首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Decision criteria in signal detection model are not based on the objective likelihood ratio. 信号检测模型中的决策准则不是基于目标似然比的。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001438
Xiao Hu, Chunliang Yang, Liang Luo

How people set decision criteria in signal detection model is an important research question. The likelihood ratio (LR) theory, which is one of the most influential theories about criteria setting, typically assumes that (a) decisions are based on the objective LR of the signal and noise distributions, and (b) LR criteria do not change across tasks with various difficulty levels. However, it is often questioned whether people are really able to know the exact shape of signal and noise distributions, and compute the objective LR accordingly. Here we suggest whether decision criteria are set based on objective LR can be tested in two-condition experiments with different difficulty levels across conditions. We then asked participants in three empirical experiments to perform two-condition perceptual or memory tasks, and give their answer using confidence rating scale. Results revealed that the two assumptions of LR theory contradicted with each other: if we assumed decision criteria were based on objective LR, then the estimated LR criteria differed across difficulty levels, and fanned out as task difficulty decreased. We suggest people might inaccurately estimate the LR in signal detection tasks, and several possible explanations for the distortion of LR are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

人们如何在信号检测模型中设置决策准则是一个重要的研究问题。似然比(LR)理论是关于标准设置的最具影响力的理论之一,它通常假设(a)决策基于信号和噪声分布的目标LR,以及(b)LR标准在具有不同难度级别的任务之间不会改变。然而,人们经常质疑人们是否真的能够知道信号和噪声分布的确切形状,并相应地计算目标LR。在这里,我们建议是否可以在不同条件下不同难度水平的两个条件实验中测试基于目标LR的决策标准。然后,我们要求三个经验实验的参与者执行两种条件的感知或记忆任务,并使用置信度评定量表给出他们的答案。结果表明,LR理论的两个假设相互矛盾:如果我们假设决策标准是基于客观LR的,那么估计的LR标准在不同的难度水平上是不同的,并且随着任务难度的降低而呈扇形分布。我们认为人们在信号检测任务中可能会不准确地估计LR,并讨论了LR失真的几种可能解释。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Decision criteria in signal detection model are not based on the objective likelihood ratio.","authors":"Xiao Hu,&nbsp;Chunliang Yang,&nbsp;Liang Luo","doi":"10.1037/xge0001438","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001438","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How people set decision criteria in signal detection model is an important research question. The likelihood ratio (LR) theory, which is one of the most influential theories about criteria setting, typically assumes that (a) decisions are based on the objective LR of the signal and noise distributions, and (b) LR criteria do not change across tasks with various difficulty levels. However, it is often questioned whether people are really able to know the exact shape of signal and noise distributions, and compute the objective LR accordingly. Here we suggest whether decision criteria are set based on objective LR can be tested in two-condition experiments with different difficulty levels across conditions. We then asked participants in three empirical experiments to perform two-condition perceptual or memory tasks, and give their answer using confidence rating scale. Results revealed that the two assumptions of LR theory contradicted with each other: if we assumed decision criteria were based on objective LR, then the estimated LR criteria differed across difficulty levels, and fanned out as task difficulty decreased. We suggest people might inaccurately estimate the LR in signal detection tasks, and several possible explanations for the distortion of LR are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3037-3057"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9553824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of political devotion in sharing partisan misinformation and resistance to fact-checking. 政治奉献在分享党派错误信息和抵制事实核查方面的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001436
Clara Pretus, Camila Servin-Barthet, Elizabeth A Harris, William J Brady, Oscar Vilarroya, Jay J Van Bavel

Online misinformation is disproportionality created and spread by people with extreme political attitudes, especially among the far-right. There is a debate in the literature about why people spread misinformation and what should be done about it. According to the purely cognitive account, people largely spread misinformation because they are lazy, not biased. According to a motivational account, people are also motivated to believe and spread misinformation for ideological and partisan reasons. To better understand the psychological and neurocognitive processes that underlie misinformation sharing among the far-right, we conducted a cross-cultural experiment with conservatives and far-right partisans in the Unites States and Spain (N = 1,609) and a neuroimaging study with far-right partisans in Spain (N = 36). Far-right partisans in Spain and U.S. Republicans who highly identify with Trump were more likely to share misinformation than center-right voters and other Republicans, especially when the misinformation was related to sacred values (e.g., immigration). Sacred values predicted misinformation sharing above and beyond familiarity, attitude strength, and salience of the issue. Moreover, far-right partisans were unresponsive to fact-checking and accuracy nudges. At a neural level, this group showed increased activity in brain regions implicated in mentalizing and norm compliance in response to posts with sacred values. These results suggest that the two components of political devotion-identity fusion and sacred values-play a key role in misinformation sharing, highlighting the identity-affirming dimension of misinformation sharing. We discuss the need for motivational and identity-based interventions to help curb misinformation for high-risk partisan groups. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

