结果。
{"title":"When the fly flied and when the fly flew: How semantics affect the processing of inflected verbs","authors":"Michael Ramscar, Melody Dye","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.649041","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.649041","url":null,"abstract":"Although psychological theories of inflectional morphology have traditionally considered phonological and grammatical information to be the only factors affecting inflection, there is ample evidence indicating that semantic information can play a vital role in determining the past-tense forms of homophone verb stems. In this paper, we present two experiments that use on-line measures to test the prediction that semantic context shapes readers' expectations about the past-tense form of an upcoming verb. Consistent with the predictions of “single-route” accounts that model inflection using a uniform process of comparison to stored forms in memory, and contrary to the predictions of theories that posit context-independent rules, semantics are found to strongly influence reaction time data for both irregular and regular verbs, and for both existing and nonce verb forms. At the same time, no dissociation between regular and irregulars is observed, a finding which undercuts “dual-route” arguments for a grammatical constraint on denominal verb inflection. We discuss how these results may be understood in terms of discrimination learning.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.649041","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59134952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
期刊
Language and cognitive processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1