首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Cynical people desire power but rarely acquire it: Exploring the role of cynicism in leadership attainment 愤世嫉俗的人渴望权力,但很少获得权力:探索愤世嫉俗在领导成就中的作用。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12685
Olga Stavrova, Daniel Ehlebracht, Dongning Ren

Do cynical individuals have a stronger desire for power and are they more likely to acquire power at work? The negative consequences of cynicism—for cynics themselves and the people around them—render the examination of these questions particularly important. We first examined the role of cynicism in power motives. Results showed that more cynical individuals have a greater desire for power to avoid exploitation by others (and less so to exploit others; Study 1) and score higher on dominance (but not prestige or leadership) motives (Study 2). The subsequent two studies examined the role of cynicism in power attainment at work. A study of virtual teams (Study 3) showed that more cynical individuals were less likely to emerge as group leaders, and a prospective study of ~9000 employees followed for up to 10 years (Study 4) showed that cynicism predicted a lower likelihood of attaining a leadership position in organizations. Taken together, more (vs. less) cynical individuals have a stronger power—in particular, dominance—motive but they are not more successful at power acquisition. These findings inform the literature on cynicism and power and highlight the importance of cynical worldviews for leadership attainment.

愤世嫉俗的人是否对权力有更强的欲望,他们是否更有可能在工作中获得权力?玩世不恭对愤世嫉俗者自己和周围人的负面影响使对这些问题的审查变得尤为重要。我们首先考察了犬儒主义在权力动机中的作用。结果表明,愤世嫉俗的人更渴望权力,以避免被他人剥削(而剥削他人的欲望则更低;研究1),在支配(但不是声望或领导)动机上得分更高(研究2)。随后的两项研究考察了犬儒主义在工作中获得权力的作用。一项针对虚拟团队的研究(研究3)表明,愤世嫉俗的人不太可能成为团队领导者,一项针对约9000名员工的前瞻性研究跟踪了长达10年 年(研究4)表明,犬儒主义预测在组织中获得领导职位的可能性较低。总的来说,更多(而不是更少)愤世嫉俗的人有更强的权力,特别是支配动机,但他们在权力获取方面并不更成功。这些发现为有关犬儒主义和权力的文献提供了信息,并强调了犬儒主义世界观对领导才能的重要性。
{"title":"Cynical people desire power but rarely acquire it: Exploring the role of cynicism in leadership attainment","authors":"Olga Stavrova,&nbsp;Daniel Ehlebracht,&nbsp;Dongning Ren","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12685","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12685","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Do cynical individuals have a stronger desire for power and are they more likely to acquire power at work? The negative consequences of cynicism—for cynics themselves and the people around them—render the examination of these questions particularly important. We first examined the role of cynicism in power motives. Results showed that more cynical individuals have a greater desire for power to avoid exploitation by others (and less so to exploit others; Study 1) and score higher on dominance (but not prestige or leadership) motives (Study 2). The subsequent two studies examined the role of cynicism in power attainment at work. A study of virtual teams (Study 3) showed that more cynical individuals were less likely to emerge as group leaders, and a prospective study of ~9000 employees followed for up to 10 years (Study 4) showed that cynicism predicted a lower likelihood of attaining a leadership position in organizations. Taken together, more (vs. less) cynical individuals have a stronger power—in particular, dominance—motive but they are not more successful at power acquisition. These findings inform the literature on cynicism and power and highlight the importance of cynical worldviews for leadership attainment.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"226-252"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12685","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71420877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Musicality – Tuned to the melody of vocal emotions 音乐性-与声乐情感的旋律相适应。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12684
Christine Nussbaum, Annett Schirmer, Stefan R. Schweinberger

Musicians outperform non-musicians in vocal emotion perception, likely because of increased sensitivity to acoustic cues, such as fundamental frequency (F0) and timbre. Yet, how musicians make use of these acoustic cues to perceive emotions, and how they might differ from non-musicians, is unclear. To address these points, we created vocal stimuli that conveyed happiness, fear, pleasure or sadness, either in all acoustic cues, or selectively in either F0 or timbre only. We then compared vocal emotion perception performance between professional/semi-professional musicians (N = 39) and non-musicians (N = 38), all socialized in Western music culture. Compared to non-musicians, musicians classified vocal emotions more accurately. This advantage was seen in the full and F0-modulated conditions, but was absent in the timbre-modulated condition indicating that musicians excel at perceiving the melody (F0), but not the timbre of vocal emotions. Further, F0 seemed more important than timbre for the recognition of all emotional categories. Additional exploratory analyses revealed a link between time-varying F0 perception in music and voices that was independent of musical training. Together, these findings suggest that musicians are particularly tuned to the melody of vocal emotions, presumably due to a natural predisposition to exploit melodic patterns.

