首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Learning linguistic diversity: Listeners have race-based linguistic expectations, but only for phonological variation. 学习语言多样性:听者有基于种族的语言期望,但仅限于语音变化。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001560
Alexandra M Ryken, Emma Tupper, Drew Weatherhead

In three artificial language experiments, we explored the rate at which adults learned associations between linguistic variation and speaker characteristics. Within each of the experiments, we observed that listeners sociolinguistic learning occurred, regardless of whether the speaker characteristic is social (race and sex/gender) or nonsocial (hat wearing), or whether they heard a phonological or morphological variant. However, we found that listener's initial expectations of what social properties were predictive of linguistic variation differed, impacting overall performance. First, participants were much more likely to assume that a phonological variant was predicted by a social property than a nonsocial property (Experiment 1). Most interestingly, participants were more likely to privilege speaker race than sex/gender, but only in the case of a phonological variant (Experiments 2 and 3). The same effect was found in both White and Black participants, though White participants were more likely to correctly articulate which speaker characteristic explained the variation, suggesting that sociolinguistic learning hinges on real-world experiences with language and social diversity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在三项人工语言实验中,我们探索了成人学习语言变化与说话者特征之间关联的速度。在每个实验中,我们都观察到,无论说话者的特征是社会性的(种族和性别)还是非社会性的(戴帽子),也无论他们听到的是语音还是形态变异,听者都会进行社会语言学习。然而,我们发现,听者最初对哪些社会属性可预测语言变异的预期不同,从而影响了整体表现。首先,与非社会属性相比,参与者更倾向于认为语音变体是由社会属性预测的(实验 1)。最有趣的是,参与者更倾向于认为说话人的种族而非性/性别,但仅限于语音变体的情况(实验 2 和 3)。在白人和黑人参与者中也发现了同样的效果,尽管白人参与者更有可能正确地说出哪种说话者的特征可以解释这种变异,这表明社会语言学的学习取决于语言和社会多样性方面的实际经验。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Learning linguistic diversity: Listeners have race-based linguistic expectations, but only for phonological variation.","authors":"Alexandra M Ryken, Emma Tupper, Drew Weatherhead","doi":"10.1037/xge0001560","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001560","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In three artificial language experiments, we explored the rate at which adults learned associations between linguistic variation and speaker characteristics. Within each of the experiments, we observed that listeners sociolinguistic learning occurred, regardless of whether the speaker characteristic is social (race and sex/gender) or nonsocial (hat wearing), or whether they heard a phonological or morphological variant. However, we found that listener's initial expectations of what social properties were predictive of linguistic variation differed, impacting overall performance. First, participants were much more likely to assume that a phonological variant was predicted by a social property than a nonsocial property (Experiment 1). Most interestingly, participants were more likely to privilege speaker race than sex/gender, but only in the case of a phonological variant (Experiments 2 and 3). The same effect was found in both White and Black participants, though White participants were more likely to correctly articulate which speaker characteristic explained the variation, suggesting that sociolinguistic learning hinges on real-world experiences with language and social diversity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2918-2930"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140174992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attributions to discrimination in multiracial contexts: Isolating the effect of target group membership. 多种族背景下的歧视归因:隔离目标群体成员的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001475
Laurie T O'Brien, Maria Casteigne, Tyler Waldon-Lee, Caley Lowe, Stefanie Simon

Historically, psychological models of how people make judgments of discrimination have relied on a binary conceptualization of intergroup relations, making it unclear how people make judgments of discrimination in diverse, multigroup contexts. We propose that groups can vary in the extent to which they fit the prototype for targets of discrimination and that this variation influences judgments of discrimination in ambiguous circumstances. The present research examined attributions to discrimination when job applicants are rejected for a white-collar position. People consistently made more attributions to discrimination (ATDs) when managers rejected Black American as compared to Asian American job applicants, and when managers rejected Asian American as compared to White American job applicants. People also made more ATDs for rejected Black American as compared to Latino American applicants, but ATDs were similar for Latino and Asian American applicants. Overall, similar patterns were observed in majority White American samples and a Black/African American sample; only an Asian American sample did not make more ATDs for rejected Black than Asian American applicants. Six experiments (N = 2,321) found strong support for the relative fit hypothesis and suggest that, in a white-collar employment context, White Americans are a distant fit to the prototype for targets of discrimination, Asian and Latino Americans are an intermediate fit, and Black Americans are a close fit. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