网络上的错误信息是由持极端政治态度的人制造和传播的,尤其是在极右翼中。关于人们为什么传播错误信息以及应该采取什么措施,文献中存在着争论。根据纯粹的认知解释,人们传播错误信息很大程度上是因为他们懒惰,而不是有偏见。根据一项动机描述,人们也有动机出于意识形态和党派原因相信和传播错误信息。为了更好地理解极右翼之间分享错误信息的心理和神经认知过程,我们对美国和西班牙的保守派和极右翼党派人士(N=1609)进行了一项跨文化实验,并对西班牙的极右翼党派人员(N=36)进行了神经影像学研究。西班牙的极右翼党派人士和高度认同特朗普的美国共和党人比中右翼选民和其他共和党人更有可能分享错误信息,尤其是当错误信息与神圣价值观(如移民)有关时。神圣的价值观预测了错误信息的分享,超越了熟悉度、态度强度和问题的突出性。此外,极右翼党派对事实核查和准确度的微调反应迟钝。在神经层面上,这一组人的大脑区域在对具有神圣价值观的帖子做出反应时,表现出与心理化和规范遵守有关的活动增加。这些结果表明,政治忠诚、身份融合和神圣价值观这两个组成部分在错误信息共享中发挥着关键作用,凸显了错误信息共享的身份肯定维度。我们讨论了动机和基于身份的干预措施的必要性,以帮助遏制高风险党派团体的错误信息。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The role of political devotion in sharing partisan misinformation and resistance to fact-checking.","authors":"Clara Pretus, Camila Servin-Barthet, Elizabeth A Harris, William J Brady, Oscar Vilarroya, Jay J Van Bavel","doi":"10.1037/xge0001436","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001436","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Online misinformation is disproportionality created and spread by people with extreme political attitudes, especially among the far-right. There is a debate in the literature about why people spread misinformation and what should be done about it. According to the purely cognitive account, people largely spread misinformation because they are lazy, not biased. According to a motivational account, people are also motivated to believe and spread misinformation for ideological and partisan reasons. To better understand the psychological and neurocognitive processes that underlie misinformation sharing among the far-right, we conducted a cross-cultural experiment with conservatives and far-right partisans in the Unites States and Spain (<i>N</i> = 1,609) and a neuroimaging study with far-right partisans in Spain (<i>N</i> = 36). Far-right partisans in Spain and U.S. Republicans who highly identify with Trump were more likely to share misinformation than center-right voters and other Republicans, especially when the misinformation was related to sacred values (e.g., immigration). Sacred values predicted misinformation sharing above and beyond familiarity, attitude strength, and salience of the issue. Moreover, far-right partisans were unresponsive to fact-checking and accuracy nudges. At a neural level, this group showed increased activity in brain regions implicated in mentalizing and norm compliance in response to posts with sacred values. These results suggest that the two components of political devotion-identity fusion and sacred values-play a key role in misinformation sharing, highlighting the identity-affirming dimension of misinformation sharing. We discuss the need for motivational and identity-based interventions to help curb misinformation for high-risk partisan groups. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3116-3134"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10033177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unequal opportunities from the start: Socioeconomic disparities in classroom participation in preschool. 从一开始机会就不平等:学前教育课堂参与方面的社会经济差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-29 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001437
Sébastien Goudeau, Camille Sanrey, Frédérique Autin, Nicole M Stephens, Hazel R Markus, Jean-Claude Croizet, Andrei Cimpian

Why do socioeconomic disparities in achievement emerge so early in life? Previous answers to this question have generally focused on the perceived deficits of parents from disadvantaged backgrounds (e.g., insufficient childrearing knowledge). Here, we instead focus on the structure of early childhood education and argue that early schooling contexts provide unequal opportunities for engagement to children of higher versus lower socioeconomic status (SES). As engagement is a longitudinal predictor of achievement, early SES disparities in engagement could serve to maintain or even exacerbate SES disparities in achievement. In Study 1 (1,236 observations; N = 98 children), we investigated preschool students' behavioral engagement during whole-class discussions-a core aspect of early childhood education. Low-SES children showed significantly lower engagement than their peers. Consistent with the claim of unequal opportunities for engagement, these differences were not accounted for by SES differences in language proficiency. As students' engagement in school is influenced by their peers' attitudes toward them, we also examined peer perceptions (Study 2, N = 94, and a meta-analysis, k = 2 studies). We found that preschoolers who show more engagement relative to others during whole-class discussions are perceived as possessing more positive qualities (e.g., intelligence). Given that higher-SES students are afforded more opportunities for engagement (see Study 1), they may be the ones benefiting from these positive peer perceptions as well, which might further boost their engagement. Our results suggest that aspects of early childhood education should be redesigned to foster engagement among all students, regardless of their SES. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

