首页 > 最新文献

The European journal of cognitive psychology最新文献

英文 中文
List context effects in reading Italian words and nonwords: Can the word frequency effect be eliminated? 列举阅读意大利语单词和非单词时的语境效应:词频效应可以消除吗?
Pub Date : 2010-07-15 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903216492
Despina Paizi, C. Burani, P. Zoccolotti
It has been suggested that when words and nonwords are presented mixed in the same blocks in transparent scripts the word frequency effect can be eliminated. This is either because in reading mixed blocks the lexical route is deemphasised in favour of the nonlexical route (route deemphasis) or because a homogenisation of reaction times occurs for words and nonwords by adjustment of a time criterion for articulation (time criterion). In five experiments using different list manipulations and experimental designs, we assessed the effects of frequency and length in reading aloud words and nonwords in the Italian transparent orthography. The effect of word frequency remained constant irrespective of context manipulation and nonword characteristics. As reading nonwords may be easier in Italian than in English, control over reading processing may be unnecessary for Italian readers. The results are discussed with respect to current computational models of reading.
有研究表明,在透明文本中,当单词和非单词混合在同一块中呈现时,可以消除词频效应。这要么是因为在阅读混合块时,词汇路径被弱化,而非词汇路径被弱化(路径弱化),要么是因为通过调整发音的时间标准(时间标准),单词和非单词的反应时间发生了同质化。在五个实验中,我们使用不同的列表操作和实验设计,评估了频率和长度对意大利语透明正字法朗读单词和非单词的影响。词频的影响不受语境操纵和非词特征的影响。由于意大利语阅读非单词可能比英语更容易,因此意大利语读者可能没有必要控制阅读过程。讨论了当前阅读计算模型的结果。
{"title":"List context effects in reading Italian words and nonwords: Can the word frequency effect be eliminated?","authors":"Despina Paizi, C. Burani, P. Zoccolotti","doi":"10.1080/09541440903216492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903216492","url":null,"abstract":"It has been suggested that when words and nonwords are presented mixed in the same blocks in transparent scripts the word frequency effect can be eliminated. This is either because in reading mixed blocks the lexical route is deemphasised in favour of the nonlexical route (route deemphasis) or because a homogenisation of reaction times occurs for words and nonwords by adjustment of a time criterion for articulation (time criterion). In five experiments using different list manipulations and experimental designs, we assessed the effects of frequency and length in reading aloud words and nonwords in the Italian transparent orthography. The effect of word frequency remained constant irrespective of context manipulation and nonword characteristics. As reading nonwords may be easier in Italian than in English, control over reading processing may be unnecessary for Italian readers. The results are discussed with respect to current computational models of reading.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"11 1","pages":"1039 - 1065"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82320425","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study 初学成人二语学习者对形态句法违反的敏感性:一项自定节奏阅读研究
Pub Date : 2010-07-15 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903325178
Natasha Tokowicz, Tessa C. Warren
This study investigated beginning adult second language (L2) learners' sensitivity to L2 morphosyntactic violations as a function of cross-language similarity. Online sensitivity was indexed by self-paced reading times at: (1) the critical word at which the violation could first be detected, (2) the post-critical word, and (3) the sentence-final word. In conditions in which morphosyntactic marking systems were similar or different in L1 and L2, reading times on the critical word were slower when it cued a violation than when it did not; however, this sensitivity was not apparent in a construction unique to L2. Slower reading times to violations spilled over onto the post-critical word. Cross-language similarity also influenced sentence-final word reading times. Despite this online sensitivity, post-sentence grammaticality judgements were generally poor. However, these judgements were influenced by morphosyntactic markings on words after the critical word, suggesting that learners can make use of this information.