从历史上看,人们如何判断歧视的心理模型依赖于群体间关系的二元概念化,这使得人们在不同的多群体背景下如何判断歧视变得不清楚。我们认为,群体在多大程度上符合歧视目标的原型,这种差异会影响在模糊情况下对歧视的判断。本研究调查了白领职位求职者被拒绝时的歧视归因。当经理拒绝美国黑人求职者时,与亚裔求职者相比,以及当经理拒绝亚裔求职者时与白人求职者相比时,人们总是更多地归因于歧视。与拉丁裔美国人相比,人们为被拒绝的美国黑人申请人制作了更多的ATD,但拉丁裔和亚裔美国人的ATD相似。总体而言,在大多数美国白人样本和黑人/非裔美国人样本中观察到类似的模式;只有一个亚裔美国人的样本并没有为被拒绝的黑人申请人提供比亚裔美国人更多的ATD。六项实验(N=2321)有力地支持了相对适合假说,并表明,在白领就业背景下,美国白人与歧视目标的原型相去甚远,亚裔和拉丁裔美国人是中等适合,而美国黑人则非常适合。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Attributions to discrimination in multiracial contexts: Isolating the effect of target group membership.","authors":"Laurie T O'Brien, Maria Casteigne, Tyler Waldon-Lee, Caley Lowe, Stefanie Simon","doi":"10.1037/xge0001475","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001475","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Historically, psychological models of how people make judgments of discrimination have relied on a binary conceptualization of intergroup relations, making it unclear how people make judgments of discrimination in diverse, multigroup contexts. We propose that groups can vary in the extent to which they fit the prototype for targets of discrimination and that this variation influences judgments of discrimination in ambiguous circumstances. The present research examined attributions to discrimination when job applicants are rejected for a white-collar position. People consistently made more attributions to discrimination (ATDs) when managers rejected Black American as compared to Asian American job applicants, and when managers rejected Asian American as compared to White American job applicants. People also made more ATDs for rejected Black American as compared to Latino American applicants, but ATDs were similar for Latino and Asian American applicants. Overall, similar patterns were observed in majority White American samples and a Black/African American sample; only an Asian American sample did not make more ATDs for rejected Black than Asian American applicants. Six experiments (<i>N</i> = 2,321) found strong support for the relative fit hypothesis and suggest that, in a white-collar employment context, White Americans are a distant fit to the prototype for targets of discrimination, Asian and Latino Americans are an intermediate fit, and Black Americans are a close fit. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2751-2770"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41130625","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of environmental diversity on exploration and learning: The case of bilingualism. 环境多样性对探索和学习的影响:双语教学案例。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001562
Leher Singh, Rachel Barr, Paul C Quinn, Marina Kalashnikova, Joscelin Rocha-Hidalgo, Kate Freda, Dean D'Souza

Bilingual environments provide a commonplace example of increased complexity and uncertainty. Learning multiple languages entails mastery of a larger and more variable range of sounds, words, syntactic structures, pragmatic conventions, and more complex mapping of linguistic information to objects in the world. Recent research suggests that bilingual learners demonstrate fundamental variation in how they explore and learn from their environment, which may derive from this increased complexity. In particular, the increased complexity and variability of bilingual environments may broaden the focus of learners' attention, laying a different attentional foundation for learning. In this review, we introduce a novel framework, with accompanying empirical evidence, for understanding how early learners may adapt to a more complex environment, drawing on bilingualism as an example. Three adaptations, each relevant to the demands of abstracting structure from a complex environment, are introduced. Each adaptation is discussed in the context of empirical evidence attesting to shifts in basic psychological processes in bilingual learners. This evidence converges on the notion that bilingual learners may explore their environment more broadly. Downstream consequences of broader sampling for perception and learning are discussed. Finally, recommendations for future research to expand the scientific narrative on the impact of diverse environments on learning are provided. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

