首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
Manipulating Prior Beliefs Causally Induces Under- and Overconfidence. 操纵先前的信念会诱发信心不足和过度自信。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241231572
Hélène Van Marcke, Pierre Le Denmat, Tom Verguts, Kobe Desender

Humans differ vastly in the confidence they assign to decisions. Although such under- and overconfidence relate to fundamental life outcomes, a computational account specifying the underlying mechanisms is currently lacking. We propose that prior beliefs in the ability to perform a task explain confidence differences across participants and tasks, despite similar performance. In two perceptual decision-making experiments, we show that manipulating prior beliefs about performance during training causally influences confidence in healthy adults (N = 50 each; Experiment 1: 8 men, one nonbinary; Experiment 2: 5 men) during a test phase, despite unaffected objective performance. This is true when prior beliefs are induced via manipulated comparative feedback and via manipulated training-phase difficulty. Our results were accounted for within an accumulation-to-bound model, explicitly modeling prior beliefs on the basis of earlier task exposure. Decision confidence is quantified as the probability of being correct conditional on prior beliefs, causing under- or overconfidence. We provide a fundamental mechanistic insight into the computations underlying under- and overconfidence.

人类对决策的信心存在巨大差异。虽然这种信心不足和过度自信与基本的生活结果有关,但目前还缺乏一个具体说明其潜在机制的计算账户。我们提出,尽管参与者和任务的表现相似,但对完成任务能力的先验信念可以解释不同参与者和任务之间的信心差异。在两个感知决策实验中,我们发现,尽管客观表现未受影响,但在训练期间操纵对表现的先验信念会在测试阶段对健康成年人(各50人;实验1:8名男性,1名非二元;实验2:5名男性)的信心产生因果影响。当通过操纵比较反馈和操纵训练阶段难度来诱导先验信念时,情况也是如此。我们的结果是在积累到约束模型中得到解释的,该模型明确地将先前的信念建立在先前任务暴露的基础上。决策信心被量化为以先前信念为条件的正确概率,从而导致信心不足或过度自信。我们从根本上揭示了自信不足和过度自信的计算机制。
{"title":"Manipulating Prior Beliefs Causally Induces Under- and Overconfidence.","authors":"Hélène Van Marcke, Pierre Le Denmat, Tom Verguts, Kobe Desender","doi":"10.1177/09567976241231572","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241231572","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans differ vastly in the confidence they assign to decisions. Although such under- and overconfidence relate to fundamental life outcomes, a computational account specifying the underlying mechanisms is currently lacking. We propose that prior beliefs in the ability to perform a task explain confidence differences across participants and tasks, despite similar performance. In two perceptual decision-making experiments, we show that manipulating prior beliefs about performance during training causally influences confidence in healthy adults (<i>N</i> = 50 each; Experiment 1: 8 men, one nonbinary; Experiment 2: 5 men) during a test phase, despite unaffected objective performance. This is true when prior beliefs are induced via manipulated comparative feedback and via manipulated training-phase difficulty. Our results were accounted for within an accumulation-to-bound model, explicitly modeling prior beliefs on the basis of earlier task exposure. Decision confidence is quantified as the probability of being correct conditional on prior beliefs, causing under- or overconfidence. We provide a fundamental mechanistic insight into the computations underlying under- and overconfidence.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"358-375"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Invalid Information Be Ignored When It Is Detected? 检测到无效信息后是否可以忽略?
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241231571
Adam T Ramsey, Yanjun Liu, Jennifer S Trueblood

With the rapid spread of information via social media, individuals are prone to misinformation exposure that they may utilize when forming beliefs. Over five experiments (total N = 815 adults, recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk in the United States), we investigated whether people could ignore quantitative information when they judged for themselves that it was misreported. Participants recruited online viewed sets of values sampled from Gaussian distributions to estimate the underlying means. They attempted to ignore invalid information, which were outlier values inserted into the value sequences. Results indicated participants were able to detect outliers. Nevertheless, participants' estimates were still biased in the direction of the outlier, even when they were most certain that they detected invalid information. The addition of visual warning cues and different task scenarios did not fully eliminate systematic over- and underestimation. These findings suggest that individuals may incorporate invalid information they meant to ignore when forming beliefs.