音乐家在声乐情感感知方面优于非音乐家,这可能是因为他们对基频(F0)和音色等声学线索的敏感性增加。然而,音乐家如何利用这些声学线索来感知情绪,以及他们与非音乐家有何不同,目前尚不清楚。为了解决这些问题,我们创造了传达快乐、恐惧、快乐或悲伤的声音刺激,无论是在所有的声学线索中,还是选择性地只在F0或音色中。然后,我们比较了专业/半专业音乐家的声乐情感感知表现(N = 39)和非音乐家(N = 38),都是在西方音乐文化中社会化的。与非音乐家相比,音乐家对声乐情感的分类更准确。这种优势在完全和F0调制的条件下可以看到,但在音色调制的条件中却没有,这表明音乐家擅长感知旋律(F0),但不擅长感知声乐情感的音色。此外,对于所有情感类别的识别,F0似乎比音色更重要。额外的探索性分析揭示了音乐中时变F0感知与独立于音乐训练的声音之间的联系。总之,这些发现表明,音乐家特别喜欢声乐情感的旋律,可能是因为他们天生就有利用旋律模式的倾向。
{"title":"Musicality – Tuned to the melody of vocal emotions","authors":"Christine Nussbaum,&nbsp;Annett Schirmer,&nbsp;Stefan R. Schweinberger","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12684","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12684","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Musicians outperform non-musicians in vocal emotion perception, likely because of increased sensitivity to acoustic cues, such as fundamental frequency (F0) and timbre. Yet, how musicians make use of these acoustic cues to perceive emotions, and how they might differ from non-musicians, is unclear. To address these points, we created vocal stimuli that conveyed happiness, fear, pleasure or sadness, either in all acoustic cues, or selectively in either F0 or timbre only. We then compared vocal emotion perception performance between professional/semi-professional musicians (<i>N</i> = 39) and non-musicians (<i>N</i> = 38), all socialized in Western music culture. Compared to non-musicians, musicians classified vocal emotions more accurately. This advantage was seen in the full and F0-modulated conditions, but was absent in the timbre-modulated condition indicating that musicians excel at perceiving the melody (F0), but not the timbre of vocal emotions. Further, F0 seemed more important than timbre for the recognition of all emotional categories. Additional exploratory analyses revealed a link between time-varying F0 perception in music and voices that was independent of musical training. Together, these findings suggest that musicians are particularly tuned to the melody of vocal emotions, presumably due to a natural predisposition to exploit melodic patterns.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"206-225"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12684","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41232470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Curious about threats: Morbid curiosity and interest in conspiracy theories in US adults 对威胁充满好奇美国成年人对阴谋论的病态好奇和兴趣
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12682
Coltan Scrivner, Joseph M. Stubbersfield

Conspiracy theories allege secret plots between two or more powerful actors to achieve an outcome, sometimes explaining important events or proposing alternative understandings of reality in opposition to mainstream accounts, and commonly highlight the threat presented by the plot and its conspirators. Research in psychology proposes that belief in conspiracy theories is motivated by a desire to understand threats and is predicted by increased anxiety. Morbid curiosity describes the tendency to seek out information about threatening or dangerous situations and is associated with an interest in threat-related entertainment and increased anxiety. Across three studies, we investigated the relationship between morbid curiosity and conspiracy theories in US-based samples. We found that higher trait morbid curiosity was associated with higher general conspiracist beliefs (Study 1) and the perceived threat of conspiratorial explanations of events (Study 2). Using a behavioural choice paradigm, we found that participants who chose to investigate morbidly curious stimuli were more likely to choose to learn about conspiratorial explanations for events (Study 3). Greater curiosity about the minds of dangerous people was consistently the strongest predictor of conspiratorial ideation and interest. These results suggest that morbid curiosity is an important but hitherto unstudied predictor of conspiratorial interest and belief.