本研究调查了初学成人第二语言(L2)学习者对第二语言形态句法违规的敏感性与跨语言相似性的关系。在线敏感度是通过自定阅读时间来索引的:(1)第一次发现违规的关键字,(2)后关键字,(3)句子的最后一个词。在L1和L2的形态句法标记系统相似或不同的条件下,当关键字提示违规时,阅读速度比不提示违规时慢;然而,这种敏感性在L2独有的结构中并不明显。较慢的阅读时间会影响到后批评词。跨语言相似性也会影响句子尾词的阅读时间。尽管网上有这种敏感性,但人们对句后语法的判断通常很差。然而,这些判断受到关键词之后单词的形态句法标记的影响,这表明学习者可以利用这些信息。
{"title":"Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study","authors":"Natasha Tokowicz, Tessa C. Warren","doi":"10.1080/09541440903325178","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903325178","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigated beginning adult second language (L2) learners' sensitivity to L2 morphosyntactic violations as a function of cross-language similarity. Online sensitivity was indexed by self-paced reading times at: (1) the critical word at which the violation could first be detected, (2) the post-critical word, and (3) the sentence-final word. In conditions in which morphosyntactic marking systems were similar or different in L1 and L2, reading times on the critical word were slower when it cued a violation than when it did not; however, this sensitivity was not apparent in a construction unique to L2. Slower reading times to violations spilled over onto the post-critical word. Cross-language similarity also influenced sentence-final word reading times. Despite this online sensitivity, post-sentence grammaticality judgements were generally poor. However, these judgements were influenced by morphosyntactic markings on words after the critical word, suggesting that learners can make use of this information.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"29 1","pages":"1092 - 1106"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79646156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 40
Episodic retrieval processes take place automatically in auditory negative priming 情景检索过程在听觉负启动中自动发生
Pub Date : 2010-07-15 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903409808
S. Mayr, A. Buchner
Episodic retrieval processes involved in negative priming have been argued to be susceptible to the proportion of attended repetition trials. The more trials with the same prime and probe response, the more beneficial should it be to retrieve the prime episode, particularly its response. Retrieval of the prime episode, however, is task-inappropriate for ignored repetition trials, leading to negative priming. Correspondingly, visual negative priming increases with the proportion of attended repetition trials. We tested whether the same is true for the auditory modality. Three attended repetition proportion groups (0–25–50%) showed the same amount of negative priming. All groups committed more prime response errors in ignored repetition than in control trials, implying that prime response retrieval took place. Thus, retrieval processes in auditory negative priming appear to be automatic and cannot be influenced as easily as in the visual domain. In Experiment 2, the proportion of ignored repetition trials was manipulated (25–50–75%) to test whether auditory negative priming can be strategically manipulated at all. Similar to the visual modality negative priming was reduced with increasing proportion of ignored repetition trials. Differences between visual and auditory short-term memory are discussed to account for the results.
参与消极启动的情景检索过程被认为容易受到参与重复试验的比例的影响。相同启动反应和探测反应的试验越多,检索启动事件,特别是其反应就越有益。然而,对于被忽略的重复试验,启动片段的检索是任务不合适的,导致负启动。与之相对应的是,视觉负启动随参加重复试验的比例增加而增加。我们测试了听觉模态是否也是如此。三个被试重复比例组(0 ~ 25 ~ 50%)的负启动量相同。所有组在忽略重复中比在对照试验中犯了更多的启动反应错误,这意味着启动反应检索发生了。因此,听觉负启动的检索过程似乎是自动的,不像视觉域那样容易受到影响。实验2通过操纵被忽略重复试验的比例(25-50-75%)来检验听觉负启动是否可以被策略操纵。与视觉模态相似,负启动随着被忽略重复试验比例的增加而减少。讨论了视觉和听觉短期记忆之间的差异,以解释结果。
{"title":"Episodic retrieval processes take place automatically in auditory negative priming","authors":"S. Mayr, A. Buchner","doi":"10.1080/09541440903409808","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903409808","url":null,"abstract":"Episodic retrieval processes involved in negative priming have been argued to be susceptible to the proportion of attended repetition trials. The more trials with the same prime and probe response, the more beneficial should it be to retrieve the prime episode, particularly its response. Retrieval of the prime episode, however, is task-inappropriate for ignored repetition trials, leading to negative priming. Correspondingly, visual negative priming increases with the proportion of attended repetition trials. We tested whether the same is true for the auditory modality. Three attended repetition proportion groups (0–25–50%) showed the same amount of negative priming. All groups committed more prime response errors in ignored repetition than in control trials, implying that prime response retrieval took place. Thus, retrieval processes in auditory negative priming appear to be automatic and cannot be influenced as easily as in the visual domain. In Experiment 2, the proportion of ignored repetition trials was manipulated (25–50–75%) to test whether auditory negative priming can be strategically manipulated at all. Similar to the visual modality negative priming was reduced with increasing proportion of ignored repetition trials. Differences between visual and auditory short-term memory are discussed to account for the results.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"1192 - 1221"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90296092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Rapid allocation of temporal attention in the attentional blink paradigm 注意眨眼范式中时间注意的快速分配
Pub Date : 2010-07-12 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903418924
F. Hilkenmeier, I. Scharlau
How fast can information of a first target (T1) in a rapid serial visual presentation be used for top-down allocation of attention in time? A valid cue about the temporal position of a second target (T2) was integrated into T1. The data show that 100 ms after T1 onset, T2 was identified better than without cue, raising the conditional T2 performance. T1 apparently triggers a facilitative effect of attention, known from other paradigms such as peripheral cueing.
