首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Malleable national collective memories among Black and White Americans. 美国黑人和白人中可塑的民族集体记忆。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001613
Travis G Cyr, William Hirst

How do groups remember their shared past? Are there individual differences within a group? How easy is it to change collective memories? The present article addresses these questions by focusing on differences within national subgroups, exploring how national collective memories might differ for Black and White Americans, how individual differences and external influences might moderate or alter any differences, and the temporal extent of any changes that might occur due to external influences. Across four studies, participants were asked to identify the five "most important" events in U.S. history and then asked about their political ideology and racial and national identification, though not in every study. Although individual differences emerged, Black and White participants differed in the types of events they identified as important in U.S. history, with Black participants identifying more race-relevant events than White participants and White participants identifying more traditional founding events than Black participants. As to changes in collective memory, in response to a minimal identity salience manipulation, the murder of George Floyd, and July 4th celebrations, national collective memories evidenced malleability only after the murder of George Floyd. In this instance, the mention of race-relevant events increased, even as the frequency of mention of traditional founding events remained stable. The observed increase in race-relevant events was temporary, however. Findings are discussed in relation to contemporary discussions on collective memory, especially with respect to group differences, individual differences within groups, and mnemonic inertia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

群体如何记忆共同的过去?群体内部是否存在个体差异?改变集体记忆有多容易?本文针对这些问题,将重点放在国家亚群体内部的差异上,探讨美国黑人和白人的国家集体记忆可能有何不同,个体差异和外部影响可能如何缓和或改变任何差异,以及由于外部影响而可能发生的任何变化的时间范围。在四项研究中,参与者被要求找出美国历史上五个 "最重要 "的事件,然后被问及他们的政治意识形态以及种族和民族认同,但并非每项研究都是如此。虽然出现了个体差异,但黑人和白人参与者在认定美国历史上重要事件的类型上有所不同,黑人参与者比白人参与者认定了更多与种族相关的事件,而白人参与者比黑人参与者认定了更多传统的建国事件。至于集体记忆的变化,在对最低身份显著性操作、乔治-弗洛伊德谋杀案和七月四日庆祝活动的反应中,只有在乔治-弗洛伊德谋杀案之后,国家集体记忆才表现出可塑性。在这种情况下,提及种族相关事件的频率增加了,而提及传统建国事件的频率却保持稳定。不过,所观察到的种族相关事件的增加是暂时的。本文结合当代有关集体记忆的讨论,特别是有关群体差异、群体内个体差异和记忆惯性的讨论,对研究结果进行了讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Malleable national collective memories among Black and White Americans.","authors":"Travis G Cyr, William Hirst","doi":"10.1037/xge0001613","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001613","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do groups remember their shared past? Are there individual differences within a group? How easy is it to change collective memories? The present article addresses these questions by focusing on differences within national subgroups, exploring how national collective memories might differ for Black and White Americans, how individual differences and external influences might moderate or alter any differences, and the temporal extent of any changes that might occur due to external influences. Across four studies, participants were asked to identify the five \"most important\" events in U.S. history and then asked about their political ideology and racial and national identification, though not in every study. Although individual differences emerged, Black and White participants differed in the types of events they identified as important in U.S. history, with Black participants identifying more race-relevant events than White participants and White participants identifying more traditional founding events than Black participants. As to changes in collective memory, in response to a minimal identity salience manipulation, the murder of George Floyd, and July 4th celebrations, national collective memories evidenced malleability only after the murder of George Floyd. In this instance, the mention of race-relevant events increased, even as the frequency of mention of traditional founding events remained stable. The observed increase in race-relevant events was temporary, however. Findings are discussed in relation to contemporary discussions on collective memory, especially with respect to group differences, individual differences within groups, and mnemonic inertia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141889411","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The presence of diversity initiatives leads to increased pro-White hiring decisions among conservatives. 多元化举措的存在导致保守派做出更多支持白人的招聘决定。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001614
Zeinab A Hachem, Tessa L Dover