双语环境是复杂性和不确定性增加的一个常见例子。学习多种语言需要掌握更多、更多变的声音、单词、句法结构、语用习惯,以及更复杂的语言信息与世界对象的映射。最近的研究表明,双语学习者在如何探索和学习环境方面表现出基本的差异,这可能源于这种复杂性的增加。特别是,双语环境的复杂性和可变性增加,可能会扩大学习者的注意力焦点,为学习奠定不同的注意力基础。在这篇综述中,我们以双语教学为例,介绍了一个新颖的框架,并提供了相应的经验证据,以了解早期学习者如何适应更加复杂的环境。本文介绍了三种适应性,每种适应性都与从复杂环境中抽象出结构的要求相关。在讨论每种适应时,都结合了证明双语学习者基本心理过程发生变化的经验证据。这些证据都认为,双语学习者可以更广泛地探索他们的环境。我们还讨论了更广泛的取样对感知和学习的下游影响。最后,还提出了未来研究的建议,以扩大关于多样化环境对学习的影响的科学论述。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Effects of environmental diversity on exploration and learning: The case of bilingualism.","authors":"Leher Singh, Rachel Barr, Paul C Quinn, Marina Kalashnikova, Joscelin Rocha-Hidalgo, Kate Freda, Dean D'Souza","doi":"10.1037/xge0001562","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001562","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Bilingual environments provide a commonplace example of increased complexity and uncertainty. Learning multiple languages entails mastery of a larger and more variable range of sounds, words, syntactic structures, pragmatic conventions, and more complex mapping of linguistic information to objects in the world. Recent research suggests that bilingual learners demonstrate fundamental variation in how they explore and learn from their environment, which may derive from this increased complexity. In particular, the increased complexity and variability of bilingual environments may broaden the focus of learners' attention, laying a different attentional foundation for learning. In this review, we introduce a novel framework, with accompanying empirical evidence, for understanding how early learners may adapt to a more complex environment, drawing on bilingualism as an example. Three adaptations, each relevant to the demands of abstracting structure from a complex environment, are introduced. Each adaptation is discussed in the context of empirical evidence attesting to shifts in basic psychological processes in bilingual learners. This evidence converges on the notion that bilingual learners may explore their environment more broadly. Downstream consequences of broader sampling for perception and learning are discussed. Finally, recommendations for future research to expand the scientific narrative on the impact of diverse environments on learning are provided. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2879-2898"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140848915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
United States-China differences in cognition and perception across 12 tasks: Replicability, robustness, and within-culture variation. 美国和中国在 12 项任务中的认知和感知差异:可复制性、稳健性和文化内变异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001559
Anjie Cao, Alexandra Carstensen, Shan Gao, Michael C Frank

Cultural differences between the United States and China have been investigated using a broad array of psychological tasks measuring differences between cognition, language, perception, and reasoning. Using online convenience samples of adults, we conducted two large-scale replications of 12 tasks previously reported to show differences between Western and East Asian cultures. Our results showed a heterogeneous pattern of successes and failures: five tasks yielded robust cultural differences, while five showed no difference between cultures, and two showed a small difference in the opposite direction. We observed moderate reliability for all multitrial tasks, but there was little relation between task scores. As in prior work, cross-cultural differences in cognition (in those tasks showing differences) were not strongly related to explicit measures of cultural identity and behavior. All of our tasks, data, and analyses are openly available for reuse, providing a foundation for future studies that seek to establish a robust and replicable science of cross-cultural difference. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

美国和中国之间的文化差异是通过一系列测量认知、语言、感知和推理之间差异的心理任务来研究的。我们利用在线成人便利样本,对以前报道过的显示西方和东亚文化差异的 12 项任务进行了两次大规模的重复研究。我们的结果显示了成功和失败的不同模式:五项任务产生了强烈的文化差异,五项任务显示文化间没有差异,两项任务显示了反方向的微小差异。我们观察到所有多试任务都具有适度的可靠性,但任务得分之间的关系不大。与之前的研究结果一样,认知方面的跨文化差异(在那些显示差异的任务中)与文化认同和行为的明确测量结果关系不大。我们的所有任务、数据和分析都是公开的,可供重复使用,这为今后的研究奠定了基础,有助于建立健全和可复制的跨文化差异科学。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"United States-China differences in cognition and perception across 12 tasks: Replicability, robustness, and within-culture variation.","authors":"Anjie Cao, Alexandra Carstensen, Shan Gao, Michael C Frank","doi":"10.1037/xge0001559","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001559","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cultural differences between the United States and China have been investigated using a broad array of psychological tasks measuring differences between cognition, language, perception, and reasoning. Using online convenience samples of adults, we conducted two large-scale replications of 12 tasks previously reported to show differences between Western and East Asian cultures. Our results showed a heterogeneous pattern of successes and failures: five tasks yielded robust cultural differences, while five showed no difference between cultures, and two showed a small difference in the opposite direction. We observed moderate reliability for all multitrial tasks, but there was little relation between task scores. As in prior work, cross-cultural differences in cognition (in those tasks showing differences) were not strongly related to explicit measures of cultural identity and behavior. All of our tasks, data, and analyses are openly available for reuse, providing a foundation for future studies that seek to establish a robust and replicable science of cross-cultural difference. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2657-2685"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141310773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How diversity in contexts and experiences shape perception and learning across the lifespan. 环境和经历的多样性如何影响人一生的感知和学习。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001671