在快速连续视觉呈现中,第一目标(T1)的信息用于自上而下的注意力及时分配的速度有多快?关于第二目标(T2)时间位置的有效提示被整合到T1中。数据显示,T1启动100 ms后,T2的识别能力优于无提示,提高了条件T2的表现。T1显然触发了注意力的促进效应,从其他范例如外围提示中可以知道。
{"title":"Rapid allocation of temporal attention in the attentional blink paradigm","authors":"F. Hilkenmeier, I. Scharlau","doi":"10.1080/09541440903418924","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903418924","url":null,"abstract":"How fast can information of a first target (T1) in a rapid serial visual presentation be used for top-down allocation of attention in time? A valid cue about the temporal position of a second target (T2) was integrated into T1. The data show that 100 ms after T1 onset, T2 was identified better than without cue, raising the conditional T2 performance. T1 apparently triggers a facilitative effect of attention, known from other paradigms such as peripheral cueing.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"85 1","pages":"1222 - 1234"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78698575","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
A meta-analytic review of thinking about what is true, possible, and irrelevant in reasoning from or reasoning about conditional propositions 对从条件命题推理或对条件命题推理时思考什么是真实的、可能的和不相关的元分析回顾
Pub Date : 2010-07-07 DOI: 10.1080/09541440902928915
W. Schroyens
Truth-table tasks probe for interpretations of if A then C conditionals by having people evaluate both true-antecedent cases (A and C, A and not-C) and false-antecedent cases, (not-A and C, not-A and not-C). Do these make the rule true or false, or are they irrelevant to the rule? Alternatively, are these cases possible or impossible when one assumes the rule is true? We present meta-analyses correcting the erroneous generalisation that false-antecedent cases are judged irrelevant by most people. Irrelevant judgement of a false-antecedent is only a modal response in a specific minority of cases (implicit “not-A and not-2” cases, e.g., “the letter is a B and the number is a 7” vis-à-vis “if A then 2”). Given that arguments against mental-models theory (e.g., Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 2002) are thus based on a mistaken idea about the “facts”, they require critical analysis. A reconsideration of the theory indicates that it accounts for all benchmark phenomena in truth-table tasks and even yields new predictions. The theory implies that a single case that does make the rule false does not make it true but is not irrelevant either. Such cases are consistent with the rule and corroborate it. Specific meta-analyses confirm the resulting prediction that “consistent” response rates are higher than “true” response rates, which goes at the cost of the irrelevant responses.