Despite the push and pull between pro-diversity advocates and conservative resistance, most organizations have implemented diversity initiatives in an effort to promote equitable and fair organizational practices. Past work has shown that these diversity initiatives may not be as effective as expected and may instead result in unintended negative consequences for the very individuals they are meant to support. In three novel experiments (total N = 3,664), we investigated whether and when the presence of pro-diversity messages in organizational job recruitment materials might facilitate versus hinder the hiring of underrepresented racial minorities. Participant race and political ideology were also investigated as predictors of hiring recommendations. Findings indicate that pro-diversity messages facilitate politically motivated hiring bias. Specifically, in the presence of pro-diversity messages, White and some Black, Indigenous, and people of color (BIPOC) conservatives tend to display a pro-White shift in their hiring recommendations, whereas liberals tend to display a pro-minority shift. The present research underscores the importance of organizational awareness regarding the potential for hiring managers to react, whether consciously or subconsciously, against pro-diversity efforts because of political ideology. The present research also highlights the need for organizations to move beyond just espousing pro-diversity values and actually investigate the impact diversity initiatives have on hiring, retention, and promotion of diverse employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管支持多元化的倡导者与保守派之间存在推拉,但大多数组织还是实施了多元化举措,以努力促进公平公正的组织实践。过去的研究表明,这些多元化措施可能并不像预期的那样有效,反而可能会给它们想要支持的个人带来意想不到的负面影响。在三个新颖的实验中(总人数 = 3,664),我们研究了在组织职位招聘材料中出现支持多元化的信息是否会促进或阻碍对代表性不足的少数种族的聘用。参与者的种族和政治意识形态也作为招聘建议的预测因素接受了调查。研究结果表明,支持多元化的信息会促进出于政治动机的招聘偏见。具体地说,在支持多元化的信息面前,白人和一些黑人、土著人和有色人种(BIPOC)保守派倾向于在他们的招聘建议中表现出支持白人的转变,而自由派则倾向于表现出支持少数族裔的转变。本研究强调了组织意识的重要性,即招聘经理有可能因为政治意识形态而有意识或下意识地对支持多元化的努力做出反应。本研究还强调,组织有必要超越单纯支持多元化价值观的范畴,切实调查多元化举措对多元化员工的聘用、留用和晋升所产生的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The presence of diversity initiatives leads to increased pro-White hiring decisions among conservatives.","authors":"Zeinab A Hachem, Tessa L Dover","doi":"10.1037/xge0001614","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001614","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite the push and pull between pro-diversity advocates and conservative resistance, most organizations have implemented diversity initiatives in an effort to promote equitable and fair organizational practices. Past work has shown that these diversity initiatives may not be as effective as expected and may instead result in unintended negative consequences for the very individuals they are meant to support. In three novel experiments (total N = 3,664), we investigated whether and when the presence of pro-diversity messages in organizational job recruitment materials might facilitate versus hinder the hiring of underrepresented racial minorities. Participant race and political ideology were also investigated as predictors of hiring recommendations. Findings indicate that pro-diversity messages facilitate politically motivated hiring bias. Specifically, in the presence of pro-diversity messages, White and some Black, Indigenous, and people of color (BIPOC) conservatives tend to display a pro-White shift in their hiring recommendations, whereas liberals tend to display a pro-minority shift. The present research underscores the importance of organizational awareness regarding the potential for hiring managers to react, whether consciously or subconsciously, against pro-diversity efforts because of political ideology. The present research also highlights the need for organizations to move beyond just espousing pro-diversity values and actually investigate the impact diversity initiatives have on hiring, retention, and promotion of diverse employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141889424","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Where should I search next? Messages embedded in storybooks influence children's strategic exploration in Turkey and the United States. 下一步我应该搜索哪里?故事书中的信息对土耳其和美国儿童战略探索的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001619
Julie Vaisarova, Sarah L Kiefer, Hilal Şen, Peter M Todd, Kelsey Lucca

