首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Preserving the placebo effect after disclosure: A new perspective on non-deceptive placebos 在信息披露后保留安慰剂效应:非欺骗性安慰剂的新视角。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12696
Mehran Emadi Andani, Diletta Barbiani, Marco Bonetto, Rudy Menegaldo, Bernardo Villa-Sánchez, Mirta Fiorio

The present study explores whether a particular style of placebo disclosure could serve as a tool to foster a renewed trust in one's own inherent resources and elicit a meaningful placebo effect. In a motor performance task, two placebo groups received inert transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) in each of four sessions along with information on its force-enhancing properties. Before the final session, one of the placebo groups was informed about the placebo, which was portrayed as a means to unleash an inherent potential. Along with force, we systematically monitored task-specific self-efficacy to test whether this variable would be differentially modulated in the two placebo groups. Compared to two control groups, placebo groups showed higher force and self-efficacy in the last session. No differences in self-efficacy were observed in the placebo groups even after revealing the placebo procedure, suggesting that the disclosure was effective in ‘safeguarding’ individuals' self-efficacy. These findings may have important implications, paving the way for the use of placebos that not only are ethically permissible but also support individuals' self-efficacy.

本研究探讨了一种特殊的安慰剂披露方式能否作为一种工具,促进人们对自身固有资源的重新信任,并引发有意义的安慰剂效应。在一项运动表现任务中,两个安慰剂组分别在四个疗程中接受了惰性经皮神经电刺激(TENS)以及有关其增强力量特性的信息。在最后一个疗程之前,其中一组安慰剂被告知安慰剂是一种释放内在潜能的手段。除了力量,我们还系统地监测了特定任务的自我效能感,以检验这一变量是否会在两个安慰剂组中受到不同程度的调节。与两个对照组相比,安慰剂组在最后一次训练中表现出更高的力量和自我效能感。即使在揭示了安慰剂程序之后,也没有观察到安慰剂组在自我效能感方面的差异,这表明揭示程序在 "保护 "个人自我效能感方面是有效的。这些发现可能具有重要意义,为使用安慰剂铺平了道路,安慰剂不仅在伦理上是允许的,而且还能支持个人的自我效能感。
{"title":"Preserving the placebo effect after disclosure: A new perspective on non-deceptive placebos","authors":"Mehran Emadi Andani,&nbsp;Diletta Barbiani,&nbsp;Marco Bonetto,&nbsp;Rudy Menegaldo,&nbsp;Bernardo Villa-Sánchez,&nbsp;Mirta Fiorio","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12696","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12696","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The present study explores whether a particular style of placebo disclosure could serve as a tool to foster a renewed trust in one's own inherent resources and elicit a meaningful placebo effect. In a motor performance task, two placebo groups received inert transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) in each of four sessions along with information on its force-enhancing properties. Before the final session, one of the placebo groups was informed about the placebo, which was portrayed as a means to unleash an inherent potential. Along with force, we systematically monitored task-specific self-efficacy to test whether this variable would be differentially modulated in the two placebo groups. Compared to two control groups, placebo groups showed higher force and self-efficacy in the last session. No differences in self-efficacy were observed in the placebo groups even after revealing the placebo procedure, suggesting that the disclosure was effective in ‘safeguarding’ individuals' self-efficacy. These findings may have important implications, paving the way for the use of placebos that not only are ethically permissible but also support individuals' self-efficacy.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 3","pages":"437-453"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139472222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding anxiety through uncertainty quantification. 通过不确定性量化了解焦虑。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12693
Friederike Elisabeth Hedley, Emmett Larsen, Aprajita Mohanty, Jeremiah Zhe Liu, Jingwen Jin

Uncertainty has been a central concept in psychological theories of anxiety. However, this concept has been plagued by divergent connotations and operationalizations. The lack of consensus hinders the current search for cognitive and biological mechanisms of anxiety, jeopardizes theory creation and comparison, and restrains translation of basic research into improved diagnoses and interventions. Drawing upon uncertainty decomposition in Bayesian Decision Theory, we propose a well-defined conceptual structure of uncertainty in cognitive and clinical sciences, with a focus on anxiety. We discuss how this conceptual structure provides clarity and can be naturally applied to existing frameworks of psychopathology research. Furthermore, it allows formal quantification of various types of uncertainty that can benefit both research and clinical practice in the era of computational psychiatry.