为什么社会经济成就差距在人生早期就出现了?以前对这个问题的回答通常集中在来自弱势背景的父母的感知缺陷上(例如,育儿知识不足)。在这里,我们转而关注幼儿教育的结构,并认为早期教育环境为社会经济地位较高和较低的儿童提供了不平等的参与机会。由于参与度是成就的纵向预测因素,早期社会经济地位参与度的差异可能有助于维持甚至加剧社会经济地位成就的差异。在研究1(1236项观察;N=98名儿童)中,我们调查了学龄前学生在全班讨论中的行为参与,这是幼儿教育的核心方面。低社会经济地位儿童的参与度明显低于同龄人。与参与机会不平等的说法一致,这些差异并没有被SES语言水平的差异所解释。由于学生在学校的参与度受到同龄人对他们的态度的影响,我们还调查了同龄人的看法(研究2,N=94,荟萃分析,k=2项研究)。我们发现,在全班讨论中表现出更多参与度的学龄前儿童被认为具有更积极的品质(如智力)。鉴于社会经济地位较高的学生有更多的参与机会(见研究1),他们可能也会从这些积极的同伴认知中受益,这可能会进一步提高他们的参与度。我们的研究结果表明,应该重新设计幼儿教育的各个方面,以促进所有学生的参与,无论他们的社会经济地位如何。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Unequal opportunities from the start: Socioeconomic disparities in classroom participation in preschool.","authors":"Sébastien Goudeau, Camille Sanrey, Frédérique Autin, Nicole M Stephens, Hazel R Markus, Jean-Claude Croizet, Andrei Cimpian","doi":"10.1037/xge0001437","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001437","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Why do socioeconomic disparities in achievement emerge so early in life? Previous answers to this question have generally focused on the perceived deficits of parents from disadvantaged backgrounds (e.g., insufficient childrearing knowledge). Here, we instead focus on the structure of early childhood education and argue that early schooling contexts provide <i>unequal opportunities for engagement</i> to children of higher versus lower socioeconomic status (SES). As engagement is a longitudinal predictor of achievement, early SES disparities in engagement could serve to maintain or even exacerbate SES disparities in achievement. In Study 1 (1,236 observations; <i>N</i> = 98 children), we investigated preschool students' behavioral engagement during whole-class discussions-a core aspect of early childhood education. Low-SES children showed significantly lower engagement than their peers. Consistent with the claim of unequal opportunities for engagement, these differences were not accounted for by SES differences in language proficiency. As students' engagement in school is influenced by their peers' attitudes toward them, we also examined peer perceptions (Study 2, <i>N</i> = 94, and a meta-analysis, <i>k</i> = 2 studies). We found that preschoolers who show more engagement relative to others during whole-class discussions are perceived as possessing more positive qualities (e.g., intelligence). Given that higher-SES students are afforded more opportunities for engagement (see Study 1), they may be the ones benefiting from these positive peer perceptions as well, which might further boost their engagement. Our results suggest that aspects of early childhood education should be redesigned to foster engagement among <i>all</i> students, regardless of their SES. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3135-3152"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9699111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential attentional costs of encoding specific and gist episodic memory representations. 编码特定情景记忆表征和要点情景记忆表征的不同注意成本。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-20 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001448
Nathaniel R Greene, Moshe Naveh-Benjamin

Theories of episodic memory posit that more attentional resources are needed for encoding specific compared to gist representations. This position has been challenged by recent findings of similar divided attention (DA) at encoding costs on both specific and gist representations. However, the disrupting effects of DA on specific representations may emerge under less difficult DA conditions than those under which effects on gist representations emerge. The present study addressed this possibility by manipulating the difficulty of a concurrent DA task (low, intermediate, or high difficulty) during encoding among 176 young adult participants, who encoded face-scene pairs under either full attention or one of the three levels of DA. During retrieval, participants discriminated intact pairs from recombined pairs that varied in how similar they were to studied pairs. Results, interpreted using a multinomial-processing-tree model of specific and gist memory, showed that the disrupting effects of DA on specific representations emerged under less difficult attentional loads (intermediate-demanding condition) compared to those under which gist representations were disrupted (high-demanding condition). These findings reinforce the suggestion of differential attentional demands for specific and gist representations and also provide insights into attentional resource theories of adult age-related cognitive decline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