Despite the vital role of curiosity-driven exploration in learning, our understanding of how to enhance children's curiosity remains limited. Here, we tested whether hearing a strategic curiosity story with curiosity-promoting themes (e.g., strategically approaching uncertainty, adapting flexibly to new information) versus a control story with traditional pedagogical themes (e.g., following rules, learning from others) would influence children's strategic exploration across two cultures. Three- to 6-year-olds from the United States (N = 138) and Turkey (N = 88) were randomly assigned to hear one of these stories over Zoom, before playing a game in which they searched for sea creatures across five fish tanks. All tanks had the same number of hiding spots but varied in the number of creatures they contained. Time was limited and children could not return to prior tanks, pushing them to allocate search effort strategically. Results indicated that across both countries, children in the strategic curiosity condition explored the virtual "aquarium" more broadly; they moved through tanks more rapidly than children in the control condition and were more likely to explore all five tanks before time ran out. Children in the strategic curiosity condition also showed relatively more strategic search, adapting their search based on the likelihood of finding creatures in each tank. While further research is needed to pinpoint which elements of our stories produced differences in search behavior and whether they did so by enhancing or inhibiting children's strategic exploration, storybooks appear to be a promising method for shaping children's exploration across multiple countries. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管好奇心驱动的探索在学习中起着至关重要的作用,但我们对如何增强儿童好奇心的了解仍然有限。在这里,我们测试了听一个以促进好奇心为主题的策略性好奇心故事(如策略性地对待不确定性、灵活地适应新信息)与听一个以传统教学为主题的对照故事(如遵守规则、向他人学习)是否会影响两种文化背景下儿童的策略性探索。来自美国(138 人)和土耳其(88 人)的 3 至 6 岁儿童被随机分配到 Zoom 中听其中一个故事,然后再进行游戏,在五个鱼缸中寻找海洋生物。所有鱼缸的藏身点数量相同,但鱼缸中的生物数量各不相同。由于时间有限,儿童不能返回之前的鱼缸,这就促使他们战略性地分配搜索精力。结果表明,在这两个国家中,处于策略性好奇心条件下的儿童对虚拟 "水族馆 "的探索范围更广;与处于对照条件下的儿童相比,他们在水族箱中的移动速度更快,而且更有可能在时间用完之前探索完所有五个水族箱。策略性好奇心条件下的儿童还表现出相对更多的策略性搜索,他们会根据在每个水族箱中找到生物的可能性来调整自己的搜索。虽然还需要进一步的研究来确定我们故事中的哪些因素会导致搜索行为的差异,以及这些因素是通过增强还是抑制儿童的策略性探索来实现的,但故事书似乎是一种很有前途的方法,可以塑造儿童在多个国家的探索行为。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Where should I search next? Messages embedded in storybooks influence children's strategic exploration in Turkey and the United States.","authors":"Julie Vaisarova, Sarah L Kiefer, Hilal Şen, Peter M Todd, Kelsey Lucca","doi":"10.1037/xge0001619","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001619","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite the vital role of curiosity-driven exploration in learning, our understanding of how to enhance children's curiosity remains limited. Here, we tested whether hearing a strategic curiosity story with curiosity-promoting themes (e.g., strategically approaching uncertainty, adapting flexibly to new information) versus a control story with traditional pedagogical themes (e.g., following rules, learning from others) would influence children's strategic exploration across two cultures. Three- to 6-year-olds from the United States (N = 138) and Turkey (N = 88) were randomly assigned to hear one of these stories over Zoom, before playing a game in which they searched for sea creatures across five fish tanks. All tanks had the same number of hiding spots but varied in the number of creatures they contained. Time was limited and children could not return to prior tanks, pushing them to allocate search effort strategically. Results indicated that across both countries, children in the strategic curiosity condition explored the virtual \"aquarium\" more broadly; they moved through tanks more rapidly than children in the control condition and were more likely to explore all five tanks before time ran out. Children in the strategic curiosity condition also showed relatively more strategic search, adapting their search based on the likelihood of finding creatures in each tank. While further research is needed to pinpoint which elements of our stories produced differences in search behavior and whether they did so by enhancing or inhibiting children's strategic exploration, storybooks appear to be a promising method for shaping children's exploration across multiple countries. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141889426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contextual coherence increases perceived numerosity independent of semantic content. 上下文的连贯性会增加感知的数量感,而与语义内容无关。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001595
Chuyan Qu, Michael F Bonner, Nicholas K DeWind, Elizabeth M Brannon

Number perception emerges from multiple stages of visual processing. Understanding how systematic biases in number perception occur within a hierarchy of increasingly complex feature representations helps uncover the multistage processing underlying our visual number sense. Recent work demonstrated that reducing coherence of low-level visual attributes, such as color and orientation, systematically reduces perceived number. Here, we ask when in the visual processing hierarchy coherence affects numerosity perception and specifically whether the coherence effect is exclusive to low-level visual features or instead whether it can be driven by contextual or semantic relationships. We tested adults in an ordinal numerical comparison task with contextual coherence mathematically manipulated using a statistical model of visual object co-occurrence. Across several experiments, we found that arrays with high contextual coherence were perceived as numerically larger than arrays with low contextual coherence. This contextual coherence effect was not attenuated even when we reduced objects to texforms (unrecognizable images that preserve midlevel visual features) or removed semantic content from the images through box scrambling and diffeomorphic warping. Together, these results suggest that visual coherence derived from natural statistics of object co-occurrence systematically alters perceived numerosity at low-level visual processing, even before later stages at which items can be explicitly categorized and identified. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