Sarah E Gaither, Rachel Wu

The field of psychology has a long history of studying how diversity influences various outcomes such as identity development, social behaviors, perceptions, and decision making. However, considering the ways that diversity science research has expanded in recent years, the goal of this special issue is to provide space to highlight work that centers on identifying and testing new pathways from which we learn about diversity broadly defined. Specifically, this set of articles across the November and December 2024 issues stresses the need for us as a field to consider one's context, one's social identities or group memberships, and other various individual difference factors that all shape how we experience different forms of diversity across the lifespan (infancy through early adulthood). We also discuss areas of research not reflected through the submissions and push the field to fill those gaps in future work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

心理学领域在研究多样性如何影响身份发展、社会行为、认知和决策等各种结果方面有着悠久的历史。然而,考虑到近年来多样性科学研究的扩展方式,本特刊的目标是提供空间,重点介绍那些以识别和测试新途径为中心的工作,我们可以从中了解广义上的多样性。具体来说,2024 年 11 月刊和 12 月刊的这组文章强调,作为一个领域,我们需要考虑个人的背景、个人的社会身份或群体成员身份,以及其他各种个体差异因素,这些因素都会影响我们在整个生命周期(从婴儿期到成年早期)中如何体验不同形式的多样性。我们还讨论了未在提交的论文中得到反映的研究领域,并推动该领域在未来的工作中填补这些空白。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"How diversity in contexts and experiences shape perception and learning across the lifespan.","authors":"Sarah E Gaither, Rachel Wu","doi":"10.1037/xge0001671","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001671","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The field of psychology has a long history of studying how diversity influences various outcomes such as identity development, social behaviors, perceptions, and decision making. However, considering the ways that diversity science research has expanded in recent years, the goal of this special issue is to provide space to highlight work that centers on identifying and testing new pathways from which we learn about diversity broadly defined. Specifically, this set of articles across the November and December 2024 issues stresses the need for us as a field to consider one's context, one's social identities or group memberships, and other various individual difference factors that all shape how we experience different forms of diversity across the lifespan (infancy through early adulthood). We also discuss areas of research not reflected through the submissions and push the field to fill those gaps in future work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"153 11","pages":"2627-2630"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142545809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expectations of intergroup empathy bias emerge by early childhood. 群体间共情偏见的预期出现在幼儿期。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-13 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001505
Rodney Tompkins, Katie Vasquez, Emily Gerdin, Yarrow Dunham, Zoe Liberman

Across two preregistered studies with children (3-12-year-olds; N = 356) and adults (N = 262) from the United States, we find robust expectations for intergroup empathic biases. Participants predicted that people would feel better about ingroup fortunes than outgroup fortunes and worse about ingroup misfortunes than outgroup misfortunes. Expectations of empathic bias were stronger when there was animosity and weaker when there was fondness between groups. The largest developmental differences emerged in participants' expectations about how others feel about outgroup misfortunes, particularly when there was intergroup animosity. Whereas young children (3-5-year-olds) generally expected people to feel empathy for the outgroup (regardless of the relationship between the groups), older children (9-12-year-olds) and adults expected Schadenfreude (feeling good when an outgroup experiences a misfortune) when the groups disliked one another. Overall, expectations of empathic biases emerge early but may be weaker when there are positive intergroup relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在两项预先注册的儿童研究中(3-12岁;N = 356)和来自美国的成年人(N = 262),我们发现对群体间共情偏见的强烈期望。参与者预测,人们对群体内的幸运比群体外的幸运感觉更好,对群体内的不幸比群体外的不幸感觉更差。当群体之间存在敌意时,移情偏见的预期会更强,而当群体之间存在好感时,移情偏见的预期会更弱。最大的发展差异出现在参与者对他人如何看待群体外的不幸的期望上,特别是当群体间存在敌意时。年幼的孩子(3-5岁)通常期望人们对外群体感同身受(不管群体之间的关系如何),而大一点的孩子(9-12岁)和成年人在群体彼此不喜欢时期望人们幸灾乐祸(当外群体遭遇不幸时感觉良好)。总的来说,对共情偏见的预期出现得很早,但当有积极的群体间关系时,这种预期可能会减弱。