情节记忆理论认为,与要点表征相比,编码特定表征需要更多的注意力资源。这一立场受到了最近在特定表示和要点表示的编码成本上类似的注意力分散(DA)的发现的挑战。然而,DA对特定表征的破坏作用可能在比对要点表征产生影响的DA条件更难的DA条件下出现。本研究通过在176名年轻成年参与者的编码过程中操纵并发DA任务的难度(低难度、中等难度或高难度)来解决这种可能性,这些参与者在完全注意力或三个DA水平之一的情况下编码人脸场景对,参与者将完整配对与重组配对区分开来,重组配对与研究配对的相似程度各不相同。使用特定记忆和要点记忆的多项式处理树模型解释的结果表明,与要点表征被破坏的情况(高要求条件)相比,DA对特定表征的破坏作用在难度较小的注意负荷(中等要求条件)下出现。这些发现强化了对特定表征和要点表征的不同注意需求的建议,也为成人年龄相关认知能力下降的注意资源理论提供了见解。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Differential attentional costs of encoding specific and gist episodic memory representations.","authors":"Nathaniel R Greene,&nbsp;Moshe Naveh-Benjamin","doi":"10.1037/xge0001448","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001448","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories of episodic memory posit that more attentional resources are needed for encoding specific compared to gist representations. This position has been challenged by recent findings of similar divided attention (DA) at encoding costs on both specific and gist representations. However, the disrupting effects of DA on specific representations may emerge under less difficult DA conditions than those under which effects on gist representations emerge. The present study addressed this possibility by manipulating the difficulty of a concurrent DA task (low, intermediate, or high difficulty) during encoding among 176 young adult participants, who encoded face-scene pairs under either full attention or one of the three levels of DA. During retrieval, participants discriminated intact pairs from recombined pairs that varied in how similar they were to studied pairs. Results, interpreted using a multinomial-processing-tree model of specific and gist memory, showed that the disrupting effects of DA on specific representations emerged under less difficult attentional loads (intermediate-demanding condition) compared to those under which gist representations were disrupted (high-demanding condition). These findings reinforce the suggestion of differential attentional demands for specific and gist representations and also provide insights into attentional resource theories of adult age-related cognitive decline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3292-3299"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9840887","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Knowledge updating in real-world estimation: Connecting hindsight bias and seeding effects. 现实世界估计中的知识更新:将后见之明的偏见和种子效应联系起来。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-03 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001452
Julia Groß, Barbara K Kreis, Hartmut Blank, Thorsten Pachur

When people estimate the quantities of objects (e.g., country populations), are then presented with the objects' actual quantities, and subsequently asked to remember their initial estimates, responses are often distorted towards the actual quantities. This hindsight bias-traditionally considered to reflect a cognitive error-has more recently been proposed to result from adaptive knowledge updating. But how to conceptualize such knowledge-updating processes and their potentially beneficial consequences? Here, we provide a framework that conceptualizes knowledge updating in the context of hindsight bias in real-world estimation by connecting it with research on seeding effects-improvements in people's estimation accuracy after exposure to numerical facts. This integrative perspective highlights a previously neglected facet of knowledge updating, namely, recalibration of metric domain knowledge, which can be expected to lead to transfer learning and thus improve estimation for objects from a domain more generally. We develop an experimental paradigm to investigate the association of hindsight bias with improved estimation accuracy. In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that the classical approach to induce hindsight bias indeed produces transfer learning. In Experiment 2, we provide evidence for the novel prediction that hindsight bias can be triggered via transfer learning; this establishes a direct link from knowledge updating to hindsight bias. Our work integrates two prominent but previously unconnected research programs on the effects of knowledge updating in real-world estimation and supports the notion that hindsight bias is driven by adaptive learning processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当人们估计物体的数量(例如,国家人口),然后向他们提供物体的实际数量,然后要求他们记住最初的估计时,人们的反应往往会扭曲为实际数量。这种传统上被认为反映认知错误的后见之明的偏见最近被认为是自适应知识更新的结果。但是,如何将这种知识更新过程及其潜在的有益后果概念化呢?在这里,我们提供了一个框架,通过将知识更新与暴露于数字事实后人们估计准确性的种子效应改进研究联系起来,将知识更新概念化为现实世界估计中的事后偏差。这种综合视角突出了知识更新的一个以前被忽视的方面,即对度量域知识的重新校准,这可以预期会导致迁移学习,从而提高对来自更广泛领域的对象的估计。我们开发了一个实验范式来研究后见之明偏差与提高估计准确性的关系。在实验1中,我们证明了诱导后见偏差的经典方法确实产生了迁移学习。在实验2中,我们为新的预测提供了证据,即后见偏差可以通过迁移学习触发;这建立了从知识更新到事后偏见的直接联系。我们的工作整合了两个关于知识更新在现实世界估计中的影响的突出但以前没有联系的研究项目,并支持后见之明的偏见是由自适应学习过程驱动的这一观点。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Knowledge updating in real-world estimation: Connecting hindsight bias and seeding effects.","authors":"Julia Groß, Barbara K Kreis, Hartmut Blank, Thorsten Pachur","doi":"10.1037/xge0001452","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001452","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When people estimate the quantities of objects (e.g., country populations), are then presented with the objects' actual quantities, and subsequently asked to remember their initial estimates, responses are often distorted towards the actual quantities. This <i>hindsight bias</i>-traditionally considered to reflect a cognitive error-has more recently been proposed to result from adaptive knowledge updating. But how to conceptualize such knowledge-updating processes and their potentially beneficial consequences? Here, we provide a framework that conceptualizes knowledge updating in the context of hindsight bias in real-world estimation by connecting it with research on <i>seeding</i> <i>effects</i>-improvements in people's estimation accuracy after exposure to numerical facts. This integrative perspective highlights a previously neglected facet of knowledge updating, namely, recalibration of metric domain knowledge, which can be expected to lead to transfer learning and thus improve estimation for objects from a domain more generally. We develop an experimental paradigm to investigate the association of hindsight bias with improved estimation accuracy. In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that the classical approach to induce hindsight bias indeed produces transfer learning. In Experiment 2, we provide evidence for the novel prediction that hindsight bias can be triggered via transfer learning; this establishes a direct link from knowledge updating to hindsight bias. Our work integrates two prominent but previously unconnected research programs on the effects of knowledge updating in real-world estimation and supports the notion that hindsight bias is driven by adaptive learning processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3167-3188"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9988101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conscientiousness does not moderate the association between political ideology and susceptibility to fake news sharing. 良心并不能缓和政治意识形态和容易分享假新闻之间的联系。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001467
Hause Lin, David G Rand, Gordon Pennycook