数字感知产生于视觉处理的多个阶段。了解数字感知中的系统性偏差是如何在日益复杂的特征表征层次中产生的,有助于揭示视觉数字感的多级处理过程。最近的研究表明,降低低级视觉属性(如颜色和方向)的一致性会系统性地降低感知到的数字。在这里,我们要问的是,在视觉处理层级中,连贯性何时会影响数字感知,具体来说,连贯性效应是否是低级视觉特征所独有的,或者它是否会受到上下文或语义关系的驱动。我们在一项顺序数字比较任务中对成人进行了测试,并利用视觉物体共现统计模型对上下文连贯性进行了数学处理。在多项实验中,我们发现上下文一致性高的阵列在数值上要比上下文一致性低的阵列大。即使我们将物体缩小为texforms(保留了中层视觉特征的不可识别图像),或通过方块扰乱和差异变形扭曲去除图像中的语义内容,这种上下文一致性效应也不会减弱。总之,这些结果表明,在低级视觉处理过程中,从物体共现的自然统计中获得的视觉连贯性会系统地改变感知到的数值,甚至在项目可以明确分类和识别的后期阶段之前也是如此。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Contextual coherence increases perceived numerosity independent of semantic content.","authors":"Chuyan Qu, Michael F Bonner, Nicholas K DeWind, Elizabeth M Brannon","doi":"10.1037/xge0001595","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001595","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Number perception emerges from multiple stages of visual processing. Understanding how systematic biases in number perception occur within a hierarchy of increasingly complex feature representations helps uncover the multistage processing underlying our visual number sense. Recent work demonstrated that reducing coherence of low-level visual attributes, such as color and orientation, systematically reduces perceived number. Here, we ask when in the visual processing hierarchy coherence affects numerosity perception and specifically whether the coherence effect is exclusive to low-level visual features or instead whether it can be driven by contextual or semantic relationships. We tested adults in an ordinal numerical comparison task with contextual coherence mathematically manipulated using a statistical model of visual object co-occurrence. Across several experiments, we found that arrays with high contextual coherence were perceived as numerically larger than arrays with low contextual coherence. This contextual coherence effect was not attenuated even when we reduced objects to <i>texforms</i> (unrecognizable images that preserve midlevel visual features) or removed semantic content from the images through box scrambling and diffeomorphic warping. Together, these results suggest that visual coherence derived from natural statistics of object co-occurrence systematically alters perceived numerosity at low-level visual processing, even before later stages at which items can be explicitly categorized and identified. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141590400","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tool-sensed object information effectively supports vision for multisensory grasping. 工具感应物体信息可有效支持视觉进行多感官抓取。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001592
Ivan Camponogara, Alessandro Farnè, Robert Volcic

Tools enable humans to extend their sensing abilities beyond the natural limits of their hands, allowing them to sense objects as if they were using their hands directly. The similarities between direct hand interactions with objects (hand-based sensing) and the ability to extend sensory information processing beyond the hand (tool-mediated sensing) entail the existence of comparable processes for integrating tool- and hand-sensed information with vision, raising the question of whether tools support vision in bimanual object manipulations. Here, we investigated participants' performance while grasping objects either held with a tool or with their hand and compared these conditions with visually guided grasping (Experiment 1). By measuring reaction time, peak velocity, and peak of grip aperture, we found that actions were initiated earlier and performed with a smaller peak grip aperture when the object was seen and held with the tool or the contralateral hand compared to when it was only seen. Thus, tool-mediated sensing effectively supports vision in multisensory grasping and, even more intriguingly, resembles hand-based sensing. We excluded that results were due to the force exerted on the tool's handle (Experiment 2). Additionally, as for hand-based sensing, we found evidence that the tool supports vision by mainly providing object positional information (Experiment 3). Thus, integrating the tool-sensed position of the object with vision is sufficient to promote a multisensory advantage in grasping. Our findings indicate that multisensory integration mechanisms significantly improve grasping actions and fine-tune contralateral hand movements even when object information is only indirectly sensed through a tool. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