阴谋论指控两个或两个以上有权势的行为者之间为达到某种目的而进行的秘密阴谋,有时解释重要事件或提出与主流说法相反的对现实的另一种理解,并通常强调阴谋及其阴谋者所带来的威胁。心理学研究表明,相信阴谋论的动机是希望了解威胁,并预测焦虑会增加。病态好奇心描述了一种寻找有关威胁或危险情况信息的倾向,与对威胁相关娱乐活动的兴趣和焦虑增加有关。通过三项研究,我们调查了美国样本中病态好奇心与阴谋论之间的关系。我们发现,较高的病态好奇心与较高的一般阴谋论信念(研究 1)和对阴谋论解释事件的感知威胁(研究 2)有关。通过行为选择范式,我们发现选择调查病态好奇心刺激的参与者更有可能选择了解阴谋论对事件的解释(研究 3)。对危险人物思想的更大好奇心一直是阴谋论思想和兴趣的最强预测因素。这些结果表明,病态好奇心是预测阴谋论兴趣和信念的一个重要因素,但迄今为止尚未进行过研究。
{"title":"Curious about threats: Morbid curiosity and interest in conspiracy theories in US adults","authors":"Coltan Scrivner,&nbsp;Joseph M. Stubbersfield","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12682","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12682","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Conspiracy theories allege secret plots between two or more powerful actors to achieve an outcome, sometimes explaining important events or proposing alternative understandings of reality in opposition to mainstream accounts, and commonly highlight the threat presented by the plot and its conspirators. Research in psychology proposes that belief in conspiracy theories is motivated by a desire to understand threats and is predicted by increased anxiety. Morbid curiosity describes the tendency to seek out information about threatening or dangerous situations and is associated with an interest in threat-related entertainment and increased anxiety. Across three studies, we investigated the relationship between morbid curiosity and conspiracy theories in US-based samples. We found that higher trait morbid curiosity was associated with higher general conspiracist beliefs (Study 1) and the perceived threat of conspiratorial explanations of events (Study 2). Using a behavioural choice paradigm, we found that participants who chose to investigate morbidly curious stimuli were more likely to choose to learn about conspiratorial explanations for events (Study 3). Greater curiosity about the minds of dangerous people was consistently the strongest predictor of conspiratorial ideation and interest. These results suggest that morbid curiosity is an important but hitherto unstudied predictor of conspiratorial interest and belief.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"129-147"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12682","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134960485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self as a prior: The malleability of Bayesian multisensory integration to social salience 作为先验的自我:贝叶斯多感官整合对社会显著性的延展性。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12683
Meike Scheller, Huilin Fang, Jie Sui

Our everyday perceptual experiences are grounded in the integration of information within and across our senses. Due to this direct behavioural relevance, cross-modal integration retains a certain degree of contextual flexibility, even to social relevance. However, how social relevance modulates cross-modal integration remains unclear. To investigate possible mechanisms, Experiment 1 tested the principles of audio-visual integration for numerosity estimation by deriving a Bayesian optimal observer model with perceptual prior from empirical data to explain perceptual biases. Such perceptual priors may shift towards locations of high salience in the stimulus space. Our results showed that the tendency to over- or underestimate numerosity, expressed in the frequency and strength of fission and fusion illusions, depended on the actual event numerosity. Experiment 2 replicated the effects of social relevance on multisensory integration from Scheller & Sui, 2022 JEP:HPP, using a lower number of events, thereby favouring the opposite illusion through enhanced influences of the prior. In line with the idea that the self acts like a prior, the more frequently observed illusion (more malleable to prior influences) was modulated by self-relevance. Our findings suggest that the self can influence perception by acting like a prior in cue integration, biasing perceptual estimates towards areas of high self-relevance.

