首页 > 最新文献

Language and cognitive processes最新文献

英文 中文
The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in reading Chinese: Evidence from ERPs 汉语阅读中语义和句法加工的时间过程:来自erp的证据
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.660169
Suiping Wang, Deyuan Mo, M. Xiang, Ruiping Xu, Hsuan-Chich Chen
The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in reading Chinese was examined by recording event-related brain potentials (ERPs) as native Chinese speakers read individually presented sentences for comprehension and performed semantic plausibility judgments. The transitivity of the verbs in Chinese ba/bei constructions was manipulated to form three types of stimuli: Congruent sentences (CON), sentences with semantic violation (SEM), and sentences with combined semantic and syntactic violation (SEM+SYN). Compared with the critical words in CON, those in SEM and SEM+SYN elicited an N400-P600 biphasic pattern. The N400 effects in both violation conditions were of similar size and distribution, but the P600 in SEM+SYN was bigger than that in SEM. Overall, the lack of a difference between SEM and SEM+SYN in the earlier time window (i.e., N400 window) suggested that syntactic processing in Chinese does not necessarily occur earlier than semantic processing.
通过记录事件相关脑电位(ERPs),研究了汉语母语者在阅读单独呈现的句子进行理解和语义合理性判断时语义和句法加工的时间过程。通过对动词及物性的操纵,形成了三种类型的刺激:一致句(CON)、语义违背句(SEM)和语义与句法结合违背句(SEM+SYN)。与CON中的关键字相比,SEM和SEM+SYN中的关键字引发了N400-P600双相模式。两种违反条件下的N400效应大小和分布相似,但SEM+SYN条件下的P600效应大于SEM条件下的。总体而言,SEM和SEM+SYN在更早的时间窗口(即N400窗口)中没有差异,这表明汉语的句法加工并不一定早于语义加工。
{"title":"The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in reading Chinese: Evidence from ERPs","authors":"Suiping Wang, Deyuan Mo, M. Xiang, Ruiping Xu, Hsuan-Chich Chen","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.660169","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.660169","url":null,"abstract":"The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in reading Chinese was examined by recording event-related brain potentials (ERPs) as native Chinese speakers read individually presented sentences for comprehension and performed semantic plausibility judgments. The transitivity of the verbs in Chinese ba/bei constructions was manipulated to form three types of stimuli: Congruent sentences (CON), sentences with semantic violation (SEM), and sentences with combined semantic and syntactic violation (SEM+SYN). Compared with the critical words in CON, those in SEM and SEM+SYN elicited an N400-P600 biphasic pattern. The N400 effects in both violation conditions were of similar size and distribution, but the P600 in SEM+SYN was bigger than that in SEM. Overall, the lack of a difference between SEM and SEM+SYN in the earlier time window (i.e., N400 window) suggested that syntactic processing in Chinese does not necessarily occur earlier than semantic processing.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.660169","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Processing load during listening: The influence of task characteristics on the pupil response 听时加工负荷:任务特征对瞳孔反应的影响
Pub Date : 2013-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.642267
S. Kramer, A. Lorens, F. Coninx, A. Zekveld, A. Piotrowska, H. Skarżyńśki
This study examined the magnitude of the pupillary response evoked by a number of tasks varying in the nature and complexity of the auditory and linguistic information provided. The tasks comprised passive listening, anticipation to verbally responding to a prompt signal, auditory detection, and the identification of meaningful words. Performance in the auditory detection and identification tasks was matched at 79% correct. In all, 42 normally hearing adults (aged 18–44 years, mean age 25.5 years) from three different sites (Amsterdam, Cologne, and Warsaw) participated. During each condition, the pupil diameter was measured. A Repeated Measures ANOVA was conducted to examine within and between subject (site) differences in the pupil response over 8 time intervals during the four conditions. The maximum mean pupil dilation was largest in the words-in-noise identification task (0.13 mm) and differed significantly from the maximum mean dilation in the noise-in-noise-detection task. The latter did not differ significantly from the pupil response during passive listening to noise and an answer prompt. No significant differences between sites were observed. Task evoked pupillary responses to theory-based measures of linguistic processing are robust, reliable, and sensitive to differences in task demands. Word-in-noise identification requires more processing load than nonspeech detection. To obtain information about within-subject differences in auditory processing, examination of both processing load and behavioural performance is recommended. Methodological implications are discussed.