真值表任务通过让人们评估真先行情况(A和C, A和非C)和假先行情况(非A和C,非A和非C)来探索对if A then C条件的解释。这些规则是正确的还是错误的,或者它们与规则无关?或者,当一个人假设规则为真时,这些情况是可能的还是不可能的?我们提出的荟萃分析纠正了错误的概括,即大多数人认为假前因案件无关。假先行词的无关判断仅在特定的少数情况下是模态反应(隐含的“非a和非2”情况,例如,“字母是B,数字是7”,即-à-vis“如果a则2”)。鉴于反对心理模型理论的论点(例如Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 2002)是基于对“事实”的错误看法,它们需要批判性分析。对该理论的重新考虑表明,它解释了真值表任务中的所有基准现象,甚至产生了新的预测。该理论暗示,一个使规则为假的单一情况并不使规则为真,但也不是无关紧要的。这些案例符合规则并证实了规则。具体的元分析证实了结果预测,即“一致”的反应率高于“真实”的反应率,这是以不相关的反应为代价的。
{"title":"A meta-analytic review of thinking about what is true, possible, and irrelevant in reasoning from or reasoning about conditional propositions","authors":"W. Schroyens","doi":"10.1080/09541440902928915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902928915","url":null,"abstract":"Truth-table tasks probe for interpretations of if A then C conditionals by having people evaluate both true-antecedent cases (A and C, A and not-C) and false-antecedent cases, (not-A and C, not-A and not-C). Do these make the rule true or false, or are they irrelevant to the rule? Alternatively, are these cases possible or impossible when one assumes the rule is true? We present meta-analyses correcting the erroneous generalisation that false-antecedent cases are judged irrelevant by most people. Irrelevant judgement of a false-antecedent is only a modal response in a specific minority of cases (implicit “not-A and not-2” cases, e.g., “the letter is a B and the number is a 7” vis-à-vis “if A then 2”). Given that arguments against mental-models theory (e.g., Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 2002) are thus based on a mistaken idea about the “facts”, they require critical analysis. A reconsideration of the theory indicates that it accounts for all benchmark phenomena in truth-table tasks and even yields new predictions. The theory implies that a single case that does make the rule false does not make it true but is not irrelevant either. Such cases are consistent with the rule and corroborate it. Specific meta-analyses confirm the resulting prediction that “consistent” response rates are higher than “true” response rates, which goes at the cost of the irrelevant responses.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"49 1","pages":"897 - 921"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83723095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 26
The irrelevant sound effect in short-term memory: Is there developmental change? 短期记忆中的不相关声音效应:是否存在发展变化?
Pub Date : 2010-07-07 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903378250
M. Klatte, T. Lachmann, Sabine J. Schlittmeier, J. Hellbrück
With two experiments, effects of irrelevant speech and classroom noise on serial recall of common nouns presented pictorially were investigated in children and adults. Experiment 1 used fixed list lengths for children (first graders) and adults. Experiment 2 used list lengths adjusted to participants' (second–third graders, adults) individual spans. In both experiments, children and adults were equally impaired by irrelevant speech. This contrasts with a related study (differences in methodology) by Elliott (2002), who reported severe increase in the detrimental impact of irrelevant speech with decreasing age. In both experiments, classroom noise had no effect in overall analyses. For Experiment 1, however, separate group analyses revealed impairment in children. Results suggest that effects of irrelevant sounds on serial recall stem from two separate mechanisms: Specific interference due to the sounds' automatic access to short-term memory, and/or attention capture. Only for the latter there is developmental change.