我们的日常感知体验建立在我们感官内部和感官之间信息的整合之上。由于这种直接的行为相关性,跨模态整合保留了一定程度的上下文灵活性,甚至与社会相关性有关。然而,社会相关性如何调节跨模态整合仍不清楚。为了研究可能的机制,实验1通过从经验数据中导出具有感知先验的贝叶斯最优观测器模型来解释感知偏差,从而测试了数字估计的视听集成原理。这样的感知先验可以向刺激空间中的高显著性的位置移动。我们的结果表明,过度或低估数量的倾向,表现为裂变和聚变错觉的频率和强度,取决于实际事件的数量。实验2复制了Scheller&Sui,2022 JEP:HPP中的社会相关性对多感官整合的影响,使用了较低数量的事件,从而通过增强先前的影响来支持相反的幻觉。与自我行为像先验的观点一致,更频繁观察到的幻觉(更容易受到先验影响)受到自我相关性的调节。我们的研究结果表明,自我可以通过在线索整合中表现得像先验来影响感知,将感知估计偏向于高自我相关性的区域。
{"title":"Self as a prior: The malleability of Bayesian multisensory integration to social salience","authors":"Meike Scheller,&nbsp;Huilin Fang,&nbsp;Jie Sui","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12683","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12683","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Our everyday perceptual experiences are grounded in the integration of information within and across our senses. Due to this direct behavioural relevance, cross-modal integration retains a certain degree of contextual flexibility, even to social relevance. However, how social relevance modulates cross-modal integration remains unclear. To investigate possible mechanisms, Experiment 1 tested the principles of audio-visual integration for numerosity estimation by deriving a Bayesian optimal observer model with perceptual prior from empirical data to explain perceptual biases. Such perceptual priors may shift towards locations of high salience in the stimulus space. Our results showed that the tendency to over- or underestimate numerosity, expressed in the frequency and strength of fission and fusion illusions, depended on the actual event numerosity. Experiment 2 replicated the effects of social relevance on multisensory integration from Scheller &amp; Sui, 2022 <i>JEP:HPP</i>, using a lower number of events, thereby favouring the opposite illusion through enhanced influences of the prior. In line with the idea that the self acts like a prior, the more frequently observed illusion (more malleable to prior influences) was modulated by self-relevance. Our findings suggest that the self can influence perception by acting like a prior in cue integration, biasing perceptual estimates towards areas of high self-relevance.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"185-205"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12683","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41111860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mood induction through imitation of full-body movements with different affective intentions 通过模仿具有不同情感意图的全身动作来诱导情绪。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12681
Eva-Madeleine Schmidt, Rebecca A. Smith, Andrés Fernández, Birte Emmermann, Julia F. Christensen

Theories of human emotion, including some emotion embodiment theories, suggest that our moods and affective states are reflected in the movements of our bodies. We used the reverse process for mood regulation; modulate body movements to regulate mood. Dancing is a type of full-body movement characterized by affective expressivity and, hence, offers the possibility to express different affective states through the same movement sequences. We tested whether the repeated imitation of a dancer performing two simple full-body dance movement sequences with different affective expressivity (happy or sad) could change mood states. Computer-based systems, using avatars as dance models to imitate, offer a series of advantages such as independence from physical contact and location. Therefore, we compared mood induction effects in two conditions: participants were asked to imitate dance movements from one of the two avatars showing: (a) videos of a human dancer model or (b) videos of a robot dancer model. The mood induction was successful for both happy and sad imitations, regardless of condition (human vs. robot avatar dance model). Moreover, the magnitude of happy mood induction and how much participants liked the task predicted work-related motivation after the mood induction. We conclude that mood regulation through dance movements is possible and beneficial in the work context.

人类情感理论,包括一些情感体现理论,表明我们的情绪和情感状态反映在我们身体的运动中。我们使用了相反的过程来调节情绪;调节身体运动以调节情绪。舞蹈是一种以情感表现为特征的全身运动,因此,它提供了通过相同的动作序列表达不同情感状态的可能性。我们测试了重复模仿舞者表演两个简单的全身舞蹈动作序列是否会改变情绪状态,这两个动作序列具有不同的情感表现力(快乐或悲伤)。基于计算机的系统使用化身作为舞蹈模型进行模仿,提供了一系列优势,如独立于身体接触和位置。因此,我们比较了两种情况下的情绪诱导效果:参与者被要求模仿两个化身中的一个的舞蹈动作:(a)人类舞者模型的视频或(b)机器人舞者模型的图像。情绪诱导对于快乐和悲伤的模仿都是成功的,无论条件如何(人类与机器人化身舞蹈模型)。此外,快乐情绪诱导的程度和参与者对任务的喜爱程度预测了情绪诱导后的工作动机。我们得出的结论是,通过舞蹈动作来调节情绪在工作环境中是可能的,也是有益的。
{"title":"Mood induction through imitation of full-body movements with different affective intentions","authors":"Eva-Madeleine Schmidt,&nbsp;Rebecca A. Smith,&nbsp;Andrés Fernández,&nbsp;Birte Emmermann,&nbsp;Julia F. Christensen","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12681","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12681","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Theories of human emotion, including some emotion embodiment theories, suggest that our moods and affective states are reflected in the movements of our bodies. We used the reverse process for mood regulation; modulate body movements to regulate mood. Dancing is a type of full-body movement characterized by affective expressivity and, hence, offers the possibility to express different affective states through the same movement sequences. We tested whether the repeated imitation of a dancer performing two simple full-body dance movement sequences with different affective expressivity (happy or sad) could change mood states. Computer-based systems, using avatars as dance models to imitate, offer a series of advantages such as independence from physical contact and location. Therefore, we compared mood induction effects in two conditions: participants were asked to imitate dance movements from one of the two avatars showing: (a) videos of a human dancer model or (b) videos of a robot dancer model. The mood induction was successful for both happy and sad imitations, regardless of condition (human vs. robot avatar dance model). Moreover, the magnitude of happy mood induction and how much participants liked the task predicted work-related motivation after the mood induction. We conclude that mood regulation through dance movements is possible and beneficial in the work context.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"148-180"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12681","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41106247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The moderating roles of resilience and social support in the relationships between bullying victimization and well-being among Chinese adolescents: Evidence from PISA 2018 抗逆力和社会支持在中国青少年遭受欺凌与幸福感关系中的调节作用:来自2018年国际学生评估项目的证据。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-08-26 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12678
Ying Zhang, Hao Li, Gaowei Chen, Bo Li, Na Li, Xin Zhou