随着社交媒体信息的快速传播,人们很容易接触到错误信息,并在形成信念时加以利用。在五次实验中(总人数 = 815 名成人,通过美国亚马逊 Mechanical Turk 招募),我们研究了当人们自己判断定量信息被误报时,是否会忽略这些信息。在线招募的参与者查看从高斯分布中采样的数值集,以估算基本均值。他们试图忽略无效信息,即插入数值序列的离群值。结果表明,参与者能够发现离群值。然而,即使在最确定自己检测到了无效信息的情况下,参与者的估计值仍然偏向离群值的方向。增加视觉警告提示和不同的任务情景并不能完全消除系统性高估和低估。这些研究结果表明,个体在形成信念时,可能会纳入他们本想忽略的无效信息。
{"title":"Can Invalid Information Be Ignored When It Is Detected?","authors":"Adam T Ramsey, Yanjun Liu, Jennifer S Trueblood","doi":"10.1177/09567976241231571","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241231571","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>With the rapid spread of information via social media, individuals are prone to misinformation exposure that they may utilize when forming beliefs. Over five experiments (total <i>N</i> = 815 adults, recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk in the United States), we investigated whether people could ignore quantitative information when they judged for themselves that it was misreported. Participants recruited online viewed sets of values sampled from Gaussian distributions to estimate the underlying means. They attempted to ignore invalid information, which were outlier values inserted into the value sequences. Results indicated participants were able to detect outliers. Nevertheless, participants' estimates were still biased in the direction of the outlier, even when they were most certain that they detected invalid information. The addition of visual warning cues and different task scenarios did not fully eliminate systematic over- and underestimation. These findings suggest that individuals may incorporate invalid information they meant to ignore when forming beliefs.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"328-344"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140132457","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bridging the Gap Between Self-Report and Behavioral Laboratory Measures: A Real-Time Driving Task With Inverse Reinforcement Learning. 缩小自我报告与行为实验室测量之间的差距:反强化学习的实时驾驶任务
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241228503
Sang Ho Lee, Myeong Seop Song, Min-Hwan Oh, Woo-Young Ahn

A major challenge in assessing psychological constructs such as impulsivity is the weak correlation between self-report and behavioral task measures that are supposed to assess the same construct. To address this issue, we developed a real-time driving task called the "highway task," in which participants often exhibit impulsive behaviors mirroring real-life impulsive traits captured by self-report questionnaires. Here, we show that a self-report measure of impulsivity is highly correlated with performance in the highway task but not with traditional behavioral task measures of impulsivity (47 adults aged 18-33 years). By integrating deep neural networks with an inverse reinforcement learning (IRL) algorithm, we inferred dynamic changes of subjective rewards during the highway task. The results indicated that impulsive participants attribute high subjective rewards to irrational or risky situations. Overall, our results suggest that using real-time tasks combined with IRL can help reconcile the discrepancy between self-report and behavioral task measures of psychological constructs.