不确定性一直是焦虑心理学理论的核心概念。然而,这一概念的内涵和操作方法却一直众说纷纭。缺乏共识阻碍了目前对焦虑的认知和生物机制的探索,影响了理论的创建和比较,并阻碍了将基础研究转化为改进诊断和干预措施。借鉴贝叶斯决策理论(Bayesian Decision Theory)中的不确定性分解,我们提出了认知科学和临床科学中定义明确的不确定性概念结构,重点关注焦虑问题。我们将讨论这一概念结构如何提供清晰度,并自然地应用于现有的精神病理学研究框架。此外,它还允许对各种类型的不确定性进行正式量化,这对计算精神病学时代的研究和临床实践都大有裨益。
{"title":"Understanding anxiety through uncertainty quantification.","authors":"Friederike Elisabeth Hedley, Emmett Larsen, Aprajita Mohanty, Jeremiah Zhe Liu, Jingwen Jin","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12693","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12693","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Uncertainty has been a central concept in psychological theories of anxiety. However, this concept has been plagued by divergent connotations and operationalizations. The lack of consensus hinders the current search for cognitive and biological mechanisms of anxiety, jeopardizes theory creation and comparison, and restrains translation of basic research into improved diagnoses and interventions. Drawing upon uncertainty decomposition in Bayesian Decision Theory, we propose a well-defined conceptual structure of uncertainty in cognitive and clinical sciences, with a focus on anxiety. We discuss how this conceptual structure provides clarity and can be naturally applied to existing frameworks of psychopathology research. Furthermore, it allows formal quantification of various types of uncertainty that can benefit both research and clinical practice in the era of computational psychiatry.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139432124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing the cognitive performance of action video game players and age-matched controls following a cognitively fatiguing task: A stage 2 registered report 比较动作电子游戏玩家和年龄匹配对照组在完成认知疲劳任务后的认知表现:第二阶段注册报告。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12692
Mark J. Campbell, Sarah C. Cregan, John M. Joyce, Magdalena Kowal, Adam J. Toth

Recent work demonstrates that those who regularly play action video games (AVGs) consistently outperform non-gamer (NG) controls on tests of various cognitive abilities. AVGs place high demands on several cognitive functions and are often engaged with for long periods of time (e.g., over 2 h), predisposing players to experiencing cognitive fatigue. The detrimental effects of cognitive fatigue have been widely studied in various contexts where accurate performance is crucial, including aviation, military, and sport. Even though AVG players may be prone to experiencing cognitive fatigue, this topic has received little research attention to date. In this study, we compared the effect of a cognitively fatiguing task on the subsequent cognitive performance of action video game players and NG control participants. Our results indicated AVGs showed superior spatial working memory and complex attention abilities while showing no difference from NGs on simple attention performance. Additionally, we found that our cognitive fatigue and control interventions did not differentially affect the cognitive performance of AVGs and NGs in this study. This pre-registered study provides evidence that AVGs show superior cognitive abilities in comparison to a non-gaming population, but do not appear more resilient to cognitive fatigue.

