首页 > 最新文献

Human ethology bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Passing on the Chair 传递椅子
Pub Date : 2017-06-30 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/3122/001-004
E. Oberzaucher
{"title":"Passing on the Chair","authors":"E. Oberzaucher","doi":"10.22330/HEB/3122/001-004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/3122/001-004","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"1-4"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48002836","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multiscale Analysis of Masculinity – An Exploratory Study 男子气概的多尺度分析-一项探索性研究
Pub Date : 2017-06-30 DOI: 10.22330/heb/322/005-013
Luca Kozma, Ferenc Kocsor
In the present study we investigated the connection between male dominance and masculinity. We used questionnaires to discover how self-rated dominance, identification with masculinity ideologies, aggression, and attitudes towards women, are related. Our analyses revealed that the two different gender role questionnaires we used (Bem’s Sex Role Inventory, Multicultural Masculine Ideology Scale) may focus on different aspects of masculinity, which was also reflected in their different relations to other variables – aggression, dominance, and sexism. We briefly review the main concepts related to gender roles, and discuss the results of the analyses within the frames of Social Role Theory.
在本研究中,我们调查了男性支配地位和男子气概之间的关系。我们使用问卷调查来发现自我评定的支配地位、对男性意识形态的认同、攻击性和对女性的态度是如何相关的。我们的分析表明,我们使用的两种不同的性别角色问卷(Bem 's Sex role Inventory和多元文化男性意识形态量表)可能侧重于男性气质的不同方面,这也反映在它们与其他变量的不同关系上——攻击、支配和性别歧视。我们简要回顾了与性别角色相关的主要概念,并在社会角色理论的框架内讨论了分析结果。
{"title":"Multiscale Analysis of Masculinity – An Exploratory Study","authors":"Luca Kozma, Ferenc Kocsor","doi":"10.22330/heb/322/005-013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/heb/322/005-013","url":null,"abstract":"In the present study we investigated the connection between male dominance and masculinity. We used questionnaires to discover how self-rated dominance, identification with masculinity ideologies, aggression, and attitudes towards women, are related. Our analyses revealed that the two different gender role questionnaires we used (Bem’s Sex Role Inventory, Multicultural Masculine Ideology Scale) may focus on different aspects of masculinity, which was also reflected in their different relations to other variables – aggression, dominance, and sexism. We briefly review the main concepts related to gender roles, and discuss the results of the analyses within the frames of Social Role Theory.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"5-13"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68344820","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding Variation in Reactions to Displays of Allegiance 理解对效忠表现的反应变化
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/321/017-028
D. Kruger, Michele M. Day, Ailiya Duan, Anna M. Heyblom, Dóra Juhász, Stephanie L. Misevich, Camile Phaneuf, Claire M. Saunders, Peter A. Sonnega, Vibra Sreenivasa
Sports team rivalries involve expressions of evolved psychology related to in-group loyalty and inter-group competition. ESPN ranked the University of Michigan–Ohio State University football rivalry as the greatest North American sports rivalry. Toledo, Ohio is geographically closer to Ann Arbor, MI (UM), than to Columbus, OH (OSU) and conventional wisdom holds that team loyalty is divided among local residents. Previous observational research of thousands of individuals in Toledo indicated that no one simultaneously wore apparel from the two competing teams. Inspired by these observations, a second study examined reactions to displays of mixed loyalty vs. consistent loyalty. When a research confederate wore clothing featuring both UM and OSU, he elicited more attention and reactions than when wearing equivalent outfits featuring just one of the universities. The current study examines factors explaining individual differences in attention to displays of allegiance to rival groups, whether consistent or mixed. We made several predictions for explaining variation in reaction rates based on evolved coalitional psychology. We predicted that men, young adults in the typical undergraduate age range (18-25), and those wearing university merchandise themselves will have higher reaction rates to the confederate than women, individuals in other age groups, and individuals not wearing university merchandise respectively. These hypotheses were generally supported by our observations (n = 1292). Controlling for experimental condition, reaction rates were higher overall for men, young adults, and those wearing university merchandise themselves. The highest reaction rates were by young adult men wearing university merchandise, to the mixed condition.