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Expectations of intergroup empathy bias emerge by early childhood.","authors":"Rodney Tompkins, Katie Vasquez, Emily Gerdin, Yarrow Dunham, Zoe Liberman","doi":"10.1037/xge0001505","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001505","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across two preregistered studies with children (3-12-year-olds; <i>N</i> = 356) and adults (<i>N</i> = 262) from the United States, we find robust expectations for intergroup empathic biases. Participants predicted that people would feel better about ingroup fortunes than outgroup fortunes and worse about ingroup misfortunes than outgroup misfortunes. Expectations of empathic bias were stronger when there was animosity and weaker when there was fondness between groups. The largest developmental differences emerged in participants' expectations about how others feel about outgroup misfortunes, particularly when there was intergroup animosity. Whereas young children (3-5-year-olds) generally expected people to feel empathy for the outgroup (regardless of the relationship between the groups), older children (9-12-year-olds) and adults expected Schadenfreude (feeling good when an outgroup experiences a misfortune) when the groups disliked one another. Overall, expectations of empathic biases emerge early but may be weaker when there are positive intergroup relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2700-2714"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92154721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind-wandering when studying valuable information: The roles of age, dispositional traits, and contextual factors. 学习有价值信息时的思维游离:年龄、性格特征和环境因素的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001674
Ashley L Miller, Alan D Castel

The factors that trigger lapses of attention (e.g., mind-wandering) during new learning remain unclear. The present study investigated whether the likelihood of experiencing an attentional lapse depends on (a) the importance of the material being studied and (b) the learner's age. In two experiments, younger and older adults completed a delayed free recall task in which to-be-remembered words were paired with point values. Thought probes were embedded into the encoding phase of each list to provide an index of one's ability to maintain attention on task and prevent recurrent lapses of attention (i.e., the consistency of attention). Experiment 1 revealed all individuals better remembered high-value information at the expense of low-value information, and older adults were more frequently focused on the task than younger adults. Participants were also less likely to remember an item at test if they experienced an attentional lapse while learning said item, and they were more consistently focused on the task when studying high-value information than when studying low-value information. Age did not moderate either of these effects. Experiment 2 replicated the findings from Experiment 1 and further revealed that the positive association between age and attentional consistency was explained by age-related differences in affect, motivation, personality, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptomology. Once these factors were accounted for, older age was associated with increased attentional inconsistency (less on-task focus). While future replication of this finding is needed, implications for education and theories of both mind-wandering and aging are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在新的学习过程中,引发注意力缺失(如思维游离)的因素仍不清楚。本研究调查了发生注意力缺失的可能性是否取决于(a)学习材料的重要性和(b)学习者的年龄。在两项实验中,年轻人和老年人分别完成了一项延迟自由回忆任务,在该任务中,待记单词与点值配对。在每个列表的编码阶段都嵌入了思维探究,以提供一个指标来衡量一个人是否有能力将注意力保持在任务上,并防止出现反复的注意力缺失(即注意力的一致性)。实验 1 显示,所有个体都能更好地记住高价值信息,而忽略了低价值信息。此外,如果参与者在学习某个项目时出现了注意力不集中的情况,那么他们在测试时记住该项目的率也会降低,而且他们在学习高价值信息时比学习低价值信息时更能始终如一地集中注意力完成任务。年龄对上述两种效应都没有调节作用。实验 2 复制了实验 1 的研究结果,并进一步揭示了年龄与注意力一致性之间的正相关关系是由与年龄相关的情感、动机、个性和注意力缺陷多动障碍症状差异所解释的。一旦考虑了这些因素,年龄越大,注意力的不一致性就越高(对任务的专注程度越低)。虽然这一研究结果还需要进一步的验证,但我们讨论了这一研究结果对教育以及思维游离和老龄化理论的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mind-wandering when studying valuable information: The roles of age, dispositional traits, and contextual factors.","authors":"Ashley L Miller, Alan D Castel","doi":"10.1037/xge0001674","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001674","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The factors that trigger lapses of attention (e.g., mind-wandering) during new learning remain unclear. The present study investigated whether the likelihood of experiencing an attentional lapse depends on (a) the importance of the material being studied and (b) the learner's age. In two experiments, younger and older adults completed a delayed free recall task in which to-be-remembered words were paired with point values. Thought probes were embedded into the encoding phase of each list to provide an index of one's ability to maintain attention on task and prevent recurrent lapses of attention (i.e., the consistency of attention). Experiment 1 revealed all individuals better remembered high-value information at the expense of low-value information, and older adults were more frequently focused on the task than younger adults. Participants were also less likely to remember an item at test if they experienced an attentional lapse while learning said item, and they were more consistently focused on the task when studying high-value information than when studying low-value information. Age did not moderate either of these effects. Experiment 2 replicated the findings from Experiment 1 and further revealed that the positive association between age and attentional consistency was explained by age-related differences in affect, motivation, personality, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptomology. Once these factors were accounted for, older age was associated with increased attentional <i>inconsistency</i> (less on-task focus). While future replication of this finding is needed, implications for education and theories of both mind-wandering and aging are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142545808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The paradox of explaining: When feeling unknowledgeable prevents learners from engaging in effective learning strategies. 