最近的研究表明,经常玩动作电子游戏(AVGs)的人在各种认知能力测试中的表现一直优于非游戏玩家(NG)对照组。动作电子游戏对多种认知功能的要求很高,而且通常需要长时间(如超过 2 小时)进行,因此容易导致玩家出现认知疲劳。认知疲劳的有害影响已在航空、军事和体育等各种对准确表现至关重要的环境中得到广泛研究。尽管 AVG 运动员可能容易出现认知疲劳,但迄今为止,这一主题很少受到研究关注。在本研究中,我们比较了认知疲劳任务对动作视频游戏玩家和 NG 对照组参与者后续认知表现的影响。研究结果表明,动作电子游戏玩家在空间工作记忆和复杂注意力方面表现出更强的能力,而在简单注意力方面与普通玩家没有差异。此外,我们还发现,在本研究中,我们的认知疲劳干预和控制干预并未对 AVGs 和 NGs 的认知表现产生不同影响。这项预注册研究提供的证据表明,与非游戏人群相比,AVGs 表现出更出色的认知能力,但他们似乎对认知疲劳没有更强的适应能力。
{"title":"Comparing the cognitive performance of action video game players and age-matched controls following a cognitively fatiguing task: A stage 2 registered report","authors":"Mark J. Campbell,&nbsp;Sarah C. Cregan,&nbsp;John M. Joyce,&nbsp;Magdalena Kowal,&nbsp;Adam J. Toth","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12692","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12692","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent work demonstrates that those who regularly play action video games (AVGs) consistently outperform non-gamer (NG) controls on tests of various cognitive abilities. AVGs place high demands on several cognitive functions and are often engaged with for long periods of time (e.g., over 2 h), predisposing players to experiencing cognitive fatigue. The detrimental effects of cognitive fatigue have been widely studied in various contexts where accurate performance is crucial, including aviation, military, and sport. Even though AVG players may be prone to experiencing cognitive fatigue, this topic has received little research attention to date. In this study, we compared the effect of a cognitively fatiguing task on the subsequent cognitive performance of action video game players and NG control participants. Our results indicated AVGs showed superior spatial working memory and complex attention abilities while showing no difference from NGs on simple attention performance. Additionally, we found that our cognitive fatigue and control interventions did not differentially affect the cognitive performance of AVGs and NGs in this study. This pre-registered study provides evidence that AVGs show superior cognitive abilities in comparison to a non-gaming population, but do not appear more resilient to cognitive fatigue.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 3","pages":"363-385"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12692","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138884483","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Satisfied on our own, yet ready to leave together: An actor–partner interdependence mediation model on job satisfaction and turnover intentions in leader–follower dyads 对自己满意,却准备一起离开:领导者-追随者二人组中工作满意度和离职意向的行动者-伙伴相互依赖调解模型。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12694
Marjolein C. J. Caniëls, Petru L. Curseu

Drawing on conservation of resources theory and job embeddedness, this study aims to investigate crossovers of positivity and negativiy between leaders and their followers with respect to work-related variables (i.e. work engagement, emotional exhaustion, job satisfaction and turnover intentions). Two waves of multisource data were collected from 244 leader–follower dyads. An actor–partner interdependence model extended to mediation (APIMeM) was used to test two mediation models and examine crossovers between leaders and their followers. Findings show that negative events are positively associated with emotional exhaustion of leaders (followers), which in turn is positively associated with leaders' (followers') own turnover intention as well as that of their followers (leaders). Positive events are positively associated with work engagement of leaders, which in turn is positively associated with leaders' own job satisfaction, but not with the job satisfaction of their followers. In other words, negative events have cross-over effects, while positive events do not. In essence, we found evidence of a crossovered negativity bias. This study is unique in using a dyadic approach to analyse leader–follower crossovers with respect to job satisfaction and turnover intention. This study reveals the mediating and cross-over effects of work engagement and emotional exhaustion on the links between positive (negative) events and work outcomes.

