首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
Corrigendum to "Auxiliary Scene-Context Information Provided by Anchor Objects Guides Attention and Locomotion in Natural Search Behavior". “锚定对象提供的辅助场景上下文信息指导自然搜索行为中的注意和运动”的勘误表。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231154916
{"title":"Corrigendum to \"Auxiliary Scene-Context Information Provided by Anchor Objects Guides Attention and Locomotion in Natural Search Behavior\".","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/09567976231154916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231154916","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"523"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9312648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On How to Be Liked in First Encounters: The Effects of Agentic and Communal Behaviors on Popularity and Unique Liking. 如何在初次接触中被喜欢:代理和公共行为对受欢迎程度和独特喜欢的影响。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221147258
Michael Dufner, Sascha Krause

When meeting other people for the first time, how should one behave in order to be liked? We investigated the effects of agentic and communal behaviors on two forms of being liked: popularity (being generally liked by others) and unique liking (being uniquely liked by specific interaction partners). In a round-robin study, 139 unacquainted German adults had dyadic conversations and provided liking ratings afterward. The conversations were recorded on video, and four agentic behaviors (leading, dominant, confident, boastful) and four communal behaviors (polite, benevolent, warm, friendly) were each rated by trained observers. Participants who generally showed agentic and communal behavior were also generally liked (popularity). When participants' level of communal, but not agentic, behavior exceeded their personal standards during an interaction, they were particularly well-liked by the respective interaction partner (unique liking). The behavioral predictors of being liked thus differ, depending on whether one focuses on popularity or unique liking.

第一次和别人见面时,应该怎样做才能被人喜欢?我们调查了代理行为和公共行为对两种被喜欢形式的影响:受欢迎程度(被其他人普遍喜欢)和独特喜欢(被特定的互动伙伴独特喜欢)。在一项循环研究中,139名不认识的德国成年人进行了二元对话,并在之后提供了喜欢程度评分。这些对话被录成视频,然后由训练有素的观察员对四种代理行为(领导、支配、自信、自夸)和四种公共行为(礼貌、仁慈、热情、友好)进行打分。通常表现出主观和集体行为的参与者也通常被喜欢(受欢迎)。在互动过程中,当参与者的公共行为水平(而非代理行为)超过他们的个人标准时,他们会特别受到各自的互动伙伴的喜爱(独特的喜欢)。因此,被喜欢的行为预测因素是不同的,这取决于一个人关注的是受欢迎程度还是独特的喜欢程度。
{"title":"On How to Be Liked in First Encounters: The Effects of Agentic and Communal Behaviors on Popularity and Unique Liking.","authors":"Michael Dufner,&nbsp;Sascha Krause","doi":"10.1177/09567976221147258","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221147258","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When meeting other people for the first time, how should one behave in order to be liked? We investigated the effects of agentic and communal behaviors on two forms of being liked: popularity (being generally liked by others) and unique liking (being uniquely liked by specific interaction partners). In a round-robin study, 139 unacquainted German adults had dyadic conversations and provided liking ratings afterward. The conversations were recorded on video, and four agentic behaviors (leading, dominant, confident, boastful) and four communal behaviors (polite, benevolent, warm, friendly) were each rated by trained observers. Participants who generally showed agentic and communal behavior were also generally liked (popularity). When participants' level of communal, but not agentic, behavior exceeded their personal standards during an interaction, they were particularly well-liked by the respective interaction partner (unique liking). The behavioral predictors of being liked thus differ, depending on whether one focuses on popularity or unique liking.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"481-489"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9346827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Implicit-Bias-Oriented Diversity Training on Police Officers' Beliefs, Motivations, and Actions. 内隐偏见导向的多元性训练对警员信念、动机和行动的影响。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221150617
Calvin K Lai, Jaclyn A Lisnek

U.S. police departments have attempted to address racial inequities in policing with diversity training. However, little research has evaluated whether these trainings are effective at changing officers' beliefs, motivations, and actions. To examine their efficacy, we tested a day-long implicit-bias-oriented diversity training designed to increase U.S. police officers' knowledge of biases, concerns about bias, and use of evidence-based strategies to mitigate bias (total N = 3,764). The training was immediately effective at increasing knowledge about bias, concerns about bias, and intentions to address bias, relative to baseline. However, the effects were fleeting. Although the training was linked to higher knowledge for at least 1 month, it was ineffective at durably increasing concerns or strategy use. These findings suggest that diversity trainings as they are currently practiced are unlikely to change police behavior. We conclude with theorizing about what organizations and training programs could do for greater impact.