通过两个实验,研究了不相关言语和课堂噪音对儿童和成人常用名词系列记忆的影响。实验1对儿童(一年级)和成人使用固定的列表长度。实验2使用的列表长度根据参与者(二、三年级、成人)的个体跨度进行调整。在这两个实验中,儿童和成人同样受到无关言语的损害。这与Elliott(2002)的一项相关研究(方法论差异)形成对比,Elliott报告说,随着年龄的下降,不相关言语的有害影响会严重增加。在两个实验中,课堂噪音对整体分析没有影响。然而,在实验1中,单独的组分析显示了儿童的损伤。结果表明,不相关声音对连续回忆的影响源于两个独立的机制:由于声音自动进入短期记忆而产生的特定干扰,和/或注意力捕获。只有后者才有发展性的变化。
{"title":"The irrelevant sound effect in short-term memory: Is there developmental change?","authors":"M. Klatte, T. Lachmann, Sabine J. Schlittmeier, J. Hellbrück","doi":"10.1080/09541440903378250","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903378250","url":null,"abstract":"With two experiments, effects of irrelevant speech and classroom noise on serial recall of common nouns presented pictorially were investigated in children and adults. Experiment 1 used fixed list lengths for children (first graders) and adults. Experiment 2 used list lengths adjusted to participants' (second–third graders, adults) individual spans. In both experiments, children and adults were equally impaired by irrelevant speech. This contrasts with a related study (differences in methodology) by Elliott (2002), who reported severe increase in the detrimental impact of irrelevant speech with decreasing age. In both experiments, classroom noise had no effect in overall analyses. For Experiment 1, however, separate group analyses revealed impairment in children. Results suggest that effects of irrelevant sounds on serial recall stem from two separate mechanisms: Specific interference due to the sounds' automatic access to short-term memory, and/or attention capture. Only for the latter there is developmental change.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"58 1","pages":"1168 - 1191"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76944090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 69
Miscomprehension, meaning, and phonology: The unknown and phonological Armstrong illusions 误解、意义和音系:未知的和音系上的阿姆斯特朗错觉
Pub Date : 2010-06-01 DOI: 10.1080/09541440902941967
Meredith A. Shafto, D. G. MacKay
People often fail to detect the anomalous word in questions such as How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the Ark?, and incorrectly answer “two” despite knowing that Noah rather than Moses launched the Ark. The current study tests an account of this “Moses illusion” in which Moses mistakes reflect miscomprehension of the presented word (Moses) as the expected word (Noah) due to bottom-up (phonological) priming, top-down (semantic) priming, or both. Two experiments supported this miscomprehension account: Lexical- and proposition-level information contributed autonomously to miscomprehensions and Moses mistakes in Experiment 1, and prior presentation of nonanomalous information reduced subsequent anomaly detection in Experiment 2. Present results contradict accounts in which Moses mistakes involve semantic but not phonological processes, involve mechanisms different from everyday language comprehension, or involve special anomaly detection mechanisms for calculating the coherence between the Moses question and the anomalous word.
人们常常在诸如“摩西带了多少种动物上方舟”这样的问题中察觉不到这个反常的词。,错误地回答“2”,尽管知道是诺亚而不是摩西发射了方舟。目前的研究测试了“摩西错觉”的一种解释,其中摩西的错误反映了由于自下而上(语音)启动,自上而下(语义)启动,或两者兼而有之,将呈现的词(摩西)误解为预期的词(诺亚)。两个实验支持这一误解解释:在实验1中,词汇和命题层面的信息自主地导致了误解和摩西错误,而在实验2中,事先呈现的非异常信息减少了随后的异常检测。目前的结果与以下说法相矛盾:摩西题错误涉及语义过程而非语音过程,涉及不同于日常语言理解的机制,或涉及计算摩西题与异常词之间一致性的特殊异常检测机制。
{"title":"Miscomprehension, meaning, and phonology: The unknown and phonological Armstrong illusions","authors":"Meredith A. Shafto, D. G. MacKay","doi":"10.1080/09541440902941967","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902941967","url":null,"abstract":"People often fail to detect the anomalous word in questions such as How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the Ark?, and incorrectly answer “two” despite knowing that Noah rather than Moses launched the Ark. The current study tests an account of this “Moses illusion” in which Moses mistakes reflect miscomprehension of the presented word (Moses) as the expected word (Noah) due to bottom-up (phonological) priming, top-down (semantic) priming, or both. Two experiments supported this miscomprehension account: Lexical- and proposition-level information contributed autonomously to miscomprehensions and Moses mistakes in Experiment 1, and prior presentation of nonanomalous information reduced subsequent anomaly detection in Experiment 2. Present results contradict accounts in which Moses mistakes involve semantic but not phonological processes, involve mechanisms different from everyday language comprehension, or involve special anomaly detection mechanisms for calculating the coherence between the Moses question and the anomalous word.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"71 1","pages":"529 - 568"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86165778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Attention capture by novel sounds: Distraction versus facilitation 新颖声音的注意力捕获:分散与促进
Pub Date : 2010-06-01 DOI: 10.1080/09541440902930994
I. SanMiguel, D. Linden, C. Escera
Unexpected sounds have been shown to capture attention, triggering an orienting response. However, opposing effects of this attention capture on the performance of a concomitant visual task have been reported, in some instances leading to distraction and in others to facilitation. Moreover, the orienting response towards the unexpected stimuli can be modulated by working memory (WM) load, but the direction of this modulation has been another issue of controversy. In four experiments, we aimed to establish the critical factors that determine whether novel sounds facilitate or disrupt task performance and the modulation of these effects by WM load. Depending on the overall attentional demands of the task, novel sounds led to faster or slower responses. WM load attenuated novel sound effects, independent of their direction (facilitation or distraction). We propose a model by which the unexpected stimuli always generate the same orienting response but result in distraction or facilitation depending critically on the attentional focusing induced by the task at hand and the temporal relationship between the irrelevant and task-related stimuli.