本研究以资源保护理论和工作嵌入性为基础,旨在调查领导者与其追随者在工作相关变量(即工作投入度、情感衰竭、工作满意度和离职意向)方面的积极性和消极性交叉。我们从 244 个领导者-追随者二元组中收集了两波多源数据。研究采用了扩展到中介的行为者-伙伴相互依赖模型(APIMeM)来检验两个中介模型,并考察了领导者与追随者之间的交叉影响。研究结果表明,负面事件与领导者(追随者)的情感衰竭呈正相关,而情感衰竭又与领导者(追随者)自身及其追随者(领导者)的离职意向呈正相关。积极事件与领导者的工作参与度呈正相关,而工作参与度又与领导者自身的工作满意度呈正相关,但与追随者的工作满意度无关。换句话说,负面事件具有交叉效应,而正面事件则没有。从本质上讲,我们发现了交叉消极偏差的证据。本研究的独特之处在于,它采用了二元方法来分析领导者与追随者在工作满意度和离职意向方面的交叉影响。本研究揭示了工作投入和情感衰竭对积极(消极)事件与工作结果之间联系的中介和交叉效应。
{"title":"Satisfied on our own, yet ready to leave together: An actor–partner interdependence mediation model on job satisfaction and turnover intentions in leader–follower dyads","authors":"Marjolein C. J. Caniëls,&nbsp;Petru L. Curseu","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12694","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12694","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Drawing on conservation of resources theory and job embeddedness, this study aims to investigate crossovers of positivity and negativiy between leaders and their followers with respect to work-related variables (i.e. work engagement, emotional exhaustion, job satisfaction and turnover intentions). Two waves of multisource data were collected from 244 leader–follower dyads. An actor–partner interdependence model extended to mediation (APIMeM) was used to test two mediation models and examine crossovers between leaders and their followers. Findings show that negative events are positively associated with emotional exhaustion of leaders (followers), which in turn is positively associated with leaders' (followers') own turnover intention as well as that of their followers (leaders). Positive events are positively associated with work engagement of leaders, which in turn is positively associated with leaders' own job satisfaction, but not with the job satisfaction of their followers. In other words, negative events have cross-over effects, while positive events do not. In essence, we found evidence of a crossovered negativity bias. This study is unique in using a dyadic approach to analyse leader–follower crossovers with respect to job satisfaction and turnover intention. This study reveals the mediating and cross-over effects of work engagement and emotional exhaustion on the links between positive (negative) events and work outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 3","pages":"386-405"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12694","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138884484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial acknowledgement 编辑致谢
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-09 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12691
{"title":"Editorial acknowledgement","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12691","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12691","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 1","pages":"183-184"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139480471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Justice perceptions and well-being: Belief in a just world is a personal resource and a coping resource 公正观念和幸福:相信世界是公正的,这是一种个人资源,也是一种应对资源。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-04 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12689
Isabel Correia, Helena Carvalho, Kathleen Otto, Gabriel Nudelman

Although the perception of justice is a core need of all individuals, the adaptive value of belief in a just world (BJW)—in everyday life and when facing severe distress—has been typically investigated in separate studies. In this article, we tested, in only one study, the possibility that BJW can be a personal resource and a coping resource. We analysed data from the European Social Survey comprised of random representative samples of 27 European countries (N = 24,776 participants). We considered distressing circumstances both at an individual level (health impairment and financial difficulty) and at a macroeconomic contextual level. The results showed that for people both facing and not facing financial or health-related distress, BJW was positively associated with well-being, supporting BJW as a personal resource. Furthermore, we found that the decrease of well-being of people facing distress, both at an individual level and at a contextual level, compared to people not facing distress, was lower for individuals with higher BJW than for individuals with lower BJW, supporting BJW as a coping resource.

尽管对正义的感知是所有个体的核心需求,但在日常生活和面临严重痛苦时,对公正世界信念的适应价值(BJW)已经在不同的研究中进行了典型的调查。在本文中,我们仅在一项研究中测试了BJW作为个人资源和应对资源的可能性。我们分析了欧洲社会调查的数据,该调查由27个欧洲国家的随机代表性样本组成(N = 24,776名参与者)。我们考虑了个人层面(健康受损和财务困难)和宏观经济背景层面的痛苦情况。结果表明,对于面临或不面临财务或健康相关压力的人来说,BJW与幸福感呈正相关,支持BJW作为个人资源。此外,我们还发现,与没有面临痛苦的人相比,面对痛苦的人在个体水平和情境水平上的幸福感下降,高BJW的个体比低BJW的个体更低,支持BJW作为应对资源。
{"title":"Justice perceptions and well-being: Belief in a just world is a personal resource and a coping resource","authors":"Isabel Correia,&nbsp;Helena Carvalho,&nbsp;Kathleen Otto,&nbsp;Gabriel Nudelman","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12689","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12689","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although the perception of justice is a core need of all individuals, the adaptive value of belief in a just world (BJW)—in everyday life and when facing severe distress—has been typically investigated in separate studies. In this article, we tested, in only one study, the possibility that BJW can be a personal resource and a coping resource. We analysed data from the European Social Survey comprised of random representative samples of 27 European countries (<i>N</i> = 24,776 participants). We considered distressing circumstances both at an individual level (health impairment and financial difficulty) and at a macroeconomic contextual level. The results showed that for people both facing and not facing financial or health-related distress, BJW was positively associated with well-being, supporting BJW as a personal resource. Furthermore, we found that the decrease of well-being of people facing distress, both at an individual level and at a contextual level, compared to people not facing distress, was lower for individuals with higher BJW than for individuals with lower BJW, supporting BJW as a coping resource.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"324-344"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138482040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of conspiracy beliefs on a targeted group: Perceived popularity of Jewish-targeted conspiracy beliefs elicits outgroup avoidant behaviours 阴谋信念对目标群体的影响:对犹太人的阴谋信念的感知普及引发了外群体回避行为。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-03 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12690
Daniel Jolley, Jenny L. Paterson, Andrew McNeill