这项研究考察了瞳孔反应的大小,这些反应是由一些任务引起的,这些任务的性质和所提供的听觉和语言信息的复杂性是不同的。这些任务包括被动倾听、对提示信号做出口头反应的预期、听觉检测和识别有意义的单词。在听觉检测和识别任务中,准确率达到79%。共有来自三个不同地点(阿姆斯特丹、科隆和华沙)的42名听力正常的成年人(年龄18-44岁,平均年龄25.5岁)参与了研究。在每一种情况下,测量瞳孔直径。采用重复测量方差分析(Repeated Measures ANOVA)来检验四种情况下受试者(部位)在8个时间间隔内瞳孔反应的差异。噪声中单词识别任务的最大平均瞳孔扩张(0.13 mm)与噪声中噪声检测任务的最大平均瞳孔扩张显著不同。后者与被动听噪音和回答提示时的学生反应没有显著差异。不同部位间无显著差异。任务诱发瞳孔对基于理论的语言加工测量的反应是稳健、可靠的,并且对任务需求的差异敏感。噪声词识别比非语音检测需要更多的处理负荷。为了获得关于主体内听觉加工差异的信息,建议对加工负荷和行为表现进行检查。讨论了方法学意义。
{"title":"Processing load during listening: The influence of task characteristics on the pupil response","authors":"S. Kramer, A. Lorens, F. Coninx, A. Zekveld, A. Piotrowska, H. Skarżyńśki","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.642267","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.642267","url":null,"abstract":"This study examined the magnitude of the pupillary response evoked by a number of tasks varying in the nature and complexity of the auditory and linguistic information provided. The tasks comprised passive listening, anticipation to verbally responding to a prompt signal, auditory detection, and the identification of meaningful words. Performance in the auditory detection and identification tasks was matched at 79% correct. In all, 42 normally hearing adults (aged 18–44 years, mean age 25.5 years) from three different sites (Amsterdam, Cologne, and Warsaw) participated. During each condition, the pupil diameter was measured. A Repeated Measures ANOVA was conducted to examine within and between subject (site) differences in the pupil response over 8 time intervals during the four conditions. The maximum mean pupil dilation was largest in the words-in-noise identification task (0.13 mm) and differed significantly from the maximum mean dilation in the noise-in-noise-detection task. The latter did not differ significantly from the pupil response during passive listening to noise and an answer prompt. No significant differences between sites were observed. Task evoked pupillary responses to theory-based measures of linguistic processing are robust, reliable, and sensitive to differences in task demands. Word-in-noise identification requires more processing load than nonspeech detection. To obtain information about within-subject differences in auditory processing, examination of both processing load and behavioural performance is recommended. Methodological implications are discussed.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.642267","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135346","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 47
Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event. 看不见,但不忘:21个月大的婴儿即使在没有相应事件的情况下也会使用句法信息来学习动词。
Pub Date : 2013-04-01 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.641744
Sudha Arunachalam, Emily Escovar, Melissa A Hansen, Sandra R Waxman
By 27 months of age, toddlers hearing a novel verb in transitive syntax are able to (1) establish an initial representation for the verb based on its syntactic properties alone, even in the absence of a relevant visual scene, and (2) retrieve this representation later when a candidate causative referent comes into view. This ability is important considering that over 60% of the verbs that mothers produce in conversations with their children refer to events that are not currently observable. Here, we advance this finding in two ways. First, we demonstrate the same ability in 21-month-olds, who do not yet show mastery of transitive structures in their own productions. Second, we use analyses of toddlers’ eye gaze to explore the time-course with which they process the novel verb and assign its referent when candidate scenes become available. These results (1) provide the first evidence that 21-month-olds establish a representation of a novel verb's meaning from syntax alone, and (2) establish that they process and assign meaning to novel verbs with a similar time-course to that for novel nouns. The findings are thus relevant to our understanding of both word learning and lexical processing of novel words.