美国警察部门试图通过多元化培训来解决警务中的种族不平等问题。然而,很少有研究评估这些培训是否有效地改变了军官的信念、动机和行动。为了检验其有效性,我们测试了为期一天的内隐偏见导向的多样性培训,旨在提高美国警察对偏见的认识,对偏见的关注,以及使用基于证据的策略来减轻偏见(总N = 3,764)。相对于基线,培训在增加关于偏见的知识、对偏见的关注和解决偏见的意图方面立即有效。然而,效果是短暂的。虽然培训与至少1个月的高级知识有关,但它在持续增加关注或策略使用方面是无效的。这些发现表明,目前实行的多元化培训不太可能改变警察的行为。我们总结了组织和培训项目可以产生更大影响的理论。
{"title":"The Impact of Implicit-Bias-Oriented Diversity Training on Police Officers' Beliefs, Motivations, and Actions.","authors":"Calvin K Lai,&nbsp;Jaclyn A Lisnek","doi":"10.1177/09567976221150617","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221150617","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>U.S. police departments have attempted to address racial inequities in policing with diversity training. However, little research has evaluated whether these trainings are effective at changing officers' beliefs, motivations, and actions. To examine their efficacy, we tested a day-long implicit-bias-oriented diversity training designed to increase U.S. police officers' knowledge of biases, concerns about bias, and use of evidence-based strategies to mitigate bias (total <i>N</i> = 3,764). The training was immediately effective at increasing knowledge about bias, concerns about bias, and intentions to address bias, relative to baseline. However, the effects were fleeting. Although the training was linked to higher knowledge for at least 1 month, it was ineffective at durably increasing concerns or strategy use. These findings suggest that diversity trainings as they are currently practiced are unlikely to change police behavior. We conclude with theorizing about what organizations and training programs could do for greater impact.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"424-434"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9342285","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Transfer of Learned Cognitive Flexibility to Novel Stimuli and Task Sets. 习得的认知灵活性向新刺激和任务集的转移。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-24 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221141854
Tanya Wen, Raphael M Geddert, Seth Madlon-Kay, Tobias Egner

Adaptive behavior requires learning about the structure of one's environment to derive optimal action policies, and previous studies have documented transfer of such structural knowledge to bias choices in new environments. Here, we asked whether people could also acquire and transfer more abstract knowledge across different task environments, specifically expectations about cognitive control demands. Over three experiments, participants (Amazon Mechanical Turk workers; N = ~80 adults per group) performed a probabilistic card-sorting task in environments of either a low or high volatility of task rule changes (requiring low or high cognitive flexibility, respectively) before transitioning to a medium-volatility environment. Using reinforcement-learning modeling, we consistently found that previous exposure to high task rule volatilities led to faster adaptation to rule changes in the subsequent transfer phase. These transfers of expectations about cognitive flexibility demands were both task independent (Experiment 2) and stimulus independent (Experiment 3), thus demonstrating the formation and generalization of environmental structure knowledge to guide cognitive control.