Bullying victimization can undermine adolescents' well-being. However, few studies have comprehensively investigated the contributions of various victimization forms to well-being and compared which forms were more harmful than others. Evidence on whether resilience and social support moderate such associations is also limited. Using a sample of 12,058 Chinese adolescents in the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2018, this cross-sectional study aimed to (1) investigate the associations of physical, verbal and relational victimization with well-being; (2) compare the strengths of these associations; and (3) examine the moderating roles of resilience and teacher and parent support in these associations. Results showed that three victimization forms were associated with poorer well-being. Relational and physical victimization were more harmful to most studied well-being outcomes than verbal victimization. Furthermore, resilience weakened the negative effects of physical victimization on negative affect and life satisfaction but aggravated the negative effects of verbal victimization on both outcomes and the negative effect of relational victimization on school belonging. Teacher support intensified the negative effects of physical victimization on school belonging. Parent support was not effective in regulating the victimization–well-being association. The findings underscored the detrimental effects of bullying victimization on adolescents' well-being and the potentially harmful sides of resilience and social support. Implications for bullying prevention were discussed.

欺凌行为会损害青少年的幸福感。然而,很少有研究全面调查了各种受害形式对幸福感的影响,并比较了哪些形式比其他形式更有害。关于抗逆力和社会支持是否会缓和这种关联的证据也很有限。本横断面研究以2018年国际学生评估项目(PISA)中的12 058名中国青少年为样本,旨在:(1)调查身体、语言和关系伤害与幸福感的关联;(2)比较这些关联的强度;(3)研究复原力和教师及家长支持在这些关联中的调节作用。结果显示,三种受害形式都与较差的幸福感有关。与言语伤害相比,关系伤害和身体伤害对大多数研究的幸福感结果危害更大。此外,复原力削弱了身体伤害对消极情绪和生活满意度的负面影响,但加剧了言语伤害对这两项结果的负面影响以及关系伤害对学校归属感的负面影响。教师的支持加剧了身体伤害对学校归属感的负面影响。家长的支持在调节受害与幸福感之间的关联方面并不起作用。研究结果强调了欺凌受害对青少年福祉的不利影响,以及复原力和社会支持的潜在危害。研究还讨论了预防欺凌的意义。
{"title":"The moderating roles of resilience and social support in the relationships between bullying victimization and well-being among Chinese adolescents: Evidence from PISA 2018","authors":"Ying Zhang,&nbsp;Hao Li,&nbsp;Gaowei Chen,&nbsp;Bo Li,&nbsp;Na Li,&nbsp;Xin Zhou","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12678","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12678","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Bullying victimization can undermine adolescents' well-being. However, few studies have comprehensively investigated the contributions of various victimization forms to well-being and compared which forms were more harmful than others. Evidence on whether resilience and social support moderate such associations is also limited. Using a sample of 12,058 Chinese adolescents in the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2018, this cross-sectional study aimed to (1) investigate the associations of physical, verbal and relational victimization with well-being; (2) compare the strengths of these associations; and (3) examine the moderating roles of resilience and teacher and parent support in these associations. Results showed that three victimization forms were associated with poorer well-being. Relational and physical victimization were more harmful to most studied well-being outcomes than verbal victimization. Furthermore, resilience weakened the negative effects of physical victimization on negative affect and life satisfaction but aggravated the negative effects of verbal victimization on both outcomes and the negative effect of relational victimization on school belonging. Teacher support intensified the negative effects of physical victimization on school belonging. Parent support was not effective in regulating the victimization–well-being association. The findings underscored the detrimental effects of bullying victimization on adolescents' well-being and the potentially harmful sides of resilience and social support. Implications for bullying prevention were discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"66-89"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12678","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10078565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral reasoning behind the veil of ignorance: An investigation into perspective-taking accessibility in the context of autonomous vehicles 无知面纱下的道德推理:自动驾驶汽车背景下的透视可及性调查。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-08-26 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12679
Giovanni Bruno, Andrea Spoto, Michela Sarlo, Lorella Lotto, Alex Marson, Nicola Cellini, Simone Cutini