在评估冲动性等心理结构时,一个主要挑战是自我报告和行为任务测量之间的相关性很弱,而这些测量本应评估同一结构。为了解决这个问题,我们开发了一种名为 "高速公路任务 "的实时驾驶任务,在这项任务中,参与者经常会表现出冲动行为,这与自我报告问卷中捕捉到的现实生活中的冲动特征如出一辙。在这里,我们展示了自我报告的冲动性测量结果与高速公路任务中的表现高度相关,但与传统行为任务中的冲动性测量结果并不相关(47 名 18-33 岁的成年人)。通过将深度神经网络与反强化学习(IRL)算法相结合,我们推断出了高速公路任务中主观奖励的动态变化。结果表明,冲动型参与者会将高主观奖励归因于不合理或有风险的情况。总之,我们的研究结果表明,使用实时任务结合 IRL 可以帮助调和自我报告和行为任务测量心理结构之间的差异。
{"title":"Bridging the Gap Between Self-Report and Behavioral Laboratory Measures: A Real-Time Driving Task With Inverse Reinforcement Learning.","authors":"Sang Ho Lee, Myeong Seop Song, Min-Hwan Oh, Woo-Young Ahn","doi":"10.1177/09567976241228503","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241228503","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A major challenge in assessing psychological constructs such as impulsivity is the weak correlation between self-report and behavioral task measures that are supposed to assess the same construct. To address this issue, we developed a real-time driving task called the \"highway task,\" in which participants often exhibit impulsive behaviors mirroring real-life impulsive traits captured by self-report questionnaires. Here, we show that a self-report measure of impulsivity is highly correlated with performance in the highway task but not with traditional behavioral task measures of impulsivity (47 adults aged 18-33 years). By integrating deep neural networks with an inverse reinforcement learning (IRL) algorithm, we inferred dynamic changes of subjective rewards during the highway task. The results indicated that impulsive participants attribute high subjective rewards to irrational or risky situations. Overall, our results suggest that using real-time tasks combined with IRL can help reconcile the discrepancy between self-report and behavioral task measures of psychological constructs.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"345-357"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139973175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do They Look the Same Unless They Are Angry? Investigating the Other-Race Effect in the Presence of Angry Expressions. 除非愤怒,否则他们看起来都一样吗?调查愤怒表情下的他种效应。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231218640
Roland Imhoff, Barbara C N Müller, Verena Heidrich

Ethnic out-group members are disproportionately more often the victim of misidentifications. The so-called other-race effect (ORE), the tendency to better remember faces of individuals belonging to one's own ethnic in-group than faces belonging to an ethnic out-group, has been identified as one causal ingredient in such tragic incidents. Investigating an important aspect for the ORE-that is, emotional expression-the seminal study by Ackerman and colleagues (2006) found that White participants remembered neutral White faces better than neutral Black faces, but crucially, Black angry faces were better remembered than White angry faces (i.e., a reversed ORE). In the current study, we sought to replicate this study and directly tackle the potential causes for different results with later work. Three hundred ninety-six adult White U.S. citizens completed our study in which we manipulated the kind of employed stimuli (as in the original study vs. more standardized ones) whether participants knew of the recognition task already at the encoding phase. Additionally, participants were asked about the unusualness of the presented faces. We were able to replicate results from the Ackerman et al. (2006) study with the original stimuli but not with more standardized stimuli.

族群外成员被误认的比例更高。所谓的 "其他种族效应"(other-race effect,ORE),即与属于外来族群的面孔相比,人们更容易记住属于本族群的人的面孔。阿克曼及其同事(2006 年)的开创性研究发现,白人参与者对中性的白人面孔的记忆好于中性的黑人面孔,但至关重要的是,黑人愤怒面孔的记忆好于白人愤怒面孔(即相反的 ORE)。在本研究中,我们试图复制这项研究,并直接解决与后来研究结果不同的潜在原因。有 396 名成年美国白人公民完成了我们的研究,在这项研究中,我们对受试者在编码阶段是否已经知道识别任务进行了操作,即受试者使用的刺激物种类(如原始研究中的刺激物与更标准化的刺激物)。此外,我们还询问了参与者所呈现的人脸的不寻常性。我们使用原始刺激物复制了 Ackerman 等人(2006 年)的研究结果,但没有使用更标准化的刺激物。
{"title":"Do They Look the Same Unless They Are Angry? Investigating the Other-Race Effect in the Presence of Angry Expressions.","authors":"Roland Imhoff, Barbara C N Müller, Verena Heidrich","doi":"10.1177/09567976231218640","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231218640","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ethnic out-group members are disproportionately more often the victim of misidentifications. The so-called other-race effect (ORE), the tendency to better remember faces of individuals belonging to one's own ethnic in-group than faces belonging to an ethnic out-group, has been identified as one causal ingredient in such tragic incidents. Investigating an important aspect for the ORE-that is, emotional expression-the seminal study by Ackerman and colleagues (2006) found that White participants remembered neutral White faces better than neutral Black faces, but crucially, Black angry faces were better remembered than White angry faces (i.e., a reversed ORE). In the current study, we sought to replicate this study and directly tackle the potential causes for different results with later work. Three hundred ninety-six adult White U.S. citizens completed our study in which we manipulated the kind of employed stimuli (as in the original study vs. more standardized ones) whether participants knew of the recognition task already at the encoding phase. Additionally, participants were asked about the unusualness of the presented faces. We were able to replicate results from the Ackerman et al. (2006) study with the original stimuli but not with more standardized stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"405-414"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140137140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
White by Another Name? Can Anti-Christian Bias Claims Serve as a Racial Dog Whistle? 白人的另一个名字?反基督教偏见的说法能否成为种族问题的 "哨子"?
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241236162
Rosemary L Al-Kire, Chad A Miller, Michael H Pasek, Samuel L Perry, Clara L Wilkins