适应性行为需要了解一个人的环境结构,以得出最优的行动策略,以前的研究已经证明,这种结构知识转移到新环境中的偏见选择。在这里,我们询问人们是否也可以在不同的任务环境中获得和转移更多的抽象知识,特别是对认知控制需求的期望。在三个实验中,参与者(亚马逊土耳其机器人工人;N =每组约80名成年人)在任务规则变化的低波动性或高波动性环境(分别要求低或高的认知灵活性)中执行概率卡片分类任务,然后过渡到中等波动性环境。使用强化学习建模,我们一致地发现,先前暴露于高任务规则波动性导致在随后的迁移阶段更快地适应规则变化。这些关于认知灵活性需求的期望转移既具有任务独立性(实验2),又具有刺激独立性(实验3),从而表明环境结构知识的形成和推广对认知控制具有指导作用。
{"title":"Transfer of Learned Cognitive Flexibility to Novel Stimuli and Task Sets.","authors":"Tanya Wen, Raphael M Geddert, Seth Madlon-Kay, Tobias Egner","doi":"10.1177/09567976221141854","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976221141854","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adaptive behavior requires learning about the structure of one's environment to derive optimal action policies, and previous studies have documented transfer of such structural knowledge to bias choices in new environments. Here, we asked whether people could also acquire and transfer more abstract knowledge across different task environments, specifically expectations about cognitive control demands. Over three experiments, participants (Amazon Mechanical Turk workers; <i>N</i> = ~80 adults per group) performed a probabilistic card-sorting task in environments of either a low or high volatility of task rule changes (requiring low or high cognitive flexibility, respectively) before transitioning to a medium-volatility environment. Using reinforcement-learning modeling, we consistently found that previous exposure to high task rule volatilities led to faster adaptation to rule changes in the subsequent transfer phase. These transfers of expectations about cognitive flexibility demands were both task independent (Experiment 2) and stimulus independent (Experiment 3), thus demonstrating the formation and generalization of environmental structure knowledge to guide cognitive control.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"435-454"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10236430/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10009854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-Cultural Sex/Gender Differences in Produced Word Content Before the Age of 3 Years. 三岁前生产词内容的跨文化性别差异
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221146537
Mikkel Wallentin, Fabio Trecca

Does sex/gender matter for language acquisition? Small advantages in vocabulary size for females are well documented. In this study, however, we found that children's early vocabulary composition was a significantly better predictor of sex/gender than their vocabulary size. We conducted classification analysis on word-production data from children (12-36 months old, n = 39,553) acquiring 26 different languages. Children's sex/gender was classified at above-chance levels in 22 of 26 languages. Classification accuracy was significantly higher than for models based on vocabulary size and increased as a function of sample size. Boys produced more words for vehicles and outdoor scenes, whereas girls produced more words for clothing and body parts. Classification accuracy also increased as a function of age and peaked at 30 months, reaching accuracy levels observed in studies of adult word use. These differences in vocabulary are indicative of differences in the lifeworld of children and may themselves cause further differences in development.

性/性别对语言习得有影响吗?女性在词汇量上的小优势是有据可查的。然而,在这项研究中,我们发现儿童早期的词汇构成比他们的词汇量更能预测性别/性别。我们对学习26种不同语言的儿童(12-36个月,n = 39,553)的单词生成数据进行了分类分析。在26种语言中,有22种对儿童的性/性别进行了高概率分类。分类精度显著高于基于词汇量的模型,并且随着样本量的增加而增加。男孩会用更多的词来形容车辆和户外场景,而女孩会用更多的词来形容衣服和身体部位。分类准确率也随着年龄的增长而增加,在30个月时达到顶峰,达到了在成人词汇使用研究中观察到的准确率水平。这些词汇上的差异表明了儿童生活世界的差异,并可能导致进一步的发展差异。
{"title":"Cross-Cultural Sex/Gender Differences in Produced Word Content Before the Age of 3 Years.","authors":"Mikkel Wallentin,&nbsp;Fabio Trecca","doi":"10.1177/09567976221146537","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221146537","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Does sex/gender matter for language acquisition? Small advantages in vocabulary size for females are well documented. In this study, however, we found that children's early vocabulary composition was a significantly better predictor of sex/gender than their vocabulary size. We conducted classification analysis on word-production data from children (12-36 months old, <i>n</i> = 39,553) acquiring 26 different languages. Children's sex/gender was classified at above-chance levels in 22 of 26 languages. Classification accuracy was significantly higher than for models based on vocabulary size and increased as a function of sample size. Boys produced more words for vehicles and outdoor scenes, whereas girls produced more words for clothing and body parts. Classification accuracy also increased as a function of age and peaked at 30 months, reaching accuracy levels observed in studies of adult word use. These differences in vocabulary are indicative of differences in the lifeworld of children and may themselves cause further differences in development.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"411-423"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9398179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Impaired and Spared Auditory Category Learning in Developmental Dyslexia. 发展性阅读障碍的听觉类别学习受损和幸免。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231151581
Yafit Gabay, Casey L Roark, Lori L Holt