Perspective-taking (PT) accessibility has been recognized as an important factor in affecting moral reasoning, also playing a non-trivial role in moral investigation towards autonomous vehicles (AVs). A new proposal to deepen this effect leverages the principles of the veil of ignorance (VOI), as a moral reasoning device aimed to control self-interested decisions by limiting the access to specific perspectives and to potentially biased information. Throughout two studies, we deepen the role of VOI reasoning in the moral perception of AVs, disclosing personal and contingent information progressively throughout the experiment. With the use of the moral trilemma paradigm, two different VOI conditions were operationalized, inspired by the Original Position theory by John Rawls and the Equiprobability Model by John Harsanyi. Evidence suggests a significant role of VOI reasoning in affecting moral reasoning, which seems not independent from the order in which information is revealed. Coherently, a detrimental effect of self-involvement on utilitarian behaviours was detected. These results highlight the importance of considering PT accessibility and self-involvement when investigating moral attitudes towards AVs, since it can help the intelligibility of general concerns and hesitations towards this new technology.

视角选择(PT)的可及性已被认为是影响道德推理的一个重要因素,在对自动驾驶汽车(AV)的道德调查中也发挥着非同小可的作用。为深化这一影响,我们提出了一项新建议,即利用 "无知的面纱"(VOI)原则,将其作为一种道德推理工具,旨在通过限制特定视角和潜在偏差信息的获取来控制自利决策。通过两项研究,我们深化了 "无知推理 "在反车辆道德认知中的作用,在整个实验过程中逐步披露个人信息和偶然信息。受约翰-罗尔斯(John Rawls)的 "原初立场"(Original Position)理论和约翰-哈桑尼(John Harsanyi)的 "等概率模型"(Equiprobability Model)的启发,我们使用道德三难范式(moral trilemma paradigm),对两种不同的VOI条件进行了操作。有证据表明,VOI 推理在影响道德推理方面起着重要作用,这似乎与信息揭示的顺序无关。同时,还发现了自我卷入对功利行为的不利影响。这些结果凸显了在调查对 AV 的道德态度时考虑 PT 可及性和自我参与的重要性,因为这有助于理解对这一新技术的普遍担忧和犹豫。
{"title":"Moral reasoning behind the veil of ignorance: An investigation into perspective-taking accessibility in the context of autonomous vehicles","authors":"Giovanni Bruno,&nbsp;Andrea Spoto,&nbsp;Michela Sarlo,&nbsp;Lorella Lotto,&nbsp;Alex Marson,&nbsp;Nicola Cellini,&nbsp;Simone Cutini","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12679","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12679","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Perspective-taking (PT) accessibility has been recognized as an important factor in affecting moral reasoning, also playing a non-trivial role in moral investigation towards autonomous vehicles (AVs). A new proposal to deepen this effect leverages the principles of the veil of ignorance (VOI), as a moral reasoning device aimed to control self-interested decisions by limiting the access to specific perspectives and to potentially biased information. Throughout two studies, we deepen the role of VOI reasoning in the moral perception of AVs, disclosing personal and contingent information progressively throughout the experiment. With the use of the moral trilemma paradigm, two different VOI conditions were operationalized, inspired by the Original Position theory by John Rawls and the Equiprobability Model by John Harsanyi. Evidence suggests a significant role of VOI reasoning in affecting moral reasoning, which seems not independent from the order in which information is revealed. Coherently, a detrimental effect of self-involvement on utilitarian behaviours was detected. These results highlight the importance of considering PT accessibility and self-involvement when investigating moral attitudes towards AVs, since it can help the intelligibility of general concerns and hesitations towards this new technology.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"90-114"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12679","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10450915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Life motion signals bias the perception of apparent motion direction 生命运动信号会使人们对表面运动方向的感知产生偏差。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-08-25 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12680
Yiping Ge, Yiwen Yu, Suqi Huang, Xinyi Huang, Li Wang, Yi Jiang