Four preregistered experiments (N = 4,307) explored whether anti-Christian bias claims can discreetly signal White allyship among Christian American adults. In Experiments 1 and 2, reading about anti-Christian bias led White, but not Black, Christians to perceive more anti-White bias. Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrate the connection between Christian and White can be leveraged by politicians in the form of a racial dog whistle. In Experiment 3, White Christians perceived a politician concerned about anti-Christian bias as caring more about anti-White bias and more willing to fight for White people (relative to a control). This politician was also perceived as less offensive than a politician concerned about anti-White bias. In Experiment 4, Black Christians perceived a politician concerned about anti-Christian bias as less offensive than one concerned about anti-White bias yet still unlikely to fight for Black people. Results suggest "anti-Christian bias" can provide a relatively palatable way to signal allegiance to White people.

四项预先登记的实验(N = 4,307)探讨了反基督教偏见的说法是否能在美国成年基督徒中谨慎地发出白人同盟的信号。在实验 1 和 2 中,阅读有关反基督教偏见的文章会让白人基督徒(而非黑人基督徒)感受到更多的反白人偏见。实验 3 和实验 4 表明,政客们可以利用基督徒和白人之间的联系来吹种族口哨。在实验 3 中,白人基督徒认为关注反基督教偏见的政治家更关心反白人偏见,更愿意为白人而战(相对于对照组)。与关注反白人偏见的政治家相比,这位政治家也被认为不那么具有攻击性。在实验 4 中,黑人基督徒认为关注反基督教偏见的政治家比关注反白人偏见的政治家更不具有攻击性,但仍然不太可能为黑人而战。实验结果表明,"反基督教偏见 "可以提供一种相对可接受的方式来向白人表示效忠。
{"title":"White by Another Name? Can Anti-Christian Bias Claims Serve as a Racial Dog Whistle?","authors":"Rosemary L Al-Kire, Chad A Miller, Michael H Pasek, Samuel L Perry, Clara L Wilkins","doi":"10.1177/09567976241236162","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241236162","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Four preregistered experiments (<i>N</i> = 4,307) explored whether anti-Christian bias claims can discreetly signal White allyship among Christian American adults. In Experiments 1 and 2, reading about anti-Christian bias led White, but not Black, Christians to perceive more anti-White bias. Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrate the connection between Christian and White can be leveraged by politicians in the form of a racial dog whistle. In Experiment 3, White Christians perceived a politician concerned about anti-Christian bias as caring more about anti-White bias and more willing to fight for White people (relative to a control). This politician was also perceived as less offensive than a politician concerned about anti-White bias. In Experiment 4, Black Christians perceived a politician concerned about anti-Christian bias as less offensive than one concerned about anti-White bias yet still unlikely to fight for Black people. Results suggest \"anti-Christian bias\" can provide a relatively palatable way to signal allegiance to White people.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"415-434"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140176133","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Efficacy of Accuracy Prompts Across Partisan Lines: An Adversarial Collaboration. 论跨党派准确性提示的功效:对抗性合作。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241232905
Cameron Martel, Steve Rathje, Cory J Clark, Gordon Pennycook, Jay J Van Bavel, David G Rand, Sander van der Linden