Categorization has a deep impact on behavior, but whether category learning is served by a single system or multiple systems remains debated. Here, we designed two well-equated nonspeech auditory category learning challenges to draw on putative procedural (information-integration) versus declarative (rule-based) learning systems among adult Hebrew-speaking control participants and individuals with dyslexia, a language disorder that has been linked to a selective disruption in the procedural memory system and in which phonological deficits are ubiquitous. We observed impaired information-integration category learning and spared rule-based category learning in the dyslexia group compared with the neurotypical group. Quantitative model-based analyses revealed reduced use of, and slower shifting to, optimal procedural-based strategies in dyslexia with hypothesis-testing strategy use on par with control participants. The dissociation is consistent with multiple category learning systems and points to the possibility that procedural learning inefficiencies across categories defined by complex, multidimensional exemplars may result in difficulty in phonetic category acquisition in dyslexia.

分类对行为有深刻的影响,但类别学习是由单一系统还是多个系统提供服务仍然存在争议。在这里,我们设计了两个完全平等的非言语听觉类别学习挑战,以利用假定的程序性(信息整合)与陈述性(基于规则的)学习系统,在成年希伯来语控制组参与者和患有阅读障碍的个体中进行研究。阅读障碍是一种语言障碍,与程序记忆系统的选择性中断有关,语音缺陷普遍存在。我们观察到,与神经正常组相比,失读症组的信息整合类别学习和基于规则的类别学习受损。基于定量模型的分析显示,与对照组相比,使用假设测试策略的失读症患者使用基于程序的最佳策略的减少和转向速度较慢。这种分离与多类别学习系统是一致的,并指出了一种可能性,即由复杂的、多维的范例定义的类别之间的程序性学习效率低下可能导致阅读障碍患者语音类别习得困难。
{"title":"Impaired and Spared Auditory Category Learning in Developmental Dyslexia.","authors":"Yafit Gabay,&nbsp;Casey L Roark,&nbsp;Lori L Holt","doi":"10.1177/09567976231151581","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231151581","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Categorization has a deep impact on behavior, but whether category learning is served by a single system or multiple systems remains debated. Here, we designed two well-equated nonspeech auditory category learning challenges to draw on putative procedural (information-integration) versus declarative (rule-based) learning systems among adult Hebrew-speaking control participants and individuals with dyslexia, a language disorder that has been linked to a selective disruption in the procedural memory system and in which phonological deficits are ubiquitous. We observed impaired information-integration category learning and spared rule-based category learning in the dyslexia group compared with the neurotypical group. Quantitative model-based analyses revealed reduced use of, and slower shifting to, optimal procedural-based strategies in dyslexia with hypothesis-testing strategy use on par with control participants. The dissociation is consistent with multiple category learning systems and points to the possibility that procedural learning inefficiencies across categories defined by complex, multidimensional exemplars may result in difficulty in phonetic category acquisition in dyslexia.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"468-480"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9347297","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The Role of Beta Oscillations in Mental Time Travel. -振荡在心理时间旅行中的作用。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221147259
Mariano D'Angelo, Francesca Frassinetti, Marinella Cappelletti

The brain processes short-interval timing but also allows people to project themselves into the past and the future (i.e., mental time travel [MTT]). Beta oscillations index seconds-long-interval timing (i.e., higher beta power is associated with longer durations). Here, we used parietal transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) to investigate whether MTT is also supported by parietal beta oscillations and to test the link between MTT and short intervals. Thirty adults performed a novel MTT task while receiving beta and alpha tACS, in addition to no stimulation. Beta tACS corresponded to a temporal underestimation in past but not in future MTT. Furthermore, participants who overestimated seconds-long intervals also overestimated temporal distances in the past-projection MTT condition and showed a stronger effect of beta tACS. These data provide a unique window into temporal perception, showing how beta oscillations may be a common mechanism for short intervals and MTT.