In three studies with Jewish participants, we explored the consequences of intergroup conspiracy theories on those targeted. In Study 1 (N = 250), perceived Jewish conspiracy theory popularity was positively associated with intergroup threat and negatively associated with the closeness of contact with non-Jewish people. Study 2 (n = 194) employed an experimental design where Jewish participants were exposed to the idea that many (vs. few) non-Jewish people believe in Jewish conspiracy theories. A path model demonstrated that exposure to the many (vs. few) manipulation increased intergroup threat, which was then positively associated with emotional reactions. Intergroup anxiety and ingroup anger were then positively associated with avoidance, whilst ingroup anxiety was positively associated with approach tendencies. Study 3 (n = 201) used the same experimental design, and a path model revealed that conspiracy popularity increased intergroup threat, which, in turn, was positively associated with ingroup anger and anxiety. Ingroup anxiety was then associated with intentions to help ingroup members. Notably, conspiracy popularity rendered participants less likely to interact with a non-Jewish partner in a behavioural task. Our work provides evidence that conspiracy beliefs, especially when perceived to be widely held, are likely to significantly impact targeted ingroup members.

在三个以犹太人为参与者的研究中,我们探讨了群体间阴谋论对目标人群的影响。在研究1 (N = 250)中,犹太人阴谋论的受欢迎程度与群体间威胁呈正相关,与与非犹太人接触的亲密度呈负相关。研究2 (n = 194)采用了一种实验设计,让犹太参与者了解到许多(相对于少数)非犹太人相信犹太人的阴谋论。路径模型表明,暴露于许多(相对较少)操纵增加了群体间威胁,然后与情绪反应呈正相关。群体间焦虑和群体内愤怒与回避呈正相关,而群体内焦虑与接近倾向呈正相关。研究3 (n = 201)采用相同的实验设计,路径模型显示,阴谋受欢迎程度增加了群体间威胁,而群体间威胁反过来又与群体内愤怒和焦虑呈正相关。然后,内群体焦虑与帮助内群体成员的意图有关。值得注意的是,阴谋论的受欢迎程度使参与者在行为任务中不太可能与非犹太伙伴互动。我们的工作提供了证据,证明阴谋信念,尤其是被广泛持有的阴谋信念,可能会对群体内的目标成员产生重大影响。
{"title":"The impact of conspiracy beliefs on a targeted group: Perceived popularity of Jewish-targeted conspiracy beliefs elicits outgroup avoidant behaviours","authors":"Daniel Jolley,&nbsp;Jenny L. Paterson,&nbsp;Andrew McNeill","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12690","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12690","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In three studies with Jewish participants, we explored the consequences of intergroup conspiracy theories on those targeted. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 250), perceived Jewish conspiracy theory popularity was positively associated with intergroup threat and negatively associated with the closeness of contact with non-Jewish people. Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 194) employed an experimental design where Jewish participants were exposed to the idea that many (vs. few) non-Jewish people believe in Jewish conspiracy theories. A path model demonstrated that exposure to the many (vs. few) manipulation increased intergroup threat, which was then positively associated with emotional reactions. Intergroup anxiety and ingroup anger were then positively associated with avoidance, whilst ingroup anxiety was positively associated with approach tendencies. Study 3 (<i>n</i> = 201) used the same experimental design, and a path model revealed that conspiracy popularity increased intergroup threat, which, in turn, was positively associated with ingroup anger and anxiety. Ingroup anxiety was then associated with intentions to help ingroup members. Notably, conspiracy popularity rendered participants less likely to interact with a non-Jewish partner in a behavioural task. Our work provides evidence that conspiracy beliefs, especially when perceived to be widely held, are likely to significantly impact targeted ingroup members.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"345-362"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12690","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138476736","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comparative analysis of colour–emotion associations in 16–88-year-old adults from 31 countries 来自31个国家的16-88岁成年人颜色与情绪关联的比较分析。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-02 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12687
Domicele Jonauskaite, Déborah Epicoco, Abdulrahman S. Al-rasheed, John Jamir Benzon R. Aruta, Victoria Bogushevskaya, Sanne G. Brederoo, Violeta Corona, Sergejs Fomins, Alena Gizdic, Yulia A. Griber, Jelena Havelka, Marco Hirnstein, George John, Daniela S. Jopp, Bodil Karlsson, Nikos Konstantinou, Éric Laurent, Lynn Marquardt, Philip C. Mefoh, Daniel Oberfeld, Marietta Papadatou-Pastou, Corinna M. Perchtold-Stefan, Giulia F. M. Spagnulo, Aygun Sultanova, Takumi Tanaka, Ma. Criselda Tengco-Pacquing, Mari Uusküla, Grażyna Wąsowicz, Christine Mohr