意料之外的声音可以吸引注意力,引发定向反应。然而,这种注意力捕获对伴随的视觉任务的表现的相反影响已经被报道,在某些情况下导致分心,在其他情况下导致促进。此外,工作记忆(WM)负荷可以调节对意外刺激的定向反应,但这种调节的方向一直是另一个有争议的问题。在四个实验中,我们旨在确定决定新声音是促进还是破坏任务表现的关键因素,以及WM负载对这些影响的调节。根据任务的整体注意力需求,新奇的声音会导致更快或更慢的反应。WM负载衰减新的声音效果,独立于他们的方向(促进或分散)。我们提出了一个模型,通过该模型,意外刺激总是产生相同的定向反应,但会导致注意力分散或促进,这主要取决于手头任务引起的注意力集中以及无关刺激和任务相关刺激之间的时间关系。
{"title":"Attention capture by novel sounds: Distraction versus facilitation","authors":"I. SanMiguel, D. Linden, C. Escera","doi":"10.1080/09541440902930994","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902930994","url":null,"abstract":"Unexpected sounds have been shown to capture attention, triggering an orienting response. However, opposing effects of this attention capture on the performance of a concomitant visual task have been reported, in some instances leading to distraction and in others to facilitation. Moreover, the orienting response towards the unexpected stimuli can be modulated by working memory (WM) load, but the direction of this modulation has been another issue of controversy. In four experiments, we aimed to establish the critical factors that determine whether novel sounds facilitate or disrupt task performance and the modulation of these effects by WM load. Depending on the overall attentional demands of the task, novel sounds led to faster or slower responses. WM load attenuated novel sound effects, independent of their direction (facilitation or distraction). We propose a model by which the unexpected stimuli always generate the same orienting response but result in distraction or facilitation depending critically on the attentional focusing induced by the task at hand and the temporal relationship between the irrelevant and task-related stimuli.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"91 1","pages":"481 - 515"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85666703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 74
The tendency to overestimate what is visible in a planar mirror amongst adults and children 在成人和儿童中,对平面镜中可见的东西估计过高的倾向
Pub Date : 2010-06-01 DOI: 10.1080/09541440902934087
Marco Bertamini, Lauren A Wynne
Adults hold several mistaken beliefs about simple mechanical and optical phenomena. In particular, many adults believe that they would be able to see their own image in a mirror before they are in front of it. Similarly, they expect objects to become visible in mirrors before they actually do. This overestimation of what is visible is known as the early error (Bertamini, Spooner, & Hecht, 2003). It has been suggested that incorrect models about mechanics, and therefore erroneous beliefs, develop over time, as evidenced by good performance in young children (Kaiser, McCloskey, & Proffitt, 1986). With respect to knowledge about what is visible in mirrors we report the first developmental data. We confirmed an effect for prospective University students but found no evidence of any early error in children between the age of 5 and 11. This erroneous belief about mirrors develops during the later school years when people develop a system of beliefs based on experience.