大脑处理短间隔时间,但也允许人们将自己投射到过去和未来(即精神时间旅行[MTT])。β振荡指数秒-长间隔计时(即,更高的β功率与更长的持续时间有关)。在这里,我们使用顶叶经颅交流电刺激(tACS)来研究MTT是否也受到顶叶β振荡的支持,并测试MTT与短间隔之间的联系。30名成年人在接受β和α tACS刺激的同时进行了一项新的MTT任务。β tACS在过去的MTT中对应于时间低估,但在未来的MTT中不对应。此外,在过去投射MTT条件下,高估秒间隔的参与者也高估了时间距离,并表现出更强的β - tACS效应。这些数据为时间感知提供了一个独特的窗口,显示了β振荡如何可能是短时间间隔和MTT的共同机制。
{"title":"The Role of Beta Oscillations in Mental Time Travel.","authors":"Mariano D'Angelo,&nbsp;Francesca Frassinetti,&nbsp;Marinella Cappelletti","doi":"10.1177/09567976221147259","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221147259","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The brain processes short-interval timing but also allows people to project themselves into the past and the future (i.e., mental time travel [MTT]). Beta oscillations index seconds-long-interval timing (i.e., higher beta power is associated with longer durations). Here, we used parietal transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) to investigate whether MTT is also supported by parietal beta oscillations and to test the link between MTT and short intervals. Thirty adults performed a novel MTT task while receiving beta and alpha tACS, in addition to no stimulation. Beta tACS corresponded to a temporal underestimation in past but not in future MTT. Furthermore, participants who overestimated seconds-long intervals also overestimated temporal distances in the past-projection MTT condition and showed a stronger effect of beta tACS. These data provide a unique window into temporal perception, showing how beta oscillations may be a common mechanism for short intervals and MTT.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"490-500"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9344725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
What's in a Badge? A Computational Reproducibility Investigation of the Open Data Badge Policy in One Issue of Psychological Science. 徽章里有什么?开放数据徽章政策的计算再现性研究,发表于《心理科学》一期。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221140828
Sophia Crüwell, Deborah Apthorp, Bradley J Baker, Lincoln Colling, Malte Elson, Sandra J Geiger, Sebastian Lobentanzer, Jean Monéger, Alex Patterson, D Samuel Schwarzkopf, Mirela Zaneva, Nicholas J L Brown

In April 2019, Psychological Science published its first issue in which all Research Articles received the Open Data badge. We used that issue to investigate the effectiveness of this badge, focusing on the adherence to its aim at Psychological Science: sharing both data and code to ensure reproducibility of results. Twelve researchers of varying experience levels attempted to reproduce the results of the empirical articles in the target issue (at least three researchers per article). We found that all 14 articles provided at least some data and six provided analysis code, but only one article was rated to be exactly reproducible, and three were rated as essentially reproducible with minor deviations. We suggest that researchers should be encouraged to adhere to the higher standard in force at Psychological Science. Moreover, a check of reproducibility during peer review may be preferable to the disclosure method of awarding badges.