体育团队竞争涉及到与群体内忠诚和群体间竞争相关的进化心理的表达。ESPN将密歇根大学和俄亥俄州立大学的橄榄球比赛列为北美最伟大的体育比赛。托莱多,俄亥俄州在地理上更接近安娜堡,密歇根州(UM),而不是哥伦布市,俄亥俄州(OSU),传统观点认为,球队的忠诚度在当地居民中是有分歧的。先前对托莱多数千人的观察研究表明,没有人同时穿着两个竞争团队的服装。受这些观察结果的启发,第二项研究考察了对混合忠诚和一致忠诚的反应。当一名研究参与者同时穿着印有密歇根大学和俄勒冈州立大学图案的衣服时,他比只穿印有其中一所大学图案的衣服时引起了更多的关注和反应。目前的研究考察了一些因素,这些因素解释了个体对敌对团体忠诚表现的关注差异,无论是一致的还是混合的。基于进化的联合心理学,我们做出了几个预测来解释反应速率的变化。我们预测,男性、典型本科年龄段的年轻人(18-25岁)和那些自己穿着大学商品的人对同盟者的反应率分别高于女性、其他年龄段的个人和不穿大学商品的个人。我们的观察结果普遍支持这些假设(n = 1292)。在控制实验条件下,男性、年轻人和自己穿着大学服装的人的总体反应率更高。穿着大学服装的年轻成年男性对混合条件的反应率最高。
{"title":"Understanding Variation in Reactions to Displays of Allegiance","authors":"D. Kruger, Michele M. Day, Ailiya Duan, Anna M. Heyblom, Dóra Juhász, Stephanie L. Misevich, Camile Phaneuf, Claire M. Saunders, Peter A. Sonnega, Vibra Sreenivasa","doi":"10.22330/HEB/321/017-028","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/321/017-028","url":null,"abstract":"Sports team rivalries involve expressions of evolved psychology related to in-group loyalty and inter-group competition. ESPN ranked the University of Michigan–Ohio State University football rivalry as the greatest North American sports rivalry. Toledo, Ohio is geographically closer to Ann Arbor, MI (UM), than to Columbus, OH (OSU) and conventional wisdom holds that team loyalty is divided among local residents. Previous observational research of thousands of individuals in Toledo indicated that no one simultaneously wore apparel from the two competing teams. Inspired by these observations, a second study examined reactions to displays of mixed loyalty vs. consistent loyalty. When a research confederate wore clothing featuring both UM and OSU, he elicited more attention and reactions than when wearing equivalent outfits featuring just one of the universities. The current study examines factors explaining individual differences in attention to displays of allegiance to rival groups, whether consistent or mixed. We made several predictions for explaining variation in reaction rates based on evolved coalitional psychology. We predicted that men, young adults in the typical undergraduate age range (18-25), and those wearing university merchandise themselves will have higher reaction rates to the confederate than women, individuals in other age groups, and individuals not wearing university merchandise respectively. These hypotheses were generally supported by our observations (n = 1292). Controlling for experimental condition, reaction rates were higher overall for men, young adults, and those wearing university merchandise themselves. The highest reaction rates were by young adult men wearing university merchandise, to the mixed condition.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"17-28"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43730408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior 研究合作与竞争育儿行为整合的理论建议
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/321/006-016
M. Fisher
Over the course of evolutionary history, women have faced situations where they presumably have needed to both compete and cooperate with the same group of kin and non-kin individuals for the purposes of advancing reproductive success. Here we focus exclusively on mothers, and explore some of the advantages of cooperating with other mothers (as well as other individuals) versus competing with other mothers (and others). There has been noteworthy attention paid towards cooperative mothering behavior, but competitive mothering behavior has been relatively neglected. Therefore, our goal is to provide a review of the ways in which both types of behavior enhance a mother’s reproductive success, and ultimately, show that the most beneficial strategy is integrate both cooperative and competitive behavior.