Recent work suggests that personality moderates the relationship between political ideology and the sharing of misinformation. Specifically, Lawson and Kakkar (2022) claimed that fake news sharing was driven mostly by low conscientiousness conservatives. We reanalyzed their data and conducted five new preregistered conceptual replications to reexamine their claims (N = 2,433; stopping rule determined via Bayesian sequential sampling). The results did not support their claim that conscientious conservatives shared less fake news; instead, their findings pertain to overall sharing rates (of both true and fake news), rather than specifically to fake news. That is, the association between conscientiousness and misinformation sharing (when it occurs) is explained by lower overall sharing instead of a particular resistance to fake news per se. Our results highlight the importance of distinguishing between overall sharing tendencies and the sharing of misinformation specifically, which have different theoretical and practical implications for how to combat the spread of misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

最近的研究表明,个性调节了政治意识形态和分享错误信息之间的关系。具体而言,Lawson和Kakkar(2022)声称,假新闻分享主要是由低责任心的保守派推动的。我们重新分析了他们的数据,并进行了五次新的预先注册的概念复制,以重新检查他们的主张(N=2433;通过贝叶斯顺序抽样确定停止规则)。调查结果并不支持他们的说法,即有良知的保守派分享的假新闻较少;相反,他们的发现与(真实和虚假新闻的)总体分享率有关,而不是专门与虚假新闻有关。也就是说,尽责性和错误信息共享(当它发生时)之间的联系可以用较低的总体共享来解释,而不是对假新闻本身的特别抵制。我们的研究结果强调了区分总体共享趋势和错误信息的共享的重要性,特别是,这对如何打击错误信息的传播具有不同的理论和实践意义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Conscientiousness does not moderate the association between political ideology and susceptibility to fake news sharing.","authors":"Hause Lin, David G Rand, Gordon Pennycook","doi":"10.1037/xge0001467","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001467","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent work suggests that personality moderates the relationship between political ideology and the sharing of misinformation. Specifically, Lawson and Kakkar (2022) claimed that fake news sharing was driven mostly by low conscientiousness conservatives. We reanalyzed their data and conducted five new preregistered conceptual replications to reexamine their claims (<i>N</i> = 2,433; stopping rule determined via Bayesian sequential sampling). The results did not support their claim that conscientious conservatives shared less fake news; instead, their findings pertain to overall sharing rates (of both true and fake news), rather than specifically to fake news. That is, the association between conscientiousness and misinformation sharing (when it occurs) is explained by lower overall sharing instead of a particular resistance to fake news per se. Our results highlight the importance of distinguishing between overall sharing tendencies and the sharing of misinformation specifically, which have different theoretical and practical implications for how to combat the spread of misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3277-3284"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10034609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When does intent matter for memory? Bridging perspectives with Craik. 意图对记忆什么时候重要?与Craik沟通观点。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001486
Hannah Dames, Vencislav Popov