Walking direction conveyed by biological motion (BM) cues, which humans are highly sensitive to since birth, can elicit involuntary shifts of attention to enhance the detection of static targets. Here, we demonstrated that such intrinsic sensitivity to walking direction could also modulate the direction perception of simultaneously presented dynamic stimuli. We showed that the perceived direction of apparent motion was biased towards the walking direction even though observers had been informed in advance that the walking direction of BM did not predict the apparent motion direction. In particular, rightward BM cues had an advantage over leftward BM cues in altering the perception of motion direction. Intriguingly, this perceptual bias disappeared when BM cues were shown inverted, or when the critical biological characteristics were removed from the cues. Critically, both the perceptual direction bias and the rightward advantage persisted even when only local BM cues were presented without any global configuration. Furthermore, the rightward advantage was found to be specific to social cues (i.e., BM), as it vanished when non-social cues (i.e., arrows) were utilized. Taken together, these findings support the existence of a specific processing mechanism for life motion signals and shed new light on their influences in a dynamic environment.

人类自出生起就对生物运动(BM)线索所传达的行走方向高度敏感,这种线索可以引起注意力的不自主转移,从而增强对静态目标的检测。在这里,我们证明了这种对行走方向的内在敏感性也能调节对同时出现的动态刺激的方向感知。我们的研究表明,即使事先告知观察者 BM 的行走方向并不能预测明显的运动方向,观察者感知到的明显运动方向也会偏向行走方向。特别是,在改变运动方向感知方面,向右的 BM 提示比向左的 BM 提示更有优势。耐人寻味的是,当 BM 提示倒置显示,或从提示中去除关键的生物特征时,这种感知偏差就会消失。重要的是,即使只显示局部 BM 提示而不显示任何全局配置,感知方向偏差和右向优势也依然存在。此外,研究还发现右向优势是社会线索(即 BM)所特有的,因为当使用非社会线索(即箭头)时,这种优势就会消失。综上所述,这些研究结果支持生命运动信号存在一种特定的处理机制,并对其在动态环境中的影响产生了新的启示。
{"title":"Life motion signals bias the perception of apparent motion direction","authors":"Yiping Ge,&nbsp;Yiwen Yu,&nbsp;Suqi Huang,&nbsp;Xinyi Huang,&nbsp;Li Wang,&nbsp;Yi Jiang","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12680","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12680","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Walking direction conveyed by biological motion (BM) cues, which humans are highly sensitive to since birth, can elicit involuntary shifts of attention to enhance the detection of static targets. Here, we demonstrated that such intrinsic sensitivity to walking direction could also modulate the direction perception of simultaneously presented dynamic stimuli. We showed that the perceived direction of apparent motion was biased towards the walking direction even though observers had been informed in advance that the walking direction of BM did not predict the apparent motion direction. In particular, rightward BM cues had an advantage over leftward BM cues in altering the perception of motion direction. Intriguingly, this perceptual bias disappeared when BM cues were shown inverted, or when the critical biological characteristics were removed from the cues. Critically, both the perceptual direction bias and the rightward advantage persisted even when only local BM cues were presented without any global configuration. Furthermore, the rightward advantage was found to be specific to social cues (i.e., BM), as it vanished when non-social cues (i.e., arrows) were utilized. Taken together, these findings support the existence of a specific processing mechanism for life motion signals and shed new light on their influences in a dynamic environment.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"115-128"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10068504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A test of the Morality-Agency-Communion (MAC) model of respect and liking across positive and negative traits 对道德机构沟通(MAC)模型在积极和消极特征之间的尊重和喜欢的测试。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-08-21 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12677
Andrew Prestwich

The Morality-Agency-Communion (MAC) model of respect and liking suggests that traits linked with morality are important for respect and liking; traits related to competence or assertiveness are important for respect and traits related to warmth are important for liking. However, tests of this model have tended not to consider traits related to immorality, incompetence, lack of assertiveness or coldness. This study addressed this issue by utilizing a within-subjects design in which participants were required to rate their respect and liking for individuals with specific trait types across four categories (moral; competence; assertiveness; and warmth) at three levels (positive, negative and neutral). The central tenets of the MAC model were supported for ‘positive’ traits (morality, competence, assertiveness and warmth). However, for ‘negative’ traits (immorality, incompetence and lack of assertiveness), individuals were similarly not liked and not respected. Individuals who were cold were respected more than liked. The findings of this study extend the MAC model by indicating that the amount that individuals are respected versus liked depends not only on trait type but also whether a trait is positive or negative.