The spread of misinformation is a pressing societal challenge. Prior work shows that shifting attention to accuracy increases the quality of people's news-sharing decisions. However, researchers disagree on whether accuracy-prompt interventions work for U.S. Republicans/conservatives and whether partisanship moderates the effect. In this preregistered adversarial collaboration, we tested this question using a multiverse meta-analysis (k = 21; N = 27,828). In all 70 models, accuracy prompts improved sharing discernment among Republicans/conservatives. We observed significant partisan moderation for single-headline "evaluation" treatments (a critical test for one research team) such that the effect was stronger among Democrats than Republicans. However, this moderation was not consistently robust across different operationalizations of ideology/partisanship, exclusion criteria, or treatment type. Overall, we observed significant partisan moderation in 50% of specifications (all of which were considered critical for the other team). We discuss the conditions under which moderation is observed and offer interpretations.

错误信息的传播是一项紧迫的社会挑战。先前的研究表明,将注意力转移到准确性上可以提高人们分享新闻决策的质量。然而,对于准确性提示干预是否对美国共和党/保守党人有效,以及党派倾向是否会调节这种效果,研究人员存在分歧。在这次预先注册的对抗性合作中,我们使用多元宇宙荟萃分析(k = 21; N = 27,828)对这一问题进行了测试。在所有 70 个模型中,准确性提示提高了共和党人/保守党人的分享辨别力。我们观察到,单标题 "评价 "处理(一个研究小组的关键测试)具有明显的党派调节作用,民主党人的效果强于共和党人。然而,这种调节作用在不同的意识形态/党派、排除标准或治疗类型的操作中并不稳定。总体而言,我们在 50% 的规范中观察到了明显的党派调节作用(所有这些规范对另一个研究小组来说都是至关重要的)。我们讨论了观察到调节的条件,并提供了解释。
{"title":"On the Efficacy of Accuracy Prompts Across Partisan Lines: An Adversarial Collaboration.","authors":"Cameron Martel, Steve Rathje, Cory J Clark, Gordon Pennycook, Jay J Van Bavel, David G Rand, Sander van der Linden","doi":"10.1177/09567976241232905","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241232905","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The spread of misinformation is a pressing societal challenge. Prior work shows that shifting attention to accuracy increases the quality of people's news-sharing decisions. However, researchers disagree on whether accuracy-prompt interventions work for U.S. Republicans/conservatives and whether partisanship moderates the effect. In this preregistered adversarial collaboration, we tested this question using a multiverse meta-analysis (<i>k</i> = 21; <i>N</i> = 27,828). In all 70 models, accuracy prompts improved sharing discernment among Republicans/conservatives. We observed significant partisan moderation for single-headline \"evaluation\" treatments (a critical test for one research team) such that the effect was stronger among Democrats than Republicans. However, this moderation was not consistently robust across different operationalizations of ideology/partisanship, exclusion criteria, or treatment type. Overall, we observed significant partisan moderation in 50% of specifications (all of which were considered critical for the other team). We discuss the conditions under which moderation is observed and offer interpretations.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"435-450"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140176132","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When and Why People Conceal Infectious Disease. 人们何时及为何隐瞒传染病?
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-24 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231221990
Wilson N Merrell, Soyeon Choi, Joshua M Ackerman

People sick with infectious illnesses face negative social outcomes, like exclusion, and may take steps to conceal their illnesses from others. In 10 studies of past, current, and projected illness, we examined the prevalence and predictors of infection concealment in adult samples of U.S. university students, health-care employees, and online crowdsourced workers (total N = 4,110). About 75% reported concealing illness in interpersonal interactions, possibly placing others in harm's way. Concealment motives were largely social (e.g., wanting to attend events like parties) and achievement oriented (e.g., completing work objectives). Disease characteristics, including potential harm and illness immediacy, also influenced concealment decisions. People imagining harmful (vs. mild) infections concealed illness less frequently, whereas participants who were actually sick concealed frequently regardless of illness harm, suggesting state-specific biases underlying concealment decisions. Disease concealment appears to be a widely prevalent behavior by which concealers trade off risks to others in favor of their own goals, creating potentially important public-health consequences.