解释的悖论:当感到自己一无所知时,学习者就无法采用有效的学习策略。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001679
Stav Atir, Jane L Risen

People often need to learn complex information as part of their daily lives. One of the most effective strategies for understanding information is to explain it, for instance to a hypothetical other (Pilots 1 and 2). Yet, we find that learners prefer equally effortful but less effective learning strategies, even when incentivized to perform well (Study 1). Critically, we propose and find that learners' reluctance to explain is tied to their subjective knowledge of the material; learners who feel less knowledgeable about what they learned are most reluctant to explain it, despite the strategy being as effective for them (Study 2). An intervention that increased subjective knowledge (by having learners answer a few easy questions) increased learners' choice to explain, which was mediated by learners believing that explaining would be more pleasant and effective (Study 3). Directly manipulating beliefs about how fun and effective explaining is also boosted learners' willingness to explain (Study 4). Finally, because Studies 1-4 incentivized performance financially, we replicated key results in the classroom with students, finding improved scores on a class quiz (Study 5). The paradoxical implication of these findings is that those who need effective learning strategies the most are the ones least likely to use them. Put together, we find that subjective knowledge plays a key role in learning decisions and that boosting subjective knowledge is a simple intervention that can improve learning-related choices. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们在日常生活中经常需要学习复杂的信息。理解信息的最有效策略之一是解释信息,例如向假设的其他人解释(试验 1 和 2)。然而,我们发现,学习者更喜欢同样费力但效果较差的学习策略,即使在激励下也是如此(研究 1)。重要的是,我们提出并发现,学习者不愿意解释与他们对材料的主观认识有关;尽管学习策略对他们同样有效,但对所学知识了解较少的学习者最不愿意解释(研究 2)。增加主观知识的干预措施(让学习者回答几个简单的问题)增加了学习者解释的选择,而学习者认为解释会更愉快、更有效,这就起到了中介作用(研究 3)。直接操纵关于解释是多么有趣和有效的信念也提高了学习者解释的意愿(研究 4)。最后,由于研究 1-4 在经济上激励了学生的表现,我们在课堂上与学生一起复制了主要结果,发现学生在课堂测验中的成绩有所提高(研究 5)。这些研究结果的矛盾之处在于,那些最需要有效学习策略的人却最不可能使用这些策略。综上所述,我们发现主观知识在学习决策中起着关键作用,而增强主观知识是一种简单的干预措施,可以改善与学习有关的选择。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The paradox of explaining: When feeling unknowledgeable prevents learners from engaging in effective learning strategies.","authors":"Stav Atir, Jane L Risen","doi":"10.1037/xge0001679","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001679","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often need to learn complex information as part of their daily lives. One of the most effective strategies for understanding information is to explain it, for instance to a hypothetical other (Pilots 1 and 2). Yet, we find that learners prefer equally effortful but less effective learning strategies, even when incentivized to perform well (Study 1). Critically, we propose and find that learners' reluctance to explain is tied to their subjective knowledge of the material; learners who feel less knowledgeable about what they learned are most reluctant to explain it, despite the strategy being as effective for them (Study 2). An intervention that increased subjective knowledge (by having learners answer a few easy questions) increased learners' choice to explain, which was mediated by learners believing that explaining would be more pleasant and effective (Study 3). Directly manipulating beliefs about how fun and effective explaining is also boosted learners' willingness to explain (Study 4). Finally, because Studies 1-4 incentivized performance financially, we replicated key results in the classroom with students, finding improved scores on a class quiz (Study 5). The paradoxical implication of these findings is that those who need effective learning strategies the most are the ones least likely to use them. Put together, we find that subjective knowledge plays a key role in learning decisions and that boosting subjective knowledge is a simple intervention that can improve learning-related choices. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142467135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical inhibition after semantic violations recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. 语义违规后的词汇抑制会动用一种领域通用的抑制控制机制。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001642
Héctor O Sánchez-Meléndez, Kristi Hendrickson, Yoojeong Choo, Jan R Wessel