In his commentary, Craik (see record 2023-42323-002) argued that while intentional remembering might be effective for some populations and memory tasks, these are the exception, and that intent will not benefit memory if incidental encoding induces optimal processing. While we agree on many points, we maintain that in most situations the processes induced by the intention to remember are more effective than those induced by deep semantic processing alone. We show that effects of intent appear with a variety of tasks such as free recall, cued recall, source memory, item recognition, in both mixed and pure lists, and when studying individual words, word pairs, or word-image associations. Thus, the beneficial effects of intent generalize over tasks, list-compositions, and study materials. There are also little individual differences in the effect of intent in healthy young adults-most participants (86% out of 336 participants over six experiments) show a beneficial effect. We review evidence that the intent to remember strengthens item-context bindings in episodic memory, but that such effects could be masked and not measurable in subsequent memory tests, unless intrusions are taken into account. We agree that in principle incidental encoding could induce optimal processing, but we do not find the existing evidence convincing. We provide additional novel data that directly addresses Craik's (2023) concerns and we propose ways to further investigate how intention enhances memory. We conclude with a joint statement, coauthored by Craik and ourselves, that synthesizes our converging perspectives and current understanding of the impact of intent on memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在他的评论中,Craik(见记录2023-42323-002)认为,虽然有意记忆可能对某些群体和记忆任务有效,但这些都是例外,如果偶然编码诱导最佳处理,那么有意记忆将不会有益于记忆。虽然我们在很多方面都达成了一致,但我们坚持认为,在大多数情况下,由记忆意图引发的过程比单独由深层语义处理引发的过程更有效。我们发现,在混合和纯列表中,以及在研究单个单词、单词对或单词图像关联时,意图的影响会出现在各种任务中,如自由回忆、提示回忆、源记忆、项目识别。因此,意图的有益效果概括于任务、列出作文和学习材料。在健康的年轻人中,意向的效果也几乎没有个体差异——大多数参与者(在六个实验中336名参与者中有86%)显示出有益的效果。我们回顾了一些证据,证明记忆的意图加强了情景记忆中的项目上下文绑定,但除非考虑到入侵,否则这种影响可能会被掩盖,在随后的记忆测试中无法测量。我们同意原则上偶然编码可以诱导最佳处理,但我们认为现有的证据并不令人信服。我们提供了额外的新数据,直接解决了Craik(2023)的担忧,并提出了进一步研究意图如何增强记忆的方法。最后,我们发表了一份由Craik和我们自己共同撰写的联合声明,该声明综合了我们对意图对记忆影响的一致观点和当前理解。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"When does intent matter for memory? Bridging perspectives with Craik.","authors":"Hannah Dames, Vencislav Popov","doi":"10.1037/xge0001486","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001486","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In his commentary, Craik (see record 2023-42323-002) argued that while intentional remembering might be effective for some populations and memory tasks, these are the exception, and that intent will not benefit memory if incidental encoding induces optimal processing. While we agree on many points, we maintain that in most situations the processes induced by the intention to remember are more effective than those induced by deep semantic processing alone. We show that effects of intent appear with a variety of tasks such as free recall, cued recall, source memory, item recognition, in both mixed and pure lists, and when studying individual words, word pairs, or word-image associations. Thus, the beneficial effects of intent generalize over tasks, list-compositions, and study materials. There are also little individual differences in the effect of intent in healthy young adults-most participants (86% out of 336 participants over six experiments) show a beneficial effect. We review evidence that the intent to remember strengthens item-context bindings in episodic memory, but that such effects could be masked and not measurable in subsequent memory tests, unless intrusions are taken into account. We agree that in principle incidental encoding could induce optimal processing, but we do not find the existing evidence convincing. We provide additional novel data that directly addresses Craik's (2023) concerns and we propose ways to further investigate how intention enhances memory. We conclude with a joint statement, coauthored by Craik and ourselves, that synthesizes our converging perspectives and current understanding of the impact of intent on memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"152 11","pages":"3300-3309"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49678030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive control across the lifespan: Congruency effects reveal divergent developmental trajectories. 整个生命周期的认知控制:一致性效应揭示了不同的发展轨迹。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001429
Christopher D Erb, Laura Germine, Joshua K Hartshorne

The Simon, Stroop, and Eriksen flanker tasks are commonly used to assess cognitive control across the lifespan. However, it remains unclear whether these three tasks in fact measure the same cognitive abilities and in the same proportion. We take a developmental approach to this question: if the Simon, Stroop, and flanker tasks all roughly measure the same capacity, they should show similar patterns of age-related change. We present data from two massive online cross-sectional studies: Study 1 included 9,585 native English speakers between 10 and 80 years of age who completed the Simon and Stroop tasks, and Study 2 included 13,448 English speakers between 10 and 79 years of age who completed the flanker task. Of the three tasks, only the flanker task revealed an inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory, with performance improving until approximately 23 years of age and declining starting around 40 years of age. Performance on the Simon and Stroop tasks peaked around 34 and 26 years of age, respectively, and did not decline significantly in later life, though it is possible that age-related declines would be observed with more difficult versions of the tasks. Although the Simon and Stroop tasks are commonly interpreted to target similar underlying processes, we observed near zero correlations between the congruency effects observed in each task in terms of both accuracy and response time. We discuss these results in light of recent debates regarding the suitability of these tasks for assessing developmental and individual differences in cognitive control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