{"title":"Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event.","authors":"Sudha Arunachalam, Emily Escovar, Melissa A Hansen, Sandra R Waxman","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.641744","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.641744","url":null,"abstract":"By 27 months of age, toddlers hearing a novel verb in transitive syntax are able to (1) establish an initial representation for the verb based on its syntactic properties alone, even in the absence of a relevant visual scene, and (2) retrieve this representation later when a candidate causative referent comes into view. This ability is important considering that over 60% of the verbs that mothers produce in conversations with their children refer to events that are not currently observable. Here, we advance this finding in two ways. First, we demonstrate the same ability in 21-month-olds, who do not yet show mastery of transitive structures in their own productions. Second, we use analyses of toddlers’ eye gaze to explore the time-course with which they process the novel verb and assign its referent when candidate scenes become available. These results (1) provide the first evidence that 21-month-olds establish a representation of a novel verb's meaning from syntax alone, and (2) establish that they process and assign meaning to novel verbs with a similar time-course to that for novel nouns. The findings are thus relevant to our understanding of both word learning and lexical processing of novel words.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.641744","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40267409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 43
Formal vs. Processing Approaches to Syntactic Phenomena: A Special Issue of the journal Language and Cognitive Processes 句法现象的形式方法与加工方法:《语言与认知过程》杂志特刊
Pub Date : 2013-04-01 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2013.776803
{"title":"Formal vs. Processing Approaches to Syntactic Phenomena: A Special Issue of the journal Language and Cognitive Processes","authors":"","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2013.776803","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.776803","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2013.776803","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59136187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The object before subject bias and the processing of double-gap relative clauses in Japanese 主后宾偏误与日语双间隙关系从句的加工
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.634179
Michiko Nakamura, E. Miyamoto
We report a fragment-completion questionnaire and a self-paced reading experiment investigating relative clauses in Japanese that contain two extraction sites in the same clause. The results indicate that in such double-gap relative clauses there is a preference to fill the object position before the subject position. Previous factors, especially those related to working memory resources, which have often been implicated in extraction preferences, do not predict the patterns observed. The results support the proposal that the accessibility of extraction sites in relative clauses is inversely correlated to the order in which semantic roles are preferentially assigned. We discuss ways of incorporating such a constraint in previous proposals such as expectation-based models.