在进化史上,女性面临着这样的情况:她们可能需要与同一组亲属和非亲属竞争和合作,以促进生殖成功。在这里,我们只关注母亲,并探讨与其他母亲(以及其他人)合作与与与其他母亲竞争的一些优势。合作型育儿行为一直备受关注,但竞争型育儿行为却相对被忽视。因此,我们的目标是回顾这两种行为如何提高母亲的生育成功率,并最终表明最有益的策略是整合合作和竞争行为。
{"title":"A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior","authors":"M. Fisher","doi":"10.22330/HEB/321/006-016","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/321/006-016","url":null,"abstract":"Over the course of evolutionary history, women have faced situations where they presumably have needed to both compete and cooperate with the same group of kin and non-kin individuals for the purposes of advancing reproductive success. Here we focus exclusively on mothers, and explore some of the advantages of cooperating with other mothers (as well as other individuals) versus competing with other mothers (and others). There has been noteworthy attention paid towards cooperative mothering behavior, but competitive mothering behavior has been relatively neglected. Therefore, our goal is to provide a review of the ways in which both types of behavior enhance a mother’s reproductive success, and ultimately, show that the most beneficial strategy is integrate both cooperative and competitive behavior.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"6-16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42634748","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
A Highland fling to remember: A short report on the XXIII biennial congress on Human Ethology in Stirling, Scotland 值得纪念的高地狂欢:苏格兰斯特灵第二十三届两年一度的人类民族学大会的简短报告
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/heb/321/001-005
C. Allen
The University of Stirling, nestled between the Scottish cities of Glasgow and Edinburgh, was the stage for the 23rd Biennial Congress on Human Ethology, in August 2016. In true ISHE spirit, the organising committee were keen to create as many opportunities as possible for delegates to immerse themselves in Scottish culture and tradition, and organised a packed calendar of social events. This began with an opening ceremony with a piper, and live performances of traditional Scottish dancing by the Stirling Highland Dancers (with socially mandated audience participation, see figure 1!), This was followed by dinner and whiskey tasting overlooking the beautiful University campus, cloaked in the shadow of the Ochil Hills.
斯特灵大学位于苏格兰城市格拉斯哥和爱丁堡之间,是2016年8月第23届人类人种学双年展的舞台。本着真正的ISHE精神,组委会热衷于为代表们创造尽可能多的机会,让他们沉浸在苏格兰文化和传统中,并组织了一系列社交活动。首先是由风笛手主持的开幕式,以及斯特灵高地舞者的苏格兰传统舞蹈现场表演(有社会要求的观众参与,见图1!),然后是晚餐和威士忌品尝,俯瞰着美丽的大学校园,笼罩在奥奇尔山的阴影下。
{"title":"A Highland fling to remember: A short report on the XXIII biennial congress on Human Ethology in Stirling, Scotland","authors":"C. Allen","doi":"10.22330/heb/321/001-005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/heb/321/001-005","url":null,"abstract":"The University of Stirling, nestled between the Scottish cities of Glasgow and Edinburgh, was the stage for the 23rd Biennial Congress on Human Ethology, in August 2016. In true ISHE spirit, the organising committee were keen to create as many opportunities as possible for delegates to immerse themselves in Scottish culture and tradition, and organised a packed calendar of social events. This began with an opening ceremony with a piper, and live performances of traditional Scottish dancing by the Stirling Highland Dancers (with socially mandated audience participation, see figure 1!), This was followed by dinner and whiskey tasting overlooking the beautiful University campus, cloaked in the shadow of the Ochil Hills.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"1-5"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42024882","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Female Gaze Behaviour, Status and the Menstrual Cycle: An Exploratory Study 女性凝视行为、地位与月经周期的探索性研究
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/321/029-041
M. Sharp, Geordan E. Hamilton
Whilst not explaining all social behaviour and organisation, dominance is nevertheless an important dimension of human social interaction. It has been hypothesized that gaze behaviour reflects the dominance hierarchy of primate groups and several studies have demonstrated that cues associated with social status also influence human gaze. These studies overwhelmingly involve male participants and sex differences in coalition formation, status seeking, risk taking and dominance-related behaviour have all been explained by the fact that males needed to compete for mates while females supposedly did not. Indeed, until comparatively recently it was thought that dominance contests were of little consequence for females. However, there is increasing evidence to suggest that competitiveness is useful for females and there are attendant benefits conferred upon those with high status. Thus, it is important to understand these status-gaze relationships in women. Sixty heterosexual, caucasian female participants (mean age=22.1, SD=3.16) competed in a dyadic non-physical status encounter (woodblock game Jenga). Dyads were unknown to each other and matched by menstrual phase ( follicular/luteal) or contraceptive use. Winners were congratulated and received £5 to accentuate status disparity. In the two minutes following competition participants sat apart but in sight of each other. The investigator left the room and gaze behaviour was recorded by video camera. The length of time each participant spent looking at their opponent was related to competition outcome: winners (M=3.96, SD=1.72), losers (M=7.34, SD=4.32). There were main effects for outcome, F(1,54)=22.47, p<0.0001 and menstrual phase/contraceptive use F(2,54)=5.34, p=0.008. Follicular and Luteal phase did not differ from each other significantly. The interaction between outcome and menstrual phase/ contraceptive use was significant, F(2,54)=8.75, p<0.001, with contraceptive use reversing the findings of longer gaze time in the loss condition in normally cycling women. Our results support the hypothesis that female gaze behaviour is responsive to the outcome of status encounters.