工具使人类的感知能力超越了双手的自然极限,使人类能够像直接使用双手一样感知物体。手与物体的直接互动(以手为基础的感知)与将感知信息处理能力扩展到手以外的能力(以工具为媒介的感知)之间存在相似之处,这就意味着存在将工具和手感知到的信息与视觉进行整合的类似过程,这就提出了一个问题:在双臂操作物体时,工具是否支持视觉。在此,我们研究了参与者在用工具或手抓取物体时的表现,并将这些情况与视觉引导下的抓取进行了比较(实验 1)。通过测量反应时间、峰值速度和抓握孔径峰值,我们发现,与只看到物体时相比,当看到物体并用工具或对侧手抓握时,动作开始得更早,抓握孔径峰值也更小。因此,在多感官抓握中,以工具为媒介的感知有效地支持了视觉,而且更有趣的是,它类似于以手为基础的感知。我们排除了因工具手柄受力而导致的结果(实验 2)。此外,与手部感应一样,我们发现有证据表明工具主要通过提供物体位置信息来支持视觉(实验 3)。因此,将工具感应到的物体位置与视觉结合起来足以促进抓握时的多感官优势。我们的研究结果表明,即使只是通过工具间接感知物体信息,多感官整合机制也能显著改善抓握动作,并对对侧手部动作进行微调。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Tool-sensed object information effectively supports vision for multisensory grasping.","authors":"Ivan Camponogara, Alessandro Farnè, Robert Volcic","doi":"10.1037/xge0001592","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001592","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Tools enable humans to extend their sensing abilities beyond the natural limits of their hands, allowing them to sense objects as if they were using their hands directly. The similarities between direct hand interactions with objects (hand-based sensing) and the ability to extend sensory information processing beyond the hand (tool-mediated sensing) entail the existence of comparable processes for integrating tool- and hand-sensed information with vision, raising the question of whether tools support vision in bimanual object manipulations. Here, we investigated participants' performance while grasping objects either held with a tool or with their hand and compared these conditions with visually guided grasping (Experiment 1). By measuring reaction time, peak velocity, and peak of grip aperture, we found that actions were initiated earlier and performed with a smaller peak grip aperture when the object was seen and held with the tool or the contralateral hand compared to when it was only seen. Thus, tool-mediated sensing effectively supports vision in multisensory grasping and, even more intriguingly, resembles hand-based sensing. We excluded that results were due to the force exerted on the tool's handle (Experiment 2). Additionally, as for hand-based sensing, we found evidence that the tool supports vision by mainly providing object positional information (Experiment 3). Thus, integrating the tool-sensed position of the object with vision is sufficient to promote a multisensory advantage in grasping. Our findings indicate that multisensory integration mechanisms significantly improve grasping actions and fine-tune contralateral hand movements even when object information is only indirectly sensed through a tool. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141889425","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interdependent versus independent inconsistency: Cultural differences in how East Asian and Western people attribute hypocrisy. 相互依存与独立不一致:东亚人和西方人如何看待虚伪的文化差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001608
Minjae Seo, Shoko Watanabe, Young-Hoon Kim, Sean M Laurent

Humans worldwide have long deplored hypocrisy, a concept that has been mentioned in texts dating back 100-1,000 years (e.g., the Analects of Confucius, the Tao Te Ching, the Bible, and the Qur'an). However, what influences the extent of hypocrisy attribution or counts as hypocrisy may differ as a function of culture. Previous studies have shown that Westerners attribute greater hypocrisy for within-person attitude-behavior inconsistency than East Asians. Building on this, we predict that East Asians' (vs. Westerners') hypocrisy attribution is more heavily influenced by social relationships. Consistent with past research, this can lead to greater leniency. However, as we show, this can also result in the novel finding we present that attributions of mild-to-moderate hypocrisy are made even when no explicit within-person attitude-behavior inconsistency is present. Across six experiments, we found that Koreans (vs. participants from the United States) attributed more hypocrisy to attitude-contradicting behavior when the person enacting the behavior was not the person who stated the attitude but was someone who shared social bonds with that person (i.e., cross-person, within-relationship attitude-behavior inconsistency; "relational hypocrisy"). Specifically, Koreans attributed more hypocrisy than Americans when a child's behavior contradicted their parent's views (Experiments 1a and 1b) or when attitude-contradicting behavior was enacted by the child of a close friend (Experiment 2). Experiments 3-5 replicated the findings from Experiments 1-2 using additional social contexts (e.g., a spousal relationship). Supplementary analyses showed that differences in hypocrisy attribution between Americans and Koreans were mediated by cultural differences in their perceptions of shared responsibility within relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