我们报告了一份片段补全问卷和一项自定节奏阅读实验,调查了日语中在同一个从句中包含两个提取点的关系从句。结果表明,在这种双间隙关系从句中,宾语位置优先于主语位置。先前的因素,特别是那些与工作记忆资源有关的因素,通常与提取偏好有关,并不能预测所观察到的模式。研究结果支持了关系分句中提取位置的可及性与语义角色优先分配顺序成反比的观点。我们讨论了在以前的建议中合并这种约束的方法,例如基于期望的模型。
{"title":"The object before subject bias and the processing of double-gap relative clauses in Japanese","authors":"Michiko Nakamura, E. Miyamoto","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.634179","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.634179","url":null,"abstract":"We report a fragment-completion questionnaire and a self-paced reading experiment investigating relative clauses in Japanese that contain two extraction sites in the same clause. The results indicate that in such double-gap relative clauses there is a preference to fill the object position before the subject position. Previous factors, especially those related to working memory resources, which have often been implicated in extraction preferences, do not predict the patterns observed. The results support the proposal that the accessibility of extraction sites in relative clauses is inversely correlated to the order in which semantic roles are preferentially assigned. We discuss ways of incorporating such a constraint in previous proposals such as expectation-based models.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.634179","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Graphemic parsing and the basic orthographic syllable structure 字形解析和基本的正字法音节结构
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.641386
C. Perry
In two experiments, whether people use an orthographic syllable structure based on phonology when reading multi-syllabic words or a structure based on an orthographic scheme first proposed by Taft (1979, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21–39) was investigated. This was done by presenting words disrupted by a space based on the particular structures being investigated and examining the extent that the spaces affected responses. Results from the first experiment showed that participants were slower to process words when consonant clusters were disrupted by a space compared to when the space occurred after the consonants and before a vowel. This occurred even though the space that did not disrupt the consonants caused the words to be segmented in a way that was not congruent with either of the orthographic structures being examined. Participants also appeared to display a weak preference for processing words as syllabic units, phonologically defined. The second experiment provided further evidence that people use an orthographic syllable structure based on phonological syllables and not just orthographic regularities. It is argued that these results are best interpreted in terms of a graphemic parsing process that uses two stages, one which is sensitive to consonant and vowel status but not syllable boundaries and a second which is sensitive to syllable boundaries initially derived from phonology.
在两个实验中,研究了人们在阅读多音节单词时是使用基于音系的正字法音节结构,还是使用Taft (1979, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21-39)首先提出的正字法音节结构。这是通过在被调查的特定结构的基础上呈现被空间打乱的单词,并检查空间影响反应的程度来完成的。第一个实验的结果表明,当辅音集群被空格打断时,参与者处理单词的速度要慢于辅音集群后面和元音集群前面的空格。即使没有打乱辅音的空格导致单词以一种与被检查的正字法结构不一致的方式被分割,这种情况也会发生。参与者似乎也表现出一种微弱的偏好,即把单词当作音节单位来处理,按照音系来定义。第二个实验提供了进一步的证据,证明人们使用基于语音音节的正字法音节结构,而不仅仅是正字法规则。有人认为,这些结果是最好的解释,在文字分析过程中,使用两个阶段,一个是敏感的辅音和元音状态,但不音节边界,第二个是敏感的音节边界最初来源于音系。
{"title":"Graphemic parsing and the basic orthographic syllable structure","authors":"C. Perry","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.641386","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.641386","url":null,"abstract":"In two experiments, whether people use an orthographic syllable structure based on phonology when reading multi-syllabic words or a structure based on an orthographic scheme first proposed by Taft (1979, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21–39) was investigated. This was done by presenting words disrupted by a space based on the particular structures being investigated and examining the extent that the spaces affected responses. Results from the first experiment showed that participants were slower to process words when consonant clusters were disrupted by a space compared to when the space occurred after the consonants and before a vowel. This occurred even though the space that did not disrupt the consonants caused the words to be segmented in a way that was not congruent with either of the orthographic structures being examined. Participants also appeared to display a weak preference for processing words as syllabic units, phonologically defined. The second experiment provided further evidence that people use an orthographic syllable structure based on phonological syllables and not just orthographic regularities. It is argued that these results are best interpreted in terms of a graphemic parsing process that uses two stages, one which is sensitive to consonant and vowel status but not syllable boundaries and a second which is sensitive to syllable boundaries initially derived from phonology.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.641386","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The empirical status of data in syntax: A reply to Gibson and Fedorenko 数据在句法中的经验地位:对Gibson和Fedorenko的回答
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.703782
Jon Sprouse, Diogo Almeida
This is a commentary in response to The need for quantitative methods in syntax and semantics research, by Edward Gibson and Evelina Fedorenko.