患有传染病的人面临着负面的社会影响,比如被排斥,他们可能会采取措施向他人隐瞒自己的病情。在 10 项关于过去、当前和预期疾病的研究中,我们调查了美国大学生、医疗保健员工和网络众包工作者(总人数 = 4,110)成人样本中隐瞒感染的流行率和预测因素。约 75% 的人表示在人际交往中隐瞒病情,这可能会使他人受到伤害。隐瞒的动机主要是社交(如想参加聚会等活动)和成就导向(如完成工作目标)。疾病特征,包括潜在危害和疾病的直接性,也会影响隐瞒决定。想象中的有害(相对于轻微)感染者隐瞒疾病的频率较低,而实际患病的参与者无论疾病是否有害都会频繁隐瞒,这表明隐瞒决定背后存在特定状态的偏差。隐瞒疾病似乎是一种广泛流行的行为,隐瞒者通过这种行为来权衡对他人的风险,以达到自己的目的,从而可能对公共健康造成重要影响。
{"title":"When and Why People Conceal Infectious Disease.","authors":"Wilson N Merrell, Soyeon Choi, Joshua M Ackerman","doi":"10.1177/09567976231221990","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231221990","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People sick with infectious illnesses face negative social outcomes, like exclusion, and may take steps to conceal their illnesses from others. In 10 studies of past, current, and projected illness, we examined the prevalence and predictors of infection concealment in adult samples of U.S. university students, health-care employees, and online crowdsourced workers (total <i>N</i> = 4,110). About 75% reported concealing illness in interpersonal interactions, possibly placing others in harm's way. Concealment motives were largely social (e.g., wanting to attend events like parties) and achievement oriented (e.g., completing work objectives). Disease characteristics, including potential harm and illness immediacy, also influenced concealment decisions. People imagining harmful (vs. mild) infections concealed illness less frequently, whereas participants who were actually sick concealed frequently regardless of illness harm, suggesting state-specific biases underlying concealment decisions. Disease concealment appears to be a widely prevalent behavior by which concealers trade off risks to others in favor of their own goals, creating potentially important public-health consequences.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"215-225"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139542966","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Devaluation by Omission: Limited Identity Options Elicit Anger and Increase Identification. 因疏忽而贬值:有限的身份选择激发愤怒,增加认同。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231223416
Sean Fath, Devon Proudfoot

In the present research, we explored social-identity threat caused by subtle acts of omission, specifically situations in which social-identity information is requested but one's identity is not among the options provided. We predicted that being unable to identify with one's group-that is, in the demographics section of a survey-may signal social-identity devaluation, eliciting negative affect (e.g., anger) and increasing the importance of the omitted identity to group members' sense of self. Six preregistered experiments (N = 2,964 adults) sampling members of two minority-identity groups (i.e., gender minorities and members of a minority political party) support these predictions. Our findings document the existence of a subtle but likely pervasive form of social-identity threat.