2019年4月,《心理科学》出版了第一期,其中所有研究文章都获得了开放数据徽章。我们利用这个问题来调查这个徽章的有效性,重点关注其对心理科学目标的坚持:共享数据和代码以确保结果的可重复性。12名不同经验水平的研究人员试图重现目标期刊中实证文章的结果(每篇文章至少有3名研究人员)。我们发现所有14篇文章都至少提供了一些数据,6篇文章提供了分析代码,但只有一篇文章被评为完全可复制,三篇文章被评为具有较小偏差的基本可复制。我们建议,应该鼓励研究人员遵循《心理科学》现行的更高标准。此外,在同行评议期间检查可重复性可能比授予徽章的披露方法更可取。
{"title":"What's in a Badge? A Computational Reproducibility Investigation of the Open Data Badge Policy in One Issue of <i>Psychological Science</i>.","authors":"Sophia Crüwell,&nbsp;Deborah Apthorp,&nbsp;Bradley J Baker,&nbsp;Lincoln Colling,&nbsp;Malte Elson,&nbsp;Sandra J Geiger,&nbsp;Sebastian Lobentanzer,&nbsp;Jean Monéger,&nbsp;Alex Patterson,&nbsp;D Samuel Schwarzkopf,&nbsp;Mirela Zaneva,&nbsp;Nicholas J L Brown","doi":"10.1177/09567976221140828","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221140828","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In April 2019, <i>Psychological Science</i> published its first issue in which all Research Articles received the Open Data badge. We used that issue to investigate the effectiveness of this badge, focusing on the adherence to its aim at <i>Psychological Science</i>: sharing both data and code to ensure reproducibility of results. Twelve researchers of varying experience levels attempted to reproduce the results of the empirical articles in the target issue (at least three researchers per article). We found that all 14 articles provided at least some data and six provided analysis code, but only one article was rated to be exactly reproducible, and three were rated as essentially reproducible with minor deviations. We suggest that researchers should be encouraged to adhere to the higher standard in force at <i>Psychological Science</i>. Moreover, a check of reproducibility during peer review may be preferable to the disclosure method of awarding badges.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"512-522"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9953249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Awe Sparks Prosociality in Children. 敬畏激发孩子的亲社会性。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221150616
Eftychia Stamkou, Eddie Brummelman, Rohan Dunham, Milica Nikolic, Dacher Keltner

Rooted in the novel and the mysterious, awe is a common experience in childhood, but research is almost silent with respect to the import of this emotion for children. Awe makes individuals feel small, thereby shifting their attention to the social world. Here, we studied the effects of art-elicited awe on children's prosocial behavior toward an out-group and its unique physiological correlates. In two preregistered studies (Study 1: N = 159, Study 2: N = 353), children between 8 and 13 years old viewed movie clips that elicited awe, joy, or a neutral (control) response. Children who watched the awe-eliciting clip were more likely to spend their time on an effortful task (Study 1) and to donate their experimental earnings (Studies 1 and 2), all toward benefiting refugees. They also exhibited increased respiratory sinus arrhythmia, an index of parasympathetic nervous system activation associated with social engagement. We discuss implications for fostering prosociality by reimagining children's environments to inspire awe at a critical age.

敬畏根植于小说和神秘之中,是童年的一种常见体验,但研究几乎没有提到这种情感对儿童的重要性。敬畏使个人感到渺小,从而将他们的注意力转移到社会世界。本研究研究了艺术诱发敬畏对儿童外群体亲社会行为的影响及其独特的生理关联。在两项预先登记的研究中(研究1:N = 159,研究2:N = 353), 8至13岁的儿童观看了引起敬畏、喜悦或中性(对照)反应的电影片段。观看了令人敬畏的视频片段的孩子们更有可能把时间花在费力的任务上(研究1),并把他们的实验所得(研究1和研究2)捐给难民。他们还表现出呼吸性窦性心律失常的增加,这是与社交参与相关的副交感神经系统激活的指标。我们讨论了通过重新想象儿童环境来培养亲社会性的影响,以在关键年龄激发敬畏。
{"title":"Awe Sparks Prosociality in Children.","authors":"Eftychia Stamkou,&nbsp;Eddie Brummelman,&nbsp;Rohan Dunham,&nbsp;Milica Nikolic,&nbsp;Dacher Keltner","doi":"10.1177/09567976221150616","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221150616","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rooted in the novel and the mysterious, awe is a common experience in childhood, but research is almost silent with respect to the import of this emotion for children. Awe makes individuals feel small, thereby shifting their attention to the social world. Here, we studied the effects of art-elicited awe on children's prosocial behavior toward an out-group and its unique physiological correlates. In two preregistered studies (Study 1: <i>N</i> = 159, Study 2: <i>N</i> = 353), children between 8 and 13 years old viewed movie clips that elicited awe, joy, or a neutral (control) response. Children who watched the awe-eliciting clip were more likely to spend their time on an effortful task (Study 1) and to donate their experimental earnings (Studies 1 and 2), all toward benefiting refugees. They also exhibited increased respiratory sinus arrhythmia, an index of parasympathetic nervous system activation associated with social engagement. We discuss implications for fostering prosociality by reimagining children's environments to inspire awe at a critical age.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 4","pages":"455-467"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9342102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Universal Constraints on Linguistic Event Categories: A Cross-Cultural Study of Child Homesign. 语言事件范畴的普遍约束:儿童家语的跨文化研究。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221140328
Lilia Rissman, Laura Horton, Susan Goldin-Meadow