As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour–emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour–emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour–emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour.

随着年龄的增长,人们倾向于在室内度过更多的时间,而周围环境的颜色可能会对他们的情绪和整体健康产生重大影响。然而,缺乏经验证据来提供关于颜色选择的明智指导,而不考虑年龄组。为了做出明智的选择,我们调查了在年轻人身上观察到的颜色和情绪之间的联系是否也适用于老年人。我们招募了7393名参与者,年龄在16岁到88岁之间,来自31个国家。每个参与者将12个颜色术语与20个情感概念联系起来,并对每个相关情感的强度进行评级。不同的年龄组表现出高度相似的颜色-情绪关联模式(平均相似系数为0.97),具有微妙但有意义的年龄相关差异。青少年将最多但最少的积极偏见情绪与颜色联系在一起。年龄较大的参与者将数量较少但更强烈、更积极的情绪与所有颜色联系起来,显示出积极的效果。年龄也能预测觉醒和权力偏见,因肤色而异。研究结果表明,在年轻人和老年人之间,颜色与情绪的关联是相似的,只是存在细微但显著的年龄相关差异。下一步的研究应该评估颜色与情绪的关联是否反映了人们接触颜色时的真实感受。
{"title":"A comparative analysis of colour–emotion associations in 16–88-year-old adults from 31 countries","authors":"Domicele Jonauskaite,&nbsp;Déborah Epicoco,&nbsp;Abdulrahman S. Al-rasheed,&nbsp;John Jamir Benzon R. Aruta,&nbsp;Victoria Bogushevskaya,&nbsp;Sanne G. Brederoo,&nbsp;Violeta Corona,&nbsp;Sergejs Fomins,&nbsp;Alena Gizdic,&nbsp;Yulia A. Griber,&nbsp;Jelena Havelka,&nbsp;Marco Hirnstein,&nbsp;George John,&nbsp;Daniela S. Jopp,&nbsp;Bodil Karlsson,&nbsp;Nikos Konstantinou,&nbsp;Éric Laurent,&nbsp;Lynn Marquardt,&nbsp;Philip C. Mefoh,&nbsp;Daniel Oberfeld,&nbsp;Marietta Papadatou-Pastou,&nbsp;Corinna M. Perchtold-Stefan,&nbsp;Giulia F. M. Spagnulo,&nbsp;Aygun Sultanova,&nbsp;Takumi Tanaka,&nbsp;Ma. Criselda Tengco-Pacquing,&nbsp;Mari Uusküla,&nbsp;Grażyna Wąsowicz,&nbsp;Christine Mohr","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12687","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12687","url":null,"abstract":"<p>As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour–emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour–emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour–emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"275-305"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138469946","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are there gender differences in promotion–prevention self-regulatory focus? 在促进-预防自我调节焦点上是否存在性别差异?
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-11-20 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12688
Dinah Gutermuth, Melvyn R. W. Hamstra

The purpose of this research is to examine gender differences in promotion/prevention self-regulatory focus, a dispositional motivational orientation with major implications for human functioning. First, a review of literature using social cognitive theory as a framework suggests that, driven by socialization processes, (1) women may on average be more prevention focused than men – meaning more vigilant to maintain a secure status quo, whereas (2) men may on average be more promotion focused than women – meaning more eager to advance to a better situation than their status quo. Second, we provide data to examine these possible gender differences in self-regulatory focus with secondary analyses of (a) our own existing data on dispositional regulatory focus and of (b) a large scale, representative panel study (LISS Survey). The data suggest a highly consistent difference with women being more prevention focused than men, while the difference in promotion focus is much smaller and is only found in European samples. Auxiliary data suggest promotion-focused women hold less traditional gender role beliefs as well as showing that regulatory focus partially explains examples of behavioural differences between men and women. The analysis of gender difference in regulatory focus sheds new light on gender differences and biases already known, and on regulatory focus, and as such opens up many new and important areas of future inquiry.

