Pub Date : 2024-06-17DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100318
Daniel Núñez , Javiera Rodríguez-Delgado , Ramón D. Castillo , José Yupanqui , Heidi Kloos
Introduction
It is known that cognitive deficits are a core feature of schizophrenia and that in the general population, prior beliefs significantly influence learning and reasoning processes. However, the interaction of prior beliefs with cognitive deficits and their impact on performance in schizophrenia patients is still poorly understood. This study investigates the role of beliefs and cognitive variables (CVs) like working memory, associative learning, and processing speed on learning processes in individuals with schizophrenia. We hypothesize that beliefs will influence the ability to learn correct predictions and that first-episode schizophrenia patients (FEP) will show impaired learning due to cognitive deficits.
Methods
We used a predictive-learning task to examine how FEP (n = 23) and matched controls (n = 23) adjusted their decisional criteria concerning physical properties during the learning process when predicting the sinking behavior of two transparent containers filled with aluminum discs when placed in water.
Results
On accuracy, initial differences by group, trial type, and interaction effects of these variables disappeared when CVs were controlled. The differences by conditions, associated with differential beliefs about why the objects sink slower or faster, were seen in patients and controls, despite controlling the CVs' effect.
Conclusions
Differences between groups were mainly explained by CVs, proving that they play an important role than what is assumed in this type of task. However, beliefs about physical events were not affected by CVs, and beliefs affect in the same way the decisional criteria of the control or FEP patients' groups.
{"title":"Effect of prior beliefs and cognitive deficits on learning in first-episode schizophrenia patients","authors":"Daniel Núñez , Javiera Rodríguez-Delgado , Ramón D. Castillo , José Yupanqui , Heidi Kloos","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100318","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100318","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>It is known that cognitive deficits are a core feature of schizophrenia and that in the general population, prior beliefs significantly influence learning and reasoning processes. However, the interaction of prior beliefs with cognitive deficits and their impact on performance in schizophrenia patients is still poorly understood. This study investigates the role of beliefs and cognitive variables (CVs) like working memory, associative learning, and processing speed on learning processes in individuals with schizophrenia. We hypothesize that beliefs will influence the ability to learn correct predictions and that first-episode schizophrenia patients (FEP) will show impaired learning due to cognitive deficits.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>We used a predictive-learning task to examine how FEP (<em>n</em> = 23) and matched controls (n = 23) adjusted their decisional criteria concerning physical properties during the learning process when predicting the sinking behavior of two transparent containers filled with aluminum discs when placed in water.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>On accuracy, initial differences by group, trial type, and interaction effects of these variables disappeared when CVs were controlled. The differences by conditions, associated with differential beliefs about why the objects sink slower or faster, were seen in patients and controls, despite controlling the CVs' effect.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Differences between groups were mainly explained by CVs, proving that they play an important role than what is assumed in this type of task. However, beliefs about physical events were not affected by CVs, and beliefs affect in the same way the decisional criteria of the control or FEP patients' groups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"38 ","pages":"Article 100318"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000192/pdfft?md5=724aa1494226fad4a9ea8aa278248c96&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000192-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141424426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-12DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100317
Clara Martínez-Cao , Ainoa García-Fernández , Leticia González-Blanco , Pilar A. Sáiz , Julio Bobes , María Paz García-Portilla
Background
Cognitive impairment is a widespread feature of schizophrenia, affecting nearly 80 % of patients. Prior research has linked the anticholinergic burden of psychiatric medications to these cognitive deficits. However, the impact of the anticholinergic burden from medications for physical morbidity remains underexplored. This study aimed to evaluate the anticholinergic burden of psychiatric and physical medications in patients with schizophrenia and assess its impact on cognitive function.
Methods
A total of 178 patients with schizophrenia were recruited. The assessments included an ad hoc questionnaire for collecting demographic and clinical data. Anticholinergic burden was evaluated using the cumulative Drug Burden Index (cDBI) for each participant, and cognitive function was assessed using MATRICS. Psychopathology was measured using the PANSS, CDSS, CAINS, and the CGI-S. Statistical analysis included Student's t-tests, ANOVA, Pearson correlations, and multiple linear regressions.
Results
The average cDBI was 1.3 (SD = 0.9). The model developed explained 40.80 % of the variance. The variable with the greatest weight was the cDBI (B = −11.148, p = 0.010). Negative-expression (B = -2.740, p = 0.011) and negative-experiential (B = −1.175, p = 0.030) symptoms were also associated with lower global cognitive score. However, more years of education (B = 5.140, p < 0.001) and cigarettes per day (B = 1.331, p < 0.001) predicted a better global cognitive score.
Conclusion
This study identified specific predictors of global cognition in schizophrenia, with anticholinergic burden emerging as the strongest factor. Our findings underscore the importance of considering the anticholinergic burden of treatments, in addition to negative symptoms, when designing interventions to optimize or maintain cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia.