本文是对Edward Gibson和Evelina Fedorenko合著的《在语法和语义研究中需要定量方法》的评论。
{"title":"The empirical status of data in syntax: A reply to Gibson and Fedorenko","authors":"Jon Sprouse, Diogo Almeida","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2012.703782","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.703782","url":null,"abstract":"This is a commentary in response to The need for quantitative methods in syntax and semantics research, by Edward Gibson and Evelina Fedorenko.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2012.703782","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 90
Processing (un-)predictable word stress: ERP evidence from Turkish 处理(非)可预测的单词重音:来自土耳其语的ERP证据
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.634590
Ulrike Domahs, Safiye Genç, Johannes Knaus, Richard Wiese, Baris Kabak
This paper investigates the way the predictability of prosodic patterns in a particular language influences the processing of stress information by native speakers of that language. We extend previous findings where speakers of languages with predictable stress had difficulties to process and represent stress information when confronted with a language with distinctive stress and investigate how the co-existence of a predictable stress pattern and exceptions to that regularity within a single language influences prosodic processing. The stress system of Turkish constitutes an instructive test case since it employs predictable stress on the final syllable of a prosodic word (e.g., mısır “corn”) and some exceptional nonfinal stress (e.g., mısır “Egypt”). Results from an event-related potential (ERP) study on stress violations in Turkish trisyllabic words showed asymmetrical ERP responses for different stress violations: Stress violations with final stress produced an N400 effect whereas violations with nonfinal stress produced a P300 effect. The application of the predictable pattern to words with lexical stress led to lexical costs and the application of exceptional stress to words with default stress to effects reflecting the evaluation of this pattern. Although final stress constitutes no alternative pattern for words with exceptional stress, participants have difficulties to judge this pattern as incorrect. In contrast, exceptional stress patterns are detected easily when applied incorrectly to words that normally receive final stress. These findings demonstrate nicely the co-existence of two phonological processing routines in Turkish speakers. Furthermore, the variability of stress patterns does not affect prosodic processing in general but instead leads to differential effects in stress perception. We conclude that stress predictability does not homogenously result in the so-called “stress deafness” effects in stress processing, but that it rather emerges only for the default stress pattern.
本文研究了一种特定语言中韵律模式的可预测性对母语者重音信息加工的影响。我们扩展了先前的研究结果,即使用具有可预测重音的语言的人在面对具有独特重音的语言时难以处理和表示重音信息,并研究了在单一语言中可预测重音模式和例外情况的共存如何影响韵律处理。土耳其语的重音系统构成了一个有指导意义的测试案例,因为它在韵律词的最后一个音节上使用了可预测的重音(例如mısır“corn”)和一些特殊的非最终重音(例如mısır“Egypt”)。事件相关电位(event- correlation potential, ERP)对土耳其语三音节词中不同的应力违逆行为的研究结果表明,事件相关电位对不同的应力违逆行为的反应是不对称的:带有末重音的应力违逆产生N400效应,而带有非末重音的应力违逆产生P300效应。对具有词汇重音的词应用可预测模式导致词汇成本,对具有默认重音的词应用例外重音的效果反映了对该模式的评价。虽然重读单词的最后一个重音不构成特殊重音的替代模式,但参与者很难判断这种模式是否不正确。相反,当不正确地应用于通常接受最后重音的单词时,很容易发现异常的重音模式。这些发现很好地证明了两种语音加工程序在土耳其语使用者中共存。此外,应激模式的可变性并不影响一般的韵律加工,而是导致应激感知的差异效应。我们得出的结论是,应力可预见性并不会均匀地导致所谓的“应力聋”效应,而是只出现在默认的应力模式中。
{"title":"Processing (un-)predictable word stress: ERP evidence from Turkish","authors":"Ulrike Domahs, Safiye Genç, Johannes Knaus, Richard Wiese, Baris Kabak","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.634590","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.634590","url":null,"abstract":"This paper investigates the way the predictability of prosodic patterns in a particular language influences the processing of stress information by native speakers of that language. We extend previous findings where speakers of languages with predictable stress had difficulties to process and represent stress information when confronted with a language with distinctive stress and investigate how the co-existence of a predictable stress pattern and exceptions to that regularity within a single language influences prosodic processing. The stress