本研究的目的是探讨促进/预防自我调节焦点的性别差异,这是一种对人类功能具有重要意义的性格动机取向。首先,一篇以社会认知理论为框架的文献综述表明,在社会化进程的驱动下,(1)女性可能平均比男性更关注预防——这意味着更警惕地维持安全现状,而(2)男性可能平均比女性更关注晋升——这意味着更渴望晋升到比现状更好的境地。其次,我们提供了数据,通过对(a)我们自己现有的关于性格调节焦点的数据和(b)大规模代表性小组研究(LISS调查)的二次分析,来检验自我调节焦点中可能存在的性别差异。数据显示,女性比男性更关注预防,而关注促销的差异要小得多,而且只在欧洲的样本中发现。辅助数据表明,以晋升为中心的女性较少持有传统的性别角色信念,同时也表明,对监管的关注部分解释了男性和女性之间行为差异的例子。对监管重点中的性别差异的分析,揭示了已知的性别差异和偏见,以及监管重点,并因此开辟了许多新的、重要的未来研究领域。
{"title":"Are there gender differences in promotion–prevention self-regulatory focus?","authors":"Dinah Gutermuth,&nbsp;Melvyn R. W. Hamstra","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12688","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12688","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The purpose of this research is to examine gender differences in promotion/prevention self-regulatory focus, a dispositional motivational orientation with major implications for human functioning. First, a review of literature using social cognitive theory as a framework suggests that, driven by socialization processes, (1) women may on average be more prevention focused than men – meaning more vigilant to maintain a secure status quo, whereas (2) men may on average be more promotion focused than women – meaning more eager to advance to a better situation than their status quo. Second, we provide data to examine these possible gender differences in self-regulatory focus with secondary analyses of (a) our own existing data on dispositional regulatory focus and of (b) a large scale, representative panel study (LISS Survey). The data suggest a highly consistent difference with women being more prevention focused than men, while the difference in promotion focus is much smaller and is only found in European samples. Auxiliary data suggest promotion-focused women hold less traditional gender role beliefs as well as showing that regulatory focus partially explains examples of behavioural differences between men and women. The analysis of gender difference in regulatory focus sheds new light on gender differences and biases already known, and on regulatory focus, and as such opens up many new and important areas of future inquiry.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"306-323"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138175636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personal pronouns and person perception – Do paired and nonbinary pronouns evoke a normative gender bias? 人称代词和人的感知——成对和非二元代词会引起规范性的性别偏见吗?
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-11-20 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12686
Emma A. Renström, Anna Lindqvist, Amanda Klysing, Marie Gustafsson Sendén

Research on gender-fair language aims to identify language inclusive to a multitude of individuals, for example, increasing the visibility of women by using paired pronouns (he/she) instead of generic masculine forms (he). However, binary presentations like he/she might come with unwanted side effects and evoke what we label as normative gender bias. A normative gender bias is defined as when words lead to stronger associations with individuals with normative gender expressions than with individuals with non-normative gender expressions, thus contributing to making non-normative individuals invisible. In three experiments, we compared the extent to which the paired pronoun he/she (Swedish and English), the neo-pronouns hen (Swedish), ze (English), and the generic pronoun singular they (English) evoked a normative gender bias. Swedish- (N = 219 and 268) and English- (N = 837, from the UK) speaking participants read about individuals referred to with the paired pronoun he/she or with hen, ze, or they. In Experiment 1 (Swedish), there was no main effect of condition on a normative bias, but in Experiment 2 (Swedish), the paired pronouns he/she evoked normative gender bias while hen did not. In Experiment 3 (English), both ze and singular they evoked normative gender bias, although normative associations were lower in these conditions compared to he/she. Furthermore, the normative bias was lower among participants who had knowledge about the use of ze as a nonbinary pronoun. Finally, neither ze nor they evoked a normative gender bias when their use was explicitly stated to be nonbinary. A potential explanation for why singular they did not generally result in less normative associations, despite almost all participants knowing about it, may include its more common use as a generic pronoun. Taken together, our results suggest that neo-pronouns, but not paired pronouns, have the potential to evoke less normative associations, but that they must be both (1) actively created new words and (2) well-known to language users as nonbinary pronouns.