Simon、Stroop和Eriksen侧卫任务通常用于评估整个生命周期的认知控制。然而,目前尚不清楚这三项任务是否真的具有相同的认知能力,且比例相同。我们对这个问题采取了一种发展的方法:如果Simon、Stroop和侧卫任务都大致测量了相同的能力,那么它们应该表现出类似的年龄变化模式。我们提供了来自两项大规模在线横断面研究的数据:研究1包括9585名10至80岁的母语为英语的人,他们完成了Simon和Stroop任务,研究2包括13448名10至79岁的母语英语的人完成了侧卫任务。在这三项任务中,只有侧卫任务显示出倒U型的发展轨迹,表现在大约23岁之前有所改善,从40岁左右开始下降。Simon和Stroop任务的表现分别在34岁和26岁左右达到峰值,在以后的生活中没有显著下降,尽管在更困难的任务中可能会观察到与年龄相关的下降。尽管Simon和Stroop任务通常被解释为针对相似的底层过程,但我们观察到,在准确性和响应时间方面,每个任务中观察到的一致性效应之间的相关性几乎为零。我们根据最近关于这些任务是否适合评估认知控制的发展和个体差异的辩论来讨论这些结果。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Cognitive control across the lifespan: Congruency effects reveal divergent developmental trajectories.","authors":"Christopher D Erb, Laura Germine, Joshua K Hartshorne","doi":"10.1037/xge0001429","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001429","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Simon, Stroop, and Eriksen flanker tasks are commonly used to assess cognitive control across the lifespan. However, it remains unclear whether these three tasks in fact measure the same cognitive abilities and in the same proportion. We take a developmental approach to this question: if the Simon, Stroop, and flanker tasks all roughly measure the same capacity, they should show similar patterns of age-related change. We present data from two massive online cross-sectional studies: Study 1 included 9,585 native English speakers between 10 and 80 years of age who completed the Simon and Stroop tasks, and Study 2 included 13,448 English speakers between 10 and 79 years of age who completed the flanker task. Of the three tasks, only the flanker task revealed an inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory, with performance improving until approximately 23 years of age and declining starting around 40 years of age. Performance on the Simon and Stroop tasks peaked around 34 and 26 years of age, respectively, and did not decline significantly in later life, though it is possible that age-related declines would be observed with more difficult versions of the tasks. Although the Simon and Stroop tasks are commonly interpreted to target similar underlying processes, we observed near zero correlations between the congruency effects observed in each task in terms of both accuracy and response time. We discuss these results in light of recent debates regarding the suitability of these tasks for assessing developmental and individual differences in cognitive control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3285-3291"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9586785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pitfall or pratfall? Behavioral differences in infant learning from falling. 跌倒还是跌倒?婴儿跌倒后学习的行为差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-03 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001453
Danyang Han, Whitney G Cole, Amy S Joh, Yueqiao Liu, Scott R Robinson, Karen E Adolph

Researchers routinely infer learning and other unobservable psychological functions based on observable behavior. But what behavioral changes constitute evidence of learning? The standard approach is to infer learning based on a single behavior across individuals, including assumptions about the direction and magnitude of change (e.g., everyone should avoid falling repeatedly on a treacherous obstacle). Here we illustrate the benefits of an alternative "multiexpression, relativist, agnostic, individualized" approach. We assessed infant learning from falling based on multiple behaviors relative to each individual's baseline, agnostic about the direction and magnitude of behavioral change. We tested infants longitudinally (10.5-15 months of age) over the transition from crawling to walking. At each session, infants were repeatedly encouraged to crawl or walk over a fall-inducing foam pit interspersed with no-fall baseline trials on a rigid platform. Our approach revealed two learning profiles. Like adults in previous work, "pit-avoid" infants consistently avoided falling. In contrast, "pit-go" infants fell repeatedly across trials and sessions. However, individualized comparisons to baseline across multiple locomotor, exploratory, and social-emotional behaviors showed that pit-go infants also learned at every session. But they treated falling as an unimpactful "pratfall" rather than an aversive "pitfall." Pit-avoid infants displayed enhanced learning across sessions and partial transfer of learning from crawling to walking, whereas pit-go infants displayed neither. Thus, reliance on a predetermined, "one-size-fits-all" behavioral expression of a psychological function can obscure different behavioral profiles and lead to erroneous inferences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