在本研究中,我们探讨了微妙的遗漏行为所造成的社会身份威胁,特别是在要求提供社会身份信息但个人身份不在提供的选项中的情况下。我们预测,无法认同自己的群体--即在调查的人口统计部分--可能是社会身份贬值的信号,从而引发负面情绪(如愤怒),并增加被遗漏身份对群体成员自我意识的重要性。六项预先登记的实验(N = 2,964 名成年人)对两个少数身份群体(即性别少数群体和少数政党成员)的成员进行了抽样调查,结果支持了上述预测。我们的研究结果证明了一种微妙但可能普遍存在的社会身份威胁的存在。
{"title":"Devaluation by Omission: Limited Identity Options Elicit Anger and Increase Identification.","authors":"Sean Fath, Devon Proudfoot","doi":"10.1177/09567976231223416","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231223416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the present research, we explored social-identity threat caused by subtle acts of omission, specifically situations in which social-identity information is requested but one's identity is not among the options provided. We predicted that being unable to identify with one's group-that is, in the demographics section of a survey-may signal social-identity devaluation, eliciting negative affect (e.g., anger) and increasing the importance of the omitted identity to group members' sense of self. Six preregistered experiments (<i>N</i> = 2,964 adults) sampling members of two minority-identity groups (i.e., gender minorities and members of a minority political party) support these predictions. Our findings document the existence of a subtle but likely pervasive form of social-identity threat.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"239-249"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139576333","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrigendum to "Indulgent Foods Can Paradoxically Promote Disciplined Dietary Choices". 对 "放纵的食物可以自相矛盾地促进有节制的饮食选择 "的更正。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231217508
{"title":"Corrigendum to \"Indulgent Foods Can Paradoxically Promote Disciplined Dietary Choices\".","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/09567976231217508","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231217508","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"312"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139703282","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Universal Cognitive Bias in Word Order: Evidence From Speakers Whose Language Goes Against It. 词序中的普遍认知偏差:来自语言与之相悖的说话者的证据
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231222836
Alexander Martin, David Adger, Klaus Abels, Patrick Kanampiu, Jennifer Culbertson

There is a long-standing debate in cognitive science surrounding the source of commonalities among languages of the world. Indeed, there are many potential explanations for such commonalities-accidents of history, common processes of language change, memory limitations, constraints on linguistic representations, and so on. Recent research has used psycholinguistic experiments to provide empirical evidence linking common linguistic patterns to specific features of human cognition, but these experiments tend to use English speakers, who in many cases have direct experience with the common patterns of interest. Here we highlight the importance of testing populations whose languages go against cross-linguistic trends. We investigate whether adult monolingual speakers of Kîîtharaka, which has an unusual way of ordering words, mirror the word-order preferences of English speakers. We find that they do, supporting the hypothesis that universal cognitive representations play a role in shaping word order.

长期以来,认知科学界一直在争论世界语言共性的来源。事实上,这种共性有许多潜在的解释--历史的偶然性、语言变化的共同过程、记忆的局限性、语言表征的限制等等。最近的研究利用心理语言学实验提供了将共同语言模式与人类认知的具体特征联系起来的经验证据,但这些实验往往使用英语使用者,他们在很多情况下对感兴趣的共同模式有直接经验。在这里,我们强调了测试那些语言与跨语言趋势背道而驰的人群的重要性。Kîîtharaka语的单语成人具有不同寻常的单词排序方式,我们研究了Kîîtharaka语的单语成人是否反映了英语使用者的单词排序偏好。我们发现他们反映了这一点,从而支持了普遍认知表征在形成词序方面发挥作用的假设。
{"title":"A Universal Cognitive Bias in Word Order: Evidence From Speakers Whose Language Goes Against It.","authors":"Alexander Martin, David Adger, Klaus Abels, Patrick Kanampiu, Jennifer Culbertson","doi":"10.1177/09567976231222836","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231222836","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is a long-standing debate in cognitive science surrounding the source of commonalities among languages of the world. Indeed, there are many potential explanations for such commonalities-accidents of history, common processes of language change, memory limitations, constraints on linguistic representations, and so on. Recent research has used psycholinguistic experiments to provide empirical evidence linking common linguistic patterns to specific features of human cognition, but these experiments tend to use English speakers, who in many cases have direct experience with the common patterns of interest. Here we highlight the importance of testing populations whose languages go against cross-linguistic trends. We investigate whether adult monolingual speakers of Kîîtharaka, which has an unusual way of ordering words, mirror the word-order preferences of English speakers. We find that they do, supporting the hypothesis that universal cognitive representations play a role in shaping word order.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"304-311"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139932631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1