全世界的人类长期以来都对虚伪深恶痛绝,早在 100-1000 年前的典籍(如《论语》、《道德经》、《圣经》和《古兰经》)中就提到过这一概念。然而,影响虚伪归因程度或将其视为虚伪的因素可能因文化而异。以往的研究表明,西方人对人与人之间态度与行为不一致的虚伪归因大于东亚人。在此基础上,我们预测东亚人(与西方人相比)的虚伪归因受社会关系的影响更大。与过去的研究一致,这可能会导致更宽松的态度。然而,正如我们所展示的那样,这也会导致我们提出的新发现,即即使在人与人之间没有明确的态度-行为不一致的情况下,也会产生轻度到中度的虚伪归因。在六次实验中,我们发现,当实施与态度相悖行为的人不是表明态度的人,而是与该人有共同社会关系的人(即跨人、关系内态度行为不一致;"关系型虚伪")时,韩国人(与来自美国的参与者相比)对态度相悖行为的虚伪性归因更多。具体来说,当孩子的行为与父母的观点相悖时(实验 1a 和 1b),或者当态度相悖的行为由亲密朋友的孩子实施时(实验 2),韩国人比美国人更倾向于认为孩子虚伪。实验 3-5 利用其他社会背景(如配偶关系)复制了实验 1-2 的结果。补充分析表明,美国人和韩国人在虚伪归因上的差异是由他们对人际关系中共同责任的文化差异所中介的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Interdependent versus independent inconsistency: Cultural differences in how East Asian and Western people attribute hypocrisy.","authors":"Minjae Seo, Shoko Watanabe, Young-Hoon Kim, Sean M Laurent","doi":"10.1037/xge0001608","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001608","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans worldwide have long deplored hypocrisy, a concept that has been mentioned in texts dating back 100-1,000 years (e.g., the Analects of Confucius, the Tao Te Ching, the Bible, and the Qur'an). However, what influences the extent of hypocrisy attribution or counts as hypocrisy may differ as a function of culture. Previous studies have shown that Westerners attribute greater hypocrisy for within-person attitude-behavior inconsistency than East Asians. Building on this, we predict that East Asians' (vs. Westerners') hypocrisy attribution is more heavily influenced by social relationships. Consistent with past research, this can lead to greater leniency. However, as we show, this can also result in the novel finding we present that attributions of mild-to-moderate hypocrisy are made even when no explicit within-person attitude-behavior inconsistency is present. Across six experiments, we found that Koreans (vs. participants from the United States) attributed more hypocrisy to attitude-contradicting behavior when the person enacting the behavior was not the person who stated the attitude but was someone who shared social bonds with that person (i.e., cross-person, within-relationship attitude-behavior inconsistency; \"relational hypocrisy\"). Specifically, Koreans attributed more hypocrisy than Americans when a child's behavior contradicted their parent's views (Experiments 1a and 1b) or when attitude-contradicting behavior was enacted by the child of a close friend (Experiment 2). Experiments 3-5 replicated the findings from Experiments 1-2 using additional social contexts (e.g., a spousal relationship). Supplementary analyses showed that differences in hypocrisy attribution between Americans and Koreans were mediated by cultural differences in their perceptions of shared responsibility within relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141457350","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Independent influences of movement distance and visual distance on Fitts' law. 移动距离和视觉距离对费茨定律的独立影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001612
Naser Al-Fawakhiri, Samuel D McDougle

Fitts' Law is one among a small number of psychophysical laws. However, a fundamental variable in Fitts' Law-the movement distance, D-confounds two quantities: The physical distance the effector has to move to reach a goal, and the visually perceived distance to that goal. While these two quantities are functionally equivalent in everyday motor behavior, decoupling them might improve our understanding of the factors that shape speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Here, we leveraged the phenomenon of visuomotor gain adaptation to de-confound movement and visual distance during goal-directed reaching. We found that movement distance and visual distance can influence movement times, supporting a variant of Fitts' Law that considers both. The weighting of movement versus visual distance was modified by restricting movement range and degrading visual feedback. These results may reflect the role of sensory context in early stages of motor planning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

菲茨定律是少数心理物理定律之一。然而,菲茨定律中的一个基本变量--移动距离 D--包含两个量:运动者到达目标所需的物理距离,以及视觉感知到的目标距离。虽然这两个量在日常运动行为中的功能是等同的,但将它们分离开来可能会提高我们对速度-准确性权衡因素的理解。在这里,我们利用视觉运动增益适应现象来解构目标指向性伸手过程中的运动距离和视觉距离。我们发现,运动距离和视觉距离都会影响运动时间,这支持了菲茨定律的一个变体,即同时考虑运动距离和视觉距离。通过限制运动范围和降低视觉反馈,可以改变运动距离与视觉距离的权重。这些结果可能反映了感觉环境在运动规划早期阶段的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Independent influences of movement distance and visual distance on Fitts' law.","authors":"Naser Al-Fawakhiri, Samuel D McDougle","doi":"10.1037/xge0001612","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001612","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fitts' Law is one among a small number of psychophysical laws. However, a fundamental variable in Fitts' Law-the movement distance, <i>D</i>-confounds two quantities: The physical distance the effector has to move to reach a goal, and the visually perceived distance to that goal. While these two quantities are functionally equivalent in everyday motor behavior, decoupling them might improve our understanding of the factors that shape speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Here, we leveraged the phenomenon of visuomotor gain adaptation to de-confound movement and visual distance during goal-directed reaching. We found that movement distance and visual distance can influence movement times, supporting a variant of Fitts' Law that considers both. The weighting of movement versus visual distance was modified by restricting movement range and degrading visual feedback. These results may reflect the role of sensory context in early stages of motor planning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141457349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Combining forecasts from advisors: The impact of advice independence and verbal versus numeric format. 结合顾问的预测:建议独立性和口头与数字格式的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001611
Jeremy D Strueder, Paul D Windschitl