成年人对简单的机械和光学现象有几种错误的看法。特别是,许多成年人相信他们能够在自己站在镜子前面之前看到自己在镜子里的形象。同样地,他们期望物体在镜子中变得清晰可见。这种对可见事物的高估被称为早期误差(Bertamini, Spooner, & Hecht, 2003)。有人认为,关于力学的不正确模型,以及因此产生的错误信念,会随着时间的推移而发展,幼儿的良好表现就证明了这一点(Kaiser, McCloskey, & Proffitt, 1986)。关于在镜子中可见的知识,我们报告了第一个发展数据。我们证实了对未来的大学生的影响,但在5到11岁的儿童中没有发现任何早期错误的证据。这种关于镜子的错误信念是在上学后期形成的,那时人们根据经验形成了一套信念体系。
{"title":"The tendency to overestimate what is visible in a planar mirror amongst adults and children","authors":"Marco Bertamini, Lauren A Wynne","doi":"10.1080/09541440902934087","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902934087","url":null,"abstract":"Adults hold several mistaken beliefs about simple mechanical and optical phenomena. In particular, many adults believe that they would be able to see their own image in a mirror before they are in front of it. Similarly, they expect objects to become visible in mirrors before they actually do. This overestimation of what is visible is known as the early error (Bertamini, Spooner, & Hecht, 2003). It has been suggested that incorrect models about mechanics, and therefore erroneous beliefs, develop over time, as evidenced by good performance in young children (Kaiser, McCloskey, & Proffitt, 1986). With respect to knowledge about what is visible in mirrors we report the first developmental data. We confirmed an effect for prospective University students but found no evidence of any early error in children between the age of 5 and 11. This erroneous belief about mirrors develops during the later school years when people develop a system of beliefs based on experience.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"3 1","pages":"516 - 528"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74353948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Simulating syllable frequency effects within an interactive activation framework 在交互激活框架内模拟音节频率效应
Pub Date : 2010-05-27 DOI: 10.1080/09541440903356777
M. Conrad, S. Tamm, M. Carreiras, A. Jacobs
Simulation data are presented for a novel computational model of visual word recognition containing syllabic representation units. The model is based on the multiple readout model MROM (Grainger & Jacobs, 1996) and it was used to simulate data from a syllable frequency experiment. The model successfully simulates the inhibitory syllable frequency effect in lexical decision obtained so far in Spanish, German, and French. In addition, this model, fully representing the principles of interactive activation between the three representation layers of letter, syllable, and word units, proved successful in correctly parsing all presented stimuli contained in its lexicon with different types of syllabic structures. Multiple regression analyses on the model's output and on human data confirmed the model's ability to still account for effects of word frequency and orthographic neighbourhood like the original MROM (Grainger & Jacobs, 1996) it was derived from.
提出了一种新的包含音节表示单元的视觉词识别计算模型的仿真数据。该模型基于多读出模型存储器(Grainger & Jacobs, 1996),并用于模拟音节频率实验的数据。该模型成功地模拟了西班牙语、德语和法语词汇决策中抑制性音节频率效应。此外,该模型充分体现了字母、音节和词单元三个表征层之间的交互激活原理,能够正确解析词典中包含的所有不同音节结构类型的呈现刺激。对模型输出和人类数据的多元回归分析证实了该模型仍然能够解释词频和正字法邻居的影响,就像原始的MROM一样(Grainger & Jacobs, 1996)。
{"title":"Simulating syllable frequency effects within an interactive activation framework","authors":"M. Conrad, S. Tamm, M. Carreiras, A. Jacobs","doi":"10.1080/09541440903356777","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903356777","url":null,"abstract":"Simulation data are presented for a novel computational model of visual word recognition containing syllabic representation units. The model is based on the multiple readout model MROM (Grainger & Jacobs, 1996) and it was used to simulate data from a syllable frequency experiment. The model successfully simulates the inhibitory syllable frequency effect in lexical decision obtained so far in Spanish, German, and French. In addition, this model, fully representing the principles of interactive activation between the three representation layers of letter, syllable, and word units, proved successful in correctly parsing all presented stimuli contained in its lexicon with different types of syllabic structures. Multiple regression analyses on the model's output and on human data confirmed the model's ability to still account for effects of word frequency and orthographic neighbourhood like the original MROM (Grainger & Jacobs, 1996) it was derived from.","PeriodicalId":88321,"journal":{"name":"The European journal of cognitive psychology","volume":"7 1","pages":"861 - 893"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87822219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 44
期刊
The European journal of cognitive psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1