Languages carve up conceptual space in varying ways-for example, English uses the verb cut both for cutting with a knife and for cutting with scissors, but other languages use distinct verbs for these events. We asked whether, despite this variability, there are universal constraints on how languages categorize events involving tools (e.g., knife-cutting). We analyzed descriptions of tool events from two groups: (a) 43 hearing adult speakers of English, Spanish, and Chinese and (b) 10 deaf child homesigners ages 3 to 11 (each of whom has created a gestural language without input from a conventional language model) in five different countries (Guatemala, Nicaragua, United States, Taiwan, Turkey). We found alignment across these two groups-events that elicited tool-prominent language among the spoken-language users also elicited tool-prominent language among the homesigners. These results suggest ways of conceptualizing tool events that are so prominent as to constitute a universal constraint on how events are categorized in language.

语言以不同的方式分割概念空间——例如,英语使用动词cut来表示用刀切割和用剪刀切割,但其他语言使用不同的动词来表示这些事件。我们问,尽管存在这种可变性,但语言如何对涉及工具的事件(例如,刀切割)进行分类是否存在普遍的约束。我们分析了两组对工具事件的描述:(a)来自五个不同国家(危地马拉、尼加拉瓜、美国、台湾、土耳其)的43名说英语、西班牙语和中文的听力正常的成年人和(b) 10名3至11岁的失聪儿童(他们每个人都创造了一种没有从传统语言模型输入的手势语言)。我们发现这两个群体之间的一致性——在使用口语的用户中引起工具突出语言的事件,也在使用手语的用户中引起工具突出语言的事件。这些结果提出了概念化工具事件的方法,这些工具事件是如此突出,以至于构成了如何在语言中对事件进行分类的普遍约束。
{"title":"Universal Constraints on Linguistic Event Categories: A Cross-Cultural Study of Child Homesign.","authors":"Lilia Rissman,&nbsp;Laura Horton,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow","doi":"10.1177/09567976221140328","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221140328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Languages carve up conceptual space in varying ways-for example, English uses the verb <i>cut</i> both for cutting with a knife and for cutting with scissors, but other languages use distinct verbs for these events. We asked whether, despite this variability, there are universal constraints on how languages categorize events involving tools (e.g., knife-cutting). We analyzed descriptions of tool events from two groups: (a) 43 hearing adult speakers of English, Spanish, and Chinese and (b) 10 deaf child homesigners ages 3 to 11 (each of whom has created a gestural language without input from a conventional language model) in five different countries (Guatemala, Nicaragua, United States, Taiwan, Turkey). We found alignment across these two groups-events that elicited tool-prominent language among the spoken-language users also elicited tool-prominent language among the homesigners. These results suggest ways of conceptualizing tool events that are so prominent as to constitute a universal constraint on how events are categorized in language.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 3","pages":"298-312"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9260329","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1