Past research on advice-taking has suggested that people are often insensitive to the level of advice independence when combining forecasts from advisors. However, this has primarily been tested for cases in which people receive numeric forecasts. Recent work by Mislavsky and Gaertig (2022) shows that people sometimes employ different strategies when combining verbal versus numeric forecasts about the likelihood of future events. Specifically, likelihood judgments based on two verbal forecasts (e.g., "rather likely") are more often extreme (relative to the forecasts) than are likelihood judgments based on two numeric forecasts (e.g., "70% probability"). The goal of the present research was to investigate whether advice-takers' use of combination strategies can be sensitive to advice independence when differences in independence are highly salient and whether sensitivity to advice independence depends on the format in which advice is given. In two studies, we found that advice-takers became more extreme with their own likelihood estimate when combining forecasts from advisors who use separate evidence, as opposed to the same evidence. We also found that two verbal forecasts generally resulted in more extreme combined likelihood estimates than two numeric forecasts. However, the results did not suggest that sensitivity to advice independence depends on the format of advice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

以往关于建议采纳的研究表明,人们在综合顾问的预测时,往往对建议的独立性不敏感。然而,这主要是在人们接受数字预测的情况下进行的测试。Mislavsky 和 Gaertig(2022 年)的最新研究表明,人们在结合有关未来事件可能性的口头预测和数字预测时,有时会采用不同的策略。具体来说,与基于两个数字预测的可能性判断(如 "70%的可能性")相比,基于两个口头预测的可能性判断(如 "相当可能")往往更极端(相对于预测而言)。本研究的目的是探究当独立性差异非常突出时,建议接受者使用组合策略是否会对建议独立性敏感,以及对建议独立性的敏感性是否取决于给出建议的形式。在两项研究中,我们发现,与使用相同证据的预测相比,在结合使用不同证据的顾问的预测时,建议接受者对自己的可能性估计会变得更加极端。我们还发现,与两个数字预测相比,两个口头预测通常会导致更极端的综合可能性估计。不过,结果并不表明对建议独立性的敏感性取决于建议的形式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Combining forecasts from advisors: The impact of advice independence and verbal versus numeric format.","authors":"Jeremy D Strueder, Paul D Windschitl","doi":"10.1037/xge0001611","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001611","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research on advice-taking has suggested that people are often insensitive to the level of advice independence when combining forecasts from advisors. However, this has primarily been tested for cases in which people receive numeric forecasts. Recent work by Mislavsky and Gaertig (2022) shows that people sometimes employ different strategies when combining verbal versus numeric forecasts about the likelihood of future events. Specifically, likelihood judgments based on two verbal forecasts (e.g., \"rather likely\") are more often extreme (relative to the forecasts) than are likelihood judgments based on two numeric forecasts (e.g., \"70% probability\"). The goal of the present research was to investigate whether advice-takers' use of combination strategies can be sensitive to advice independence when differences in independence are highly salient and whether sensitivity to advice independence depends on the format in which advice is given. In two studies, we found that advice-takers became more extreme with their own likelihood estimate when combining forecasts from advisors who use separate evidence, as opposed to the same evidence. We also found that two verbal forecasts generally resulted in more extreme combined likelihood estimates than two numeric forecasts. However, the results did not suggest that sensitivity to advice independence depends on the format of advice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141889409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for When the Personal and the Collective Intersects: Memory, Future Thinking, and Perceived Agency During the COVID-19 Pandemic 当个人与集体交叉时》的补充材料:COVID-19 大流行期间的记忆、未来思考和感知机构
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001624.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for When the Personal and the Collective Intersects: Memory, Future Thinking, and Perceived Agency During the COVID-19 Pandemic","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xge0001624.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001624.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141825754","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The contribution of auditory imagery and visual rhythm perception to sensorimotor synchronization with external and imagined rhythm. 听觉意象和视觉节奏感对感觉运动与外部和想象节奏同步的贡献。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001601
Simon Andrew Whitton, Benjamin Sreenan, Fang Jiang