对性别公平语言的研究旨在识别对众多个体具有包容性的语言,例如,通过使用成对代词(他/她)而不是通用的男性形式(他)来增加女性的知名度。然而,像他/她这样的二元表现可能会带来不必要的副作用,并引发我们称之为规范的性别偏见。规范性性别偏见的定义是,当词汇导致与规范性性别表达的个体的联系比与非规范性性别表达的个体的联系更强时,从而有助于使非规范性个体隐形。在三个实验中,我们比较了成对代词he/she(瑞典语和英语)、新代词hen(瑞典语)、ze(英语)和一般代词单数they(英语)在多大程度上引起了规范的性别偏见。在瑞典语(219人和268人)和英语(837人,来自英国)中,说英语的参与者阅读了用代词he/she或hen、ze或they连用的个人。在实验1(瑞典语)中,条件对规范性偏见没有主效应,但在实验2(瑞典语)中,他/她的成对代词引起了规范性性别偏见,而母鸡没有。在实验3(英语)中,“ze”和“单数”都诱发了规范性的性别偏见,尽管这些条件下的规范性关联比“他/她”低。此外,在了解ze作为非二元代词使用的参与者中,规范偏差较低。最后,当他们的使用被明确地声明为非二元性时,他们和ze都没有引起规范的性别偏见。尽管几乎所有的参与者都知道单数they,但单数they通常不会产生不那么规范的联想,一个潜在的解释可能包括它作为一般代词的更常见用法。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,新代词,而不是成对代词,有可能引起不太规范的联想,但它们必须同时(1)积极创造新词和(2)为语言使用者所熟知的非二元代词。
{"title":"Personal pronouns and person perception – Do paired and nonbinary pronouns evoke a normative gender bias?","authors":"Emma A. Renström,&nbsp;Anna Lindqvist,&nbsp;Amanda Klysing,&nbsp;Marie Gustafsson Sendén","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12686","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12686","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research on gender-fair language aims to identify language inclusive to a multitude of individuals, for example, increasing the visibility of women by using paired pronouns (he/she) instead of generic masculine forms (he). However, binary presentations like he/she might come with unwanted side effects and evoke what we label as normative gender bias. A normative gender bias is defined as when words lead to stronger associations with individuals with normative gender expressions than with individuals with non-normative gender expressions, thus contributing to making non-normative individuals invisible. In three experiments, we compared the extent to which the paired pronoun he/she (Swedish and English), the neo-pronouns <i>hen</i> (Swedish), <i>ze</i> (English), and the generic pronoun singular they (English) evoked a normative gender bias. Swedish- (<i>N</i> = 219 and 268) and English- (<i>N</i> = 837, from the UK) speaking participants read about individuals referred to with the paired pronoun he/she or with hen, ze, or they. In Experiment 1 (Swedish), there was no main effect of condition on a normative bias, but in Experiment 2 (Swedish), the paired pronouns he/she evoked normative gender bias while hen did not. In Experiment 3 (English), both ze and singular they evoked normative gender bias, although normative associations were lower in these conditions compared to he/she. Furthermore, the normative bias was lower among participants who had knowledge about the use of ze as a nonbinary pronoun. Finally, neither ze nor they evoked a normative gender bias when their use was explicitly stated to be nonbinary. A potential explanation for why singular they did not generally result in less normative associations, despite almost all participants knowing about it, may include its more common use as a generic pronoun. Taken together, our results suggest that neo-pronouns, but not paired pronouns, have the potential to evoke less normative associations, but that they must be both (1) actively created new words and (2) well-known to language users as nonbinary pronouns.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"115 2","pages":"253-274"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12686","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138175637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1