尊重和喜欢的道德机构公报(MAC)模型表明,与道德相关的特质对尊重和喜欢很重要;与能力或自信相关的特质对尊重很重要,与温暖相关的特质对于喜欢很重要。然而,该模型的测试往往不考虑与不道德、无能、缺乏自信或冷漠有关的特征。这项研究通过使用受试者内部设计来解决这个问题,在该设计中,参与者被要求在三个水平(积极、消极和中立)上对四个类别(道德、能力、自信和热情)中具有特定特质类型的人的尊重和喜爱程度进行评分。MAC模型的核心原则得到了“积极”特质(道德、能力、自信和热情)的支持。然而,对于“负面”特征(不道德、无能和缺乏自信),个人同样不受欢迎和尊重。冷漠的人受到的尊重多于喜欢。这项研究的结果扩展了MAC模型,表明个人受到尊重与喜爱的程度不仅取决于特质类型,还取决于特质是积极的还是消极的。
{"title":"A test of the Morality-Agency-Communion (MAC) model of respect and liking across positive and negative traits","authors":"Andrew Prestwich","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12677","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12677","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Morality-Agency-Communion (MAC) model of respect and liking suggests that traits linked with morality are important for respect and liking; traits related to competence or assertiveness are important for respect and traits related to warmth are important for liking. However, tests of this model have tended not to consider traits related to immorality, incompetence, lack of assertiveness or coldness. This study addressed this issue by utilizing a within-subjects design in which participants were required to rate their respect and liking for individuals with specific trait types across four categories (moral; competence; assertiveness; and warmth) at three levels (positive, negative and neutral). The central tenets of the MAC model were supported for ‘positive’ traits (morality, competence, assertiveness and warmth). However, for ‘negative’ traits (immorality, incompetence and lack of assertiveness), individuals were similarly not liked and not respected. Individuals who were cold were respected more than liked. The findings of this study extend the MAC model by indicating that the amount that individuals are respected versus liked depends not only on trait type but also whether a trait is positive or negative.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"51-65"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12677","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10089533","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attentional asymmetries in peripheral vision 周边视觉的注意不对称。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-15 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12676
Stefanie Klatt, Benjamin Noël, Robin Schrödter

Previous research on the use of peripheral vision to identify two spatially separated stimuli simultaneously has led to the conclusion that the focus of attention has the form of a symmetric ellipse with a broader expansion along the horizontal compared to the vertical meridian. However, research on pseudoneglect has indicated that attention is not symmetrically distributed to the whole visual field. Here, we test if the attention window is indeed symmetrical with regard to its shape and resolution during peripheral vision. The results indicate that the position of those stimuli relative to the focus of attention influences the ability to identify a given set of stimuli. Specifically, stimuli presented to the left and top of the fixation point were more frequently identified correctly compared to those presented to the right bottom. That is, the attention window is rather not symmetric, which must be considered in future studies on the nature of the focus of attention.

以往关于利用周边视觉同时识别两个空间上分离的刺激物的研究得出的结论是,注意焦点呈对称椭圆形,沿水平经线的扩展范围大于垂直经线。然而,对假性近视的研究表明,注意力并不是对称地分布在整个视野中。在此,我们测试了在周边视觉时,注意力窗口的形状和分辨率是否真的对称。结果表明,这些刺激物相对于注意力焦点的位置会影响识别一组特定刺激物的能力。具体来说,与右下方的刺激物相比,定点左侧和上方的刺激物更容易被正确识别。也就是说,注意窗口并不是对称的,这一点在今后有关注意焦点性质的研究中必须加以考虑。
{"title":"Attentional asymmetries in peripheral vision","authors":"Stefanie Klatt,&nbsp;Benjamin Noël,&nbsp;Robin Schrödter","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12676","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12676","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research on the use of peripheral vision to identify two spatially separated stimuli simultaneously has led to the conclusion that the focus of attention has the form of a symmetric ellipse with a broader expansion along the horizontal compared to the vertical meridian. However, research on pseudoneglect has indicated that attention is not symmetrically distributed to the whole visual field. Here, we test if the attention window is indeed symmetrical with regard to its shape and resolution during peripheral vision. The results indicate that the position of those stimuli relative to the focus of attention influences the ability to identify a given set of stimuli. Specifically, stimuli presented to the left and top of the fixation point were more frequently identified correctly compared to those presented to the right bottom. That is, the attention window is rather not symmetric, which must be considered in future studies on the nature of the focus of attention.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"40-50"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12676","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9834919","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1