Language processing is incremental. As language signals-for example, words in a sentence-unfold, humans predict and activate likely upcoming input to facilitate comprehension. Prediction not only accelerates understanding but also prompts reassessment in the case of prediction error, fostering learning and refining comprehension skills. Therefore, it is paramount to understand what happens when linguistic predictions are violated-for example, when a sentence ends in an unpredicted word. One theory, which we test here, is that the originally predicted word is actively inhibited after semantic violations. Furthermore, we tested whether this purported lexical inhibition process is achieved by a domain-general mechanism-that is, one that also inhibits other processes (e.g., movement). We combined a semantic violation task, in which highly constrained sentences primed specific words but sometimes continued otherwise, with a motoric stop-signal task. Across two experiments, semantic violations significantly impaired simultaneous action-stopping. This implies that lexical and motor inhibition share the same process. In support of this view, multivariate decoding of electroencephalographic recordings showed early overlap in neural processing between action-stopping (motor inhibition) and semantic violations (lexical inhibition). Moreover, a known signature of motor inhibition (the stop-signal P3) was reduced after this initial overlap period, further suggesting the presence of a bottleneck due to shared processing. These findings show that semantic violations trigger inhibitory processing and suggest that this lexical inhibition recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. This provides a new perspective on long-standing debates in psycholinguistics, extends the range of a well-characterized cognitive control mechanism into the linguistic domain, and offers support for recent neurobiological models of domain-general inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

语言处理是渐进的。随着语言信号--例如句子中的单词--的叠加,人类会预测并激活可能即将出现的输入,以促进理解。预测不仅能加速理解,还能在预测错误的情况下促使重新评估,从而促进学习和完善理解技能。因此,了解语言预测被违反时会发生什么是至关重要的--例如,当句子以一个未预测的词结束时。我们在此测试的一种理论认为,语义违规后,最初预测的词会被主动抑制。此外,我们还测试了这种所谓的词汇抑制过程是否是通过一种领域通用机制实现的,也就是说,这种机制也会抑制其他过程(如运动)。我们将语义违反任务与运动停止信号任务相结合,在语义违反任务中,高度受限的句子会引出特定的词,但有时也会以其他方式继续。在两项实验中,语义违规会显著影响同时动作停止。这意味着词汇抑制和运动抑制共享同一个过程。为了支持这一观点,对脑电图记录进行多变量解码后发现,动作停止(运动抑制)和语义违反(词汇抑制)之间的神经处理存在早期重叠。此外,已知的运动抑制特征(停止信号 P3)在最初的重叠期之后有所减弱,这进一步表明由于共享处理而存在瓶颈。这些研究结果表明,语义违规会触发抑制处理,并表明这种词汇抑制会动用一种领域通用的抑制控制机制。这为心理语言学中长期存在的争论提供了一个新的视角,将一种表征良好的认知控制机制的范围扩展到了语言领域,并为最近的领域一般抑制控制神经生物学模型提供了支持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Lexical inhibition after semantic violations recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism.","authors":"Héctor O Sánchez-Meléndez, Kristi Hendrickson, Yoojeong Choo, Jan R Wessel","doi":"10.1037/xge0001642","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001642","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Language processing is incremental. As language signals-for example, words in a sentence-unfold, humans predict and activate likely upcoming input to facilitate comprehension. Prediction not only accelerates understanding but also prompts reassessment in the case of prediction error, fostering learning and refining comprehension skills. Therefore, it is paramount to understand what happens when linguistic predictions are violated-for example, when a sentence ends in an unpredicted word. One theory, which we test here, is that the originally predicted word is actively inhibited after semantic violations. Furthermore, we tested whether this purported lexical inhibition process is achieved by a domain-general mechanism-that is, one that also inhibits other processes (e.g., movement). We combined a semantic violation task, in which highly constrained sentences primed specific words but sometimes continued otherwise, with a motoric stop-signal task. Across two experiments, semantic violations significantly impaired simultaneous action-stopping. This implies that lexical and motor inhibition share the same process. In support of this view, multivariate decoding of electroencephalographic recordings showed early overlap in neural processing between action-stopping (motor inhibition) and semantic violations (lexical inhibition). Moreover, a known signature of motor inhibition (the stop-signal P3) was reduced after this initial overlap period, further suggesting the presence of a bottleneck due to shared processing. These findings show that semantic violations trigger inhibitory processing and suggest that this lexical inhibition recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. This provides a new perspective on long-standing debates in psycholinguistics, extends the range of a well-characterized cognitive control mechanism into the linguistic domain, and offers support for recent neurobiological models of domain-general inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142467133","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical inhibition after semantic violations recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. 语义违规后的词汇抑制会动用一种领域通用的抑制控制机制。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001642