虽然不能解释所有的社会行为和组织,但支配地位仍然是人类社会互动的一个重要方面。有人假设凝视行为反映了灵长类动物群体的优势等级,几项研究表明,与社会地位相关的线索也会影响人类的凝视。这些研究绝大多数涉及男性参与者,在联盟形成、地位寻求、冒险和支配相关行为方面的性别差异都可以解释为男性需要竞争配偶,而女性则不需要。事实上,直到最近,人们还认为支配地位的竞争对女性来说意义不大。然而,越来越多的证据表明,竞争力对女性有用,地位高的人也会得到相应的好处。因此,了解女性的这些地位凝视关系是很重要的。60名异性恋、高加索女性参与者(平均年龄=22.1,SD=3.16)参加了一场二人非身体状态的比赛(木版游戏Jenga)。死亡人数彼此未知,并根据月经期(卵泡/黄体)或避孕使用情况进行匹配。获奖者受到祝贺,并获得5英镑,以突出地位差距。在接下来的两分钟里,参赛者分开坐着,但彼此看在眼里。调查员离开了房间,摄像机记录下了他们的凝视行为。每个参与者看对手的时间长度与比赛结果有关:获胜者(M=3.96,SD=1.72),失败者(M=7.34,SD=4.32)。对结果有主要影响,F(1,54)=22.47,p<0.0001,月经期/避孕药具使用F(2,54)=5.34,p=0.008。卵泡期和黄体期没有显著差异。结果与月经期/避孕药具使用之间的相互作用是显著的,F(2,54)=8.75,p<0.001,避孕药具的使用逆转了正常骑自行车女性在丧失状态下凝视时间较长的发现。我们的研究结果支持了这样一种假设,即女性的凝视行为对地位遭遇的结果有反应。
{"title":"Female Gaze Behaviour, Status and the Menstrual Cycle: An Exploratory Study","authors":"M. Sharp, Geordan E. Hamilton","doi":"10.22330/HEB/321/029-041","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/321/029-041","url":null,"abstract":"Whilst not explaining all social behaviour and organisation, dominance is nevertheless an important dimension of human social interaction. It has been hypothesized that gaze behaviour reflects the dominance hierarchy of primate groups and several studies have demonstrated that cues associated with social status also influence human gaze. These studies overwhelmingly involve male participants and sex differences in coalition formation, status seeking, risk taking and dominance-related behaviour have all been explained by the fact that males needed to compete for mates while females supposedly did not. Indeed, until comparatively recently it was thought that dominance contests were of little consequence for females. However, there is increasing evidence to suggest that competitiveness is useful for females and there are attendant benefits conferred upon those with high status. Thus, it is important to understand these status-gaze relationships in women. Sixty heterosexual, caucasian female participants (mean age=22.1, SD=3.16) competed in a dyadic non-physical status encounter (woodblock game Jenga). Dyads were unknown to each other and matched by menstrual phase ( follicular/luteal) or contraceptive use. Winners were congratulated and received £5 to accentuate status disparity. In the two minutes following competition participants sat apart but in sight of each other. The investigator left the room and gaze behaviour was recorded by video camera. The length of time each participant spent looking at their opponent was related to competition outcome: winners (M=3.96, SD=1.72), losers (M=7.34, SD=4.32). There were main effects for outcome, F(1,54)=22.47, p<0.0001 and menstrual phase/contraceptive use F(2,54)=5.34, p=0.008. Follicular and Luteal phase did not differ from each other significantly. The interaction between outcome and menstrual phase/ contraceptive use was significant, F(2,54)=8.75, p<0.001, with contraceptive use reversing the findings of longer gaze time in the loss condition in normally cycling women. Our results support the hypothesis that female gaze behaviour is responsive to the outcome of status encounters.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"29-41"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48747861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior 考察合作与竞争母亲行为整合的理论建议
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/321/053-059
T. Pollet, A. Little
{"title":"A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior","authors":"T. Pollet, A. Little","doi":"10.22330/HEB/321/053-059","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/321/053-059","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"53-59"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43672600","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Mutual olfactory recognition between mother and child 母子之间的嗅觉相互识别
Pub Date : 2017-03-31 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/321/042-052