Sensorimotor synchronization (SMS) refers to the temporal coordination of an external stimulus with movement. Our previous work revealed that while SMS with visual flashing patterns was less consistent than with auditory or tactile patterns, it was still evident in a sample of nonmusicians. Although previous studies have speculated the potential role of auditory imagery, its contribution to visual SMS performance is not well quantified. Utilizing a synchronization-continuation finger-tapping task with a visual stimulus that included implied motion, we aimed to examine how participants' imagery ability, musicality, and rhythm perception affected SMS performance. We quantified participants' SMS consistency in synchronization (with visual cues) and continuation (without visual cues) phases. Participants also performed a perception task assessing their ability to detect temporal perturbations in the visual rhythm and completed musical ability and imagery questionnaires. Our linear regression model for SMS consistency included the trial phase, self-reported auditory imagery control and musicality, and visual rhythm perception as predictors. Significant effects of trial phase and auditory imagery scores on SMS consistency suggested that participants performed SMS more consistently while the guiding visual stimulus was present and that the higher one's self-reported auditory imagery ability, the better their SMS when continuing with unguided rhythm. One's visual rhythm perception accuracy significantly correlated with SMS consistency during the synchronization phase, and there was no correlation between rhythm perception and auditory imagery control. Overall, our results suggested relatively independent contributions of auditory imagery and visual rhythm perception to SMS with visual rhythm. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

感觉运动同步(SMS)是指外部刺激与运动在时间上的协调。我们之前的研究表明,虽然视觉闪烁模式的 SMS 一致性不如听觉或触觉模式,但在非音乐家样本中仍然很明显。虽然之前的研究推测了听觉意象的潜在作用,但其对视觉短信表现的贡献还没有很好地量化。我们使用了一个同步-持续手指敲击任务,其中的视觉刺激包括隐含的动作,目的是研究参与者的意象能力、音乐性和节奏感如何影响 SMS 表现。我们对参与者在同步(有视觉提示)和延续(无视觉提示)阶段的短信一致性进行了量化。参与者还完成了一项感知任务,以评估他们检测视觉节奏中时间扰动的能力,并填写了音乐能力和意象问卷。我们的 SMS 一致性线性回归模型将试验阶段、自我报告的听觉意象控制和音乐性以及视觉节奏感知作为预测因素。试验阶段和听觉意象得分对 SMS 一致性的显著影响表明,当有引导性视觉刺激时,受试者的 SMS 表现更稳定,而且自我报告的听觉意象能力越高,他们在继续无引导节奏时的 SMS 表现越好。在同步阶段,一个人的视觉节奏感知准确性与 SMS 的一致性显著相关,而节奏感知与听觉意象控制之间没有相关性。总之,我们的研究结果表明,听觉意象和视觉节奏感知对视觉节奏短信的贡献相对独立。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The contribution of auditory imagery and visual rhythm perception to sensorimotor synchronization with external and imagined rhythm.","authors":"Simon Andrew Whitton, Benjamin Sreenan, Fang Jiang","doi":"10.1037/xge0001601","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001601","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sensorimotor synchronization (SMS) refers to the temporal coordination of an external stimulus with movement. Our previous work revealed that while SMS with visual flashing patterns was less consistent than with auditory or tactile patterns, it was still evident in a sample of nonmusicians. Although previous studies have speculated the potential role of auditory imagery, its contribution to visual SMS performance is not well quantified. Utilizing a synchronization-continuation finger-tapping task with a visual stimulus that included implied motion, we aimed to examine how participants' imagery ability, musicality, and rhythm perception affected SMS performance. We quantified participants' SMS consistency in synchronization (with visual cues) and continuation (without visual cues) phases. Participants also performed a perception task assessing their ability to detect temporal perturbations in the visual rhythm and completed musical ability and imagery questionnaires. Our linear regression model for SMS consistency included the trial phase, self-reported auditory imagery control and musicality, and visual rhythm perception as predictors. Significant effects of trial phase and auditory imagery scores on SMS consistency suggested that participants performed SMS more consistently while the guiding visual stimulus was present and that the higher one's self-reported auditory imagery ability, the better their SMS when continuing with unguided rhythm. One's visual rhythm perception accuracy significantly correlated with SMS consistency during the synchronization phase, and there was no correlation between rhythm perception and auditory imagery control. Overall, our results suggested relatively independent contributions of auditory imagery and visual rhythm perception to SMS with visual rhythm. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11250674/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140848917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1