研究人员通常根据可观察的行为来推断学习和其他不可观察的心理功能。但是,哪些行为变化构成了学习的证据?标准方法是根据个人的单一行为来推断学习,包括对变化方向和幅度的假设(例如,每个人都应该避免反复摔倒在危险的障碍物上)。在这里,我们展示了另一种“多元表达、相对主义、不可知论、个性化”方法的好处。我们根据相对于每个人基线的多种行为来评估婴儿从跌倒中的学习情况,不知道行为变化的方向和程度。我们对婴儿(10.5-15个月大)从爬行到行走的过渡过程进行了纵向测试。在每次训练中,婴儿都被反复鼓励在诱导跌倒的泡沫坑上爬行或行走,其间穿插着在刚性平台上进行的无跌倒基线试验。我们的方法揭示了两个学习概况。与之前工作中的成年人一样,“避坑”婴儿始终避免摔倒。相比之下,在试验和疗程中,“原地踏步”的婴儿反复摔倒。然而,对多种运动、探索和社交情绪行为与基线的个体化比较表明,pit-go婴儿在每次训练中也会学习。但他们将跌倒视为一种不实际的“失误”,而不是一种令人厌恶的“陷阱”。避坑婴儿在整个课程中表现出学习能力的增强,学习从爬行到行走的部分转移,而坑走婴儿则两者都没有。因此,对心理功能的预先确定的“一刀切”的行为表达的依赖可能会模糊不同的行为特征,并导致错误的推断。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Pitfall or pratfall? Behavioral differences in infant learning from falling.","authors":"Danyang Han, Whitney G Cole, Amy S Joh, Yueqiao Liu, Scott R Robinson, Karen E Adolph","doi":"10.1037/xge0001453","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001453","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers routinely infer learning and other unobservable psychological functions based on observable behavior. But what behavioral changes constitute evidence of learning? The standard approach is to infer learning based on a single behavior across individuals, including assumptions about the direction and magnitude of change (e.g., everyone should avoid falling repeatedly on a treacherous obstacle). Here we illustrate the benefits of an alternative \"multiexpression, relativist, agnostic, individualized\" approach. We assessed infant learning from falling based on multiple behaviors relative to each individual's baseline, agnostic about the direction and magnitude of behavioral change. We tested infants longitudinally (10.5-15 months of age) over the transition from crawling to walking. At each session, infants were repeatedly encouraged to crawl or walk over a fall-inducing foam pit interspersed with no-fall baseline trials on a rigid platform. Our approach revealed two learning profiles. Like adults in previous work, \"pit-avoid\" infants consistently avoided falling. In contrast, \"pit-go\" infants fell repeatedly across trials and sessions. However, individualized comparisons to baseline across multiple locomotor, exploratory, and social-emotional behaviors showed that pit-go infants also learned at every session. But they treated falling as an unimpactful \"pratfall\" rather than an aversive \"pitfall.\" Pit-avoid infants displayed enhanced learning across sessions and partial transfer of learning from crawling to walking, whereas pit-go infants displayed neither. Thus, reliance on a predetermined, \"one-size-fits-all\" behavioral expression of a psychological function can obscure different behavioral profiles and lead to erroneous inferences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3243-3265"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10592507/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10308410","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I am what I am: The role of essentialist beliefs and neurodivergent identification on individuals' self-efficacy. 我就是我:本质主义信念和神经分化认同对个体自我效能感的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-27 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001457
Alexa Lebrón-Cruz, Ariana Orvell

Essentialism is the belief that members of particular categories (e.g., social and cultural) are united by an innate underlying essence. While such beliefs have been associated with negative outcomes such as stereotyping, discrimination, and prejudice, minority group members can sometimes use essentialist beliefs to validate their identities. Here, we focus on people who identify as "neurodivergent"-individuals whose brains differ from typical neurology (such as those with autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, etc.). We examined whether endorsing essentialist beliefs about neurodivergence serves a protective function among 316 neurodivergent-identifying individuals. As expected, endorsing essentialist beliefs was related to higher self-efficacy. This was especially true of people who highly identified as neurodivergent. These results illuminate how essentialist beliefs may empower a group that is often negatively stereotyped. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本质主义是一种信念,认为特定类别(如社会和文化)的成员通过天生的内在本质而团结在一起。虽然这种信念与刻板印象、歧视和偏见等负面结果有关,但少数群体成员有时可以使用本质主义信念来验证他们的身份。在这里,我们关注的是那些被认定为“神经发散”的人,即大脑与典型神经病学不同的人(如自闭症、注意力缺陷多动障碍等)。正如预期的那样,支持本质主义信念与更高的自我效能感有关。对于那些高度认同神经分化的人来说尤其如此。这些结果说明了本质主义信仰如何赋予一个经常被负面刻板印象的群体权力。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"I am what I am: The role of essentialist beliefs and neurodivergent identification on individuals' self-efficacy.","authors":"Alexa Lebrón-Cruz, Ariana Orvell","doi":"10.1037/xge0001457","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001457","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Essentialism is the belief that members of particular categories (e.g., social and cultural) are united by an innate underlying essence. While such beliefs have been associated with negative outcomes such as stereotyping, discrimination, and prejudice, minority group members can sometimes use essentialist beliefs to validate their identities. Here, we focus on people who identify as \"neurodivergent\"-individuals whose brains differ from typical neurology (such as those with autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, etc.). We examined whether endorsing essentialist beliefs about neurodivergence serves a protective function among 316 neurodivergent-identifying individuals. As expected, endorsing essentialist beliefs was related to higher self-efficacy. This was especially true of people who highly identified as neurodivergent. These results illuminate how essentialist beliefs may empower a group that is often negatively stereotyped. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2995-3001"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9882825","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1