Susan C. Roberts, Faize Eryaman
The ability of infants to recognize their mother is an important factor in the development of mother-infant social relationships. Infants must be able to distinguish her from other individuals before they form strong maternal attachment, and learning individual characteristics of the child likely helps to cement the mother’s emotional bonding with the child. Existing evidence demonstrates that very young infants can discriminate their mother’s odour and that this facilitates the onset and duration of breastfeeding, but it is not known whether this ability is maintained after weaning. Here, we investigated recognition of mothers by children of toddler age (3 – 5 years), and maternal recognition of her child, through body odour. Nineteen mother-child pairs wore clean t-shirts for 2 consecutive nights, and both mothers and children were then tested for correct identification of their respective mother/ child’s odour from an odour line-up of 6 samples. We found that mothers were able to recognise their child’s odour at rates above chance, but toddlers were not. Neither breastfeeding duration nor hours spent together on an average day were associated with correct odour recognition by either mothers or children. However, higher perceived pleasantness of their child’s odour during testing was associated with higher identification success, suggesting a possible cue to correct identification in mothers. Mothers who correctly identified their child’s odour were also more likely to correctly identify the sex of odour donors. Our study contributes to the growing literature suggesting that odour may be important in maternal-child attachment.
婴儿识别母亲的能力是母婴社会关系发展的一个重要因素。在形成强烈的母亲依恋之前,婴儿必须能够将她与其他个体区分开来,而了解孩子的个体特征可能有助于巩固母亲与孩子的情感联系。现有证据表明,非常年幼的婴儿能够辨别母亲的气味,这有助于母乳喂养的开始和持续时间,但尚不清楚这种能力是否在断奶后保持不变。在这里,我们调查了幼儿(3 - 5岁)对母亲的识别,以及母亲通过体味对孩子的识别。19对母亲和孩子连续两晚穿着干净的t恤,然后测试母亲和孩子从6个气味样本中正确识别各自母亲/孩子的气味。我们发现,母亲能够识别孩子气味的几率高于偶然,但蹒跚学步的孩子却不能。母乳喂养的持续时间和平均每天在一起的时间都与母亲或孩子对气味的正确识别无关。然而,在测试过程中,对孩子气味的感知程度越高,识别成功率就越高,这表明母亲们可能会做出正确的识别。能正确识别孩子气味的母亲也更有可能正确识别气味供体的性别。我们的研究有助于越来越多的文献表明,气味可能在母子依恋中很重要。
{"title":"Mutual olfactory recognition between mother and child","authors":"Susan C. Roberts, Faize Eryaman","doi":"10.22330/HEB/321/042-052","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/321/042-052","url":null,"abstract":"The ability of infants to recognize their mother is an important factor in the development of mother-infant social relationships. Infants must be able to distinguish her from other individuals before they form strong maternal attachment, and learning individual characteristics of the child likely helps to cement the mother’s emotional bonding with the child. Existing evidence demonstrates that very young infants can discriminate their mother’s odour and that this facilitates the onset and duration of breastfeeding, but it is not known whether this ability is maintained after weaning. Here, we investigated recognition of mothers by children of toddler age (3 – 5 years), and maternal recognition of her child, through body odour. Nineteen mother-child pairs wore clean t-shirts for 2 consecutive nights, and both mothers and children were then tested for correct identification of their respective mother/ child’s odour from an odour line-up of 6 samples. We found that mothers were able to recognise their child’s odour at rates above chance, but toddlers were not. Neither breastfeeding duration nor hours spent together on an average day were associated with correct odour recognition by either mothers or children. However, higher perceived pleasantness of their child’s odour during testing was associated with higher identification success, suggesting a possible cue to correct identification in mothers. Mothers who correctly identified their child’s odour were also more likely to correctly identify the sex of odour donors. Our study contributes to the growing literature suggesting that odour may be important in maternal-child attachment.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"32 1","pages":"42-52"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48798667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Mentalistic and Scientific Stories about Human Behavior, Biomimetic Heuristics and Psychology's Confusions 关于人类行为的心理主义和科学故事、仿生启发式和心理学困惑