system of Turkish constitutes an instructive test case since it employs predictable stress on the final syllable of a prosodic word (e.g., mısır “corn”) and some exceptional nonfinal stress (e.g., mısır “Egypt”). Results from an event-related potential (ERP) study on stress violations in Turkish trisyllabic words showed asymmetrical ERP responses for different stress violations: Stress violations with final stress produced an N400 effect whereas violations with nonfinal stress produced a P300 effect. The application of the predictable pattern to words with lexical stress led to lexical costs and the application of exceptional stress to words with default stress to effects reflecting the evaluation of this pattern. Although final stress constitutes no alternative pattern for words with exceptional stress, participants have difficulties to judge this pattern as incorrect. In contrast, exceptional stress patterns are detected easily when applied incorrectly to words that normally receive final stress. These findings demonstrate nicely the co-existence of two phonological processing routines in Turkish speakers. Furthermore, the variability of stress patterns does not affect prosodic processing in general but instead leads to differential effects in stress perception. We conclude that stress predictability does not homogenously result in the so-called “stress deafness” effects in stress processing, but that it rather emerges only for the default stress pattern.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.634590","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 37
Null pronominal (pro) resolution in Korean, a discourse-oriented language 韩国语是一种语篇导向的语言
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.645314
Nayoung Kwon, P. Sturt
We report a self-paced reading experiment examining the effect of morpho-syntactic and discourse cues on the resolution of null pronominals in Korean. Unlike many other languages, Korean does not have many morphological cues that could aid reference assignment for null pronominals. Indeed, work in theoretical linguistics has classified Korean as a “discourse oriented language”, in which empty arguments can be identified through discourse topics, with less reliance on sentence-internal cues than is the case for “sentence oriented” languages such as English. The experiment used a person-mismatch paradigm to test whether, once assigned to a discourse topic, the reference of the null pronominal could be shifted to a new intra-sentential antecedent, on the basis of morpho-syntactic cues, as has been shown in work in English on overt pronouns. The findings show that the null pronominal was assigned to the discourse topic on-line during the reading of the sentence, but that the re-assignment to the intra-sentential antecedent did not take place, despite the morpho-syntactic cues. Instead, co-reference with the intra-sentential subject was attempted only when there was no antecedent in the context. We interpret these findings in terms of the relatively high priority that Korean places on discourse cues.
我们报告了一项自定节奏阅读实验,研究了形态句法和话语线索对韩语空代词分辨的影响。与许多其他语言不同,韩语没有很多形态线索可以帮助空代词的引用分配。事实上,理论语言学的工作已经将韩语分类为“话语导向语言”,在这种语言中,空洞的论点可以通过话语主题来识别,与“句子导向”语言(如英语)相比,对句子内部线索的依赖较少。该实验使用了一个人-错配范式来测试,一旦被分配到一个话语主题,在形态-句法线索的基础上,空代词的引用是否可以转移到一个新的句子内先行词,正如在英语中对显性代词的研究所显示的那样。研究结果表明,在阅读句子过程中,空代词被在线分配到话语主题上,而句内先行词的重新分配并未发生,尽管有形态句法提示。相反,只有在语境中没有先行词的情况下,才会尝试与句子内主语进行共同指称。我们对这些发现的解释是,韩国语对话语线索的重视程度相对较高。
{"title":"Null pronominal (pro) resolution in Korean, a discourse-oriented language","authors":"Nayoung Kwon, P. Sturt","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.645314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.645314","url":null,"abstract":"We report a self-paced reading experiment examining the effect of morpho-syntactic and discourse cues on the resolution of null pronominals in Korean. Unlike many other languages, Korean does not have many morphological cues that could aid reference assignment for null pronominals. Indeed, work in theoretical linguistics has classified Korean as a “discourse oriented language”, in which empty arguments can be identified through discourse topics, with less reliance on sentence-internal cues than is the case for “sentence oriented” languages such as English. The experiment used a person-mismatch