Héctor O Sánchez-Meléndez,Kristi Hendrickson,Yoojeong Choo,Jan R Wessel
Language processing is incremental. As language signals-for example, words in a sentence-unfold, humans predict and activate likely upcoming input to facilitate comprehension. Prediction not only accelerates understanding but also prompts reassessment in the case of prediction error, fostering learning and refining comprehension skills. Therefore, it is paramount to understand what happens when linguistic predictions are violated-for example, when a sentence ends in an unpredicted word. One theory, which we test here, is that the originally predicted word is actively inhibited after semantic violations. Furthermore, we tested whether this purported lexical inhibition process is achieved by a domain-general mechanism-that is, one that also inhibits other processes (e.g., movement). We combined a semantic violation task, in which highly constrained sentences primed specific words but sometimes continued otherwise, with a motoric stop-signal task. Across two experiments, semantic violations significantly impaired simultaneous action-stopping. This implies that lexical and motor inhibition share the same process. In support of this view, multivariate decoding of electroencephalographic recordings showed early overlap in neural processing between action-stopping (motor inhibition) and semantic violations (lexical inhibition). Moreover, a known signature of motor inhibition (the stop-signal P3) was reduced after this initial overlap period, further suggesting the presence of a bottleneck due to shared processing. These findings show that semantic violations trigger inhibitory processing and suggest that this lexical inhibition recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. This provides a new perspective on long-standing debates in psycholinguistics, extends the range of a well-characterized cognitive control mechanism into the linguistic domain, and offers support for recent neurobiological models of domain-general inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
语言处理是渐进的。随着语言信号--例如句子中的单词--的叠加,人类会预测并激活可能即将出现的输入,以促进理解。预测不仅能加速理解,还能在预测错误的情况下促使重新评估,从而促进学习和完善理解技能。因此,了解语言预测被违反时会发生什么是至关重要的--例如,当句子以一个未预测的词结束时。我们在此测试的一种理论认为,语义违规后,最初预测的词会被主动抑制。此外,我们还测试了这种所谓的词汇抑制过程是否是通过一种领域通用机制实现的,也就是说,这种机制也会抑制其他过程(如运动)。我们将语义违反任务与运动停止信号任务相结合,在语义违反任务中,高度受限的句子会引出特定的词,但有时也会以其他方式继续。在两项实验中,语义违规会显著影响同时动作停止。这意味着词汇抑制和运动抑制共享同一个过程。为了支持这一观点,对脑电图记录进行多变量解码后发现,动作停止(运动抑制)和语义违反(词汇抑制)之间的神经处理存在早期重叠。此外,已知的运动抑制特征(停止信号 P3)在最初的重叠期之后有所减弱,这进一步表明由于共享处理而存在瓶颈。这些研究结果表明,语义违规会触发抑制处理,并表明这种词汇抑制会动用一种领域通用的抑制控制机制。这为心理语言学中长期存在的争论提供了一个新的视角,将一种表征良好的认知控制机制的范围扩展到了语言领域,并为最近的领域一般抑制控制神经生物学模型提供了支持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Lexical inhibition after semantic violations recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism.","authors":"Héctor O Sánchez-Meléndez,Kristi Hendrickson,Yoojeong Choo,Jan R Wessel","doi":"10.1037/xge0001642","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001642","url":null,"abstract":"Language processing is incremental. As language signals-for example, words in a sentence-unfold, humans predict and activate likely upcoming input to facilitate comprehension. Prediction not only accelerates understanding but also prompts reassessment in the case of prediction error, fostering learning and refining comprehension skills. Therefore, it is paramount to understand what happens when linguistic predictions are violated-for example, when a sentence ends in an unpredicted word. One theory, which we test here, is that the originally predicted word is actively inhibited after semantic violations. Furthermore, we tested whether this purported lexical inhibition process is achieved by a domain-general mechanism-that is, one that also inhibits other processes (e.g., movement). We combined a semantic violation task, in which highly constrained sentences primed specific words but sometimes continued otherwise, with a motoric stop-signal task. Across two experiments, semantic violations significantly impaired simultaneous action-stopping. This implies that lexical and motor inhibition share the same process. In support of this view, multivariate decoding of electroencephalographic recordings showed early overlap in neural processing between action-stopping (motor inhibition) and semantic violations (lexical inhibition). Moreover, a known signature of motor inhibition (the stop-signal P3) was reduced after this initial overlap period, further suggesting the presence of a bottleneck due to shared processing. These findings show that semantic violations trigger inhibitory processing and suggest that this lexical inhibition recruits a domain-general inhibitory control mechanism. This provides a new perspective on long-standing debates in psycholinguistics, extends the range of a well-characterized cognitive control mechanism into the linguistic domain, and offers support for recent neurobiological models of domain-general inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"44 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142486379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1