Pub Date : 2016-12-30 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/314/015-033
J. Richer
WHAT THIS PAPER IS ABOUT This paper is about a number of connected issues: • Mentalism and why and how we are a mentalistic species. • Why there are two, equally important, stories about human behaviour, the mentalistic and scientific, whose differences need to be clearly understood. • Why the mentalistic story of human behaviour is flawed starting place for a scientific study of ourselves, though absolutely necessary for everyday life. That necessity and utility has seduced many psychologists erroneously to start their would-be science with mentalistic concepts. • And yet, in the manner of those practicing biomimetics, how scientists can learn from evolved mentalism, and in particular the pervasiveness of the mentalistic understanding people at the level of motivations (feelings, intentions, etc.), and work on the heuristic expectation that patterns in behaviour will be found, not at the behavioural level, but at the level of motivation albeit inferred from the observed behaviour.
这篇论文是关于一些相互关联的问题:•唯心主义,以及我们为什么和如何成为一个唯心主义物种。•为什么有两个同样重要的关于人类行为的故事,心理主义的和科学的,它们的差异需要被清楚地理解。•为什么关于人类行为的唯心主义故事是对我们自己进行科学研究的有缺陷的起点,尽管对日常生活来说是绝对必要的。这种必要性和实用性诱使许多心理学家错误地以唯心主义的概念开始他们未来的科学。•然而,在那些实践仿生学的人的方式中,科学家如何从进化的心灵主义中学习,特别是在动机(感觉,意图等)层面上普遍存在的心灵主义理解人们,并致力于启发式期望,即行为模式将被发现,而不是在行为层面,而是在动机层面,尽管是从观察到的行为推断出来的。
{"title":"Mentalistic and Scientific Stories about Human Behavior, Biomimetic Heuristics and Psychology's Confusions","authors":"J. Richer","doi":"10.22330/HEB/314/015-033","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/314/015-033","url":null,"abstract":"WHAT THIS PAPER IS ABOUT This paper is about a number of connected issues: • Mentalism and why and how we are a mentalistic species. • Why there are two, equally important, stories about human behaviour, the mentalistic and scientific, whose differences need to be clearly understood. • Why the mentalistic story of human behaviour is flawed starting place for a scientific study of ourselves, though absolutely necessary for everyday life. That necessity and utility has seduced many psychologists erroneously to start their would-be science with mentalistic concepts. • And yet, in the manner of those practicing biomimetics, how scientists can learn from evolved mentalism, and in particular the pervasiveness of the mentalistic understanding people at the level of motivations (feelings, intentions, etc.), and work on the heuristic expectation that patterns in behaviour will be found, not at the behavioural level, but at the level of motivation albeit inferred from the observed behaviour.","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"31 1","pages":"15-33"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68344742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Compromises - The Limits of Democracy 妥协——民主的极限
Pub Date : 2016-12-30 DOI: 10.22330/HEB/314/001-004
E. Oberzaucher
{"title":"Compromises - The Limits of Democracy","authors":"E. Oberzaucher","doi":"10.22330/HEB/314/001-004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HEB/314/001-004","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"31 1","pages":"1-4"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68344942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Human ethology bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1