paradigm to test whether, once assigned to a discourse topic, the reference of the null pronominal could be shifted to a new intra-sentential antecedent, on the basis of morpho-syntactic cues, as has been shown in work in English on overt pronouns. The findings show that the null pronominal was assigned to the discourse topic on-line during the reading of the sentence, but that the re-assignment to the intra-sentential antecedent did not take place, despite the morpho-syntactic cues. Instead, co-reference with the intra-sentential subject was attempted only when there was no antecedent in the context. We interpret these findings in terms of the relatively high priority that Korean places on discourse cues.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.645314","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59135394","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
Processing presuppositions: Dynamic semantics vs pragmatic enrichment 加工预设:动态语义与语用充实
Pub Date : 2013-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2011.615221
E. Chemla, Lewis Bott
One defining and yet puzzling feature of linguistic presuppositions is the way they interact with linguistic operators. For instance, when a presupposition trigger (e.g., realise) occurs under negation (e.g., Zoologists do not realise that elephants are mammals), the sentence is most commonly interpreted with the same global presupposition (elephants are mammals) as if negation was not present. Alternatively, the presupposition may be locally accommodated, i.e., the presupposition may become part of what is negated. In this paper, we develop and test two processing accounts of presupposition projection, the global-first model and the local-first model, inspired by dynamic semantics and pragmatic theories respectively. We tested these predictions using a verification task similar to Bott and Noveck's test of default models of scalar implicature. Across two experiments, using different materials and instructions, participants were faster to derive the global interpretation than the local interpretation, in contrast to the local-first model. We discuss the results in terms of dynamic semantics vs pragmatic models of presupposition projection.
语言预设的一个定义而又令人困惑的特征是它们与语言操作符相互作用的方式。例如,当预设触发(例如,意识到)发生在否定情况下(例如,动物学家没有意识到大象是哺乳动物),句子通常用相同的全局预设(大象是哺乳动物)来解释,就好像否定不存在一样。或者,预设可以被局部适应,也就是说,预设可以成为被否定的东西的一部分。本文在动态语义学和语用学理论的启发下,开发并验证了预设投射的两种处理方法——全局优先模型和局部优先模型。我们使用类似于博特和诺韦克对标量隐含的默认模型的测试的验证任务来测试这些预测。在两个实验中,使用不同的材料和说明,参与者得出全局解释比得出局部解释要快,这与局部优先模型相反。我们从预设投射的动态语义模型和语用模型的角度讨论了结果。
{"title":"Processing presuppositions: Dynamic semantics vs pragmatic enrichment","authors":"E. Chemla, Lewis Bott","doi":"10.1080/01690965.2011.615221","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.615221","url":null,"abstract":"One defining and yet puzzling feature of linguistic presuppositions is the way they interact with linguistic operators. For instance, when a presupposition trigger (e.g., realise) occurs under negation (e.g., Zoologists do not realise that elephants are mammals), the sentence is most commonly interpreted with the same global presupposition (elephants are mammals) as if negation was not present. Alternatively, the presupposition may be locally accommodated, i.e., the presupposition may become part of what is negated. In this paper, we develop and test two processing accounts of presupposition projection, the global-first model and the local-first model, inspired by dynamic semantics and pragmatic theories respectively. We tested these predictions using a verification task similar to Bott and Noveck's test of default models of scalar implicature. Across two experiments, using different materials and instructions, participants were faster to derive the global interpretation than the local interpretation, in contrast to the local-first model. We discuss the results in terms of dynamic semantics vs pragmatic models of presupposition projection.","PeriodicalId":87410,"journal":{"name":"Language and cognitive processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01690965.2011.615221","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59134999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 65
期刊
Language and cognitive processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1