{"title":"Anticholinergic load: A commonly neglected and preventable risk to cognition during schizophrenia treatment?","authors":"Clara Martínez-Cao , Ainoa García-Fernández , Leticia González-Blanco , Pilar A. Sáiz , Julio Bobes , María Paz García-Portilla","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100317","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100317","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Cognitive impairment is a widespread feature of schizophrenia, affecting nearly 80 % of patients. Prior research has linked the anticholinergic burden of psychiatric medications to these cognitive deficits. However, the impact of the anticholinergic burden from medications for physical morbidity remains underexplored. This study aimed to evaluate the anticholinergic burden of psychiatric and physical medications in patients with schizophrenia and assess its impact on cognitive function.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>A total of 178 patients with schizophrenia were recruited. The assessments included an <em>ad hoc</em> questionnaire for collecting demographic and clinical data. Anticholinergic burden was evaluated using the cumulative Drug Burden Index (cDBI) for each participant, and cognitive function was assessed using MATRICS. Psychopathology was measured using the PANSS, CDSS, CAINS, and the CGI-S. Statistical analysis included Student's <em>t</em>-tests, ANOVA, Pearson correlations, and multiple linear regressions.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The average cDBI was 1.3 (SD = 0.9). The model developed explained 40.80 % of the variance. The variable with the greatest weight was the cDBI (B = −11.148, <em>p</em> = 0.010). Negative-expression (B = -2.740, <em>p</em> = 0.011) and negative-experiential (B = −1.175, <em>p</em> = 0.030) symptoms were also associated with lower global cognitive score. However, more years of education (B = 5.140, <em>p</em> < 0.001) and cigarettes per day (B = 1.331, p < 0.001) predicted a better global cognitive score.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>This study identified specific predictors of global cognition in schizophrenia, with anticholinergic burden emerging as the strongest factor. Our findings underscore the importance of considering the anticholinergic burden of treatments, in addition to negative symptoms, when designing interventions to optimize or maintain cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100317"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000180/pdfft?md5=5dfbd4c67ab2fb3070dd929b5789a85a&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000180-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140910069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-10DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100315
Christopher Dawes , Samuel Joy McGreal , Shivika Marwaha , Jose Prados , Antoine Reheis , Alin Dumitrescu , John L. Waddington , Paula M. Moran , Colm O'Tuathaigh
Aberrant attentional salience has been implicated in the cannabis-psychosis association. Here, history and frequency of cannabis use were examined against changes in overshadowing (OS), a cue competition paradigm that involves salience processing. Additionally, we examined the association between OS and alternative measures of aberrant salience, as well as schizotypy, in a non-clinical adult sample.
280 participants completed an online geometry learning-based OS task, while a subset (N = 149) also completed the Salience Attribution Task (SAT) measure of aberrant salience. All completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ), Aberrant Salience Inventory (ASI), and the modified Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQmv). Differences across OS and SAT performance stages and between cannabis use groups were assessed using mixed ANOVAs. Multiple regression and correlational analyses assessed the relationships between OS and SAT task metrics and SPQ and ASI subscale scores.
Current cannabis users had significantly lower OS scores during the testing phase relative to those who do not use cannabis, at medium effect sizes. Schizotypy or ASI scores did not mediate this relationship. In the SAT, current cannabis users presented significantly higher implicit aberrant salience relative to non-users. Scores in the first training phase of the OS task significantly predicted higher explicit aberrant and adaptive salience scores in the SAT.
These data indicate an association between regular cannabis use and abnormalities in cue competition effects in a healthy adult sample. Comparisons of OS and SAT cast new light on putative overlapping mechanisms underlying performance across different measures of salience.
注意力显著性异常与大麻-精神病的关联有一定联系。在此,我们针对阴影(OS)的变化研究了使用大麻的历史和频率,这是一种涉及显著性处理的线索竞争范式。此外,我们还在一个非临床成人样本中研究了OS与其他异常显著性测量方法以及精神分裂症之间的关联。280名参与者完成了基于几何学习的在线OS任务,其中一部分(N = 149)还完成了异常显著性测量方法 "显著性归因任务"(SAT)。所有参与者都完成了分裂型人格问卷(SPQ)、异常显著性量表(ASI)和修改后的大麻体验问卷(CEQmv)。使用混合方差分析评估了 OS 和 SAT 成绩阶段之间以及使用大麻组之间的差异。多元回归和相关分析评估了OS和SAT任务指标与SPQ和ASI分量表得分之间的关系。与不吸食大麻的人相比,目前吸食大麻的人在测试阶段的OS得分明显较低,影响程度为中等。精神分裂症或 ASI 分数并不影响这种关系。在 SAT 中,与不吸食大麻者相比,目前吸食大麻者的内隐异常显著性明显更高。这些数据表明,在健康的成人样本中,经常吸食大麻与线索竞争效应异常之间存在关联。对OS和SAT进行比较后,我们对不同显著性测量中可能存在的重叠机制有了新的认识。
{"title":"Overshadowing and salience attribution in relation to cannabis use","authors":"Christopher Dawes , Samuel Joy McGreal , Shivika Marwaha , Jose Prados , Antoine Reheis , Alin Dumitrescu , John L. Waddington , Paula M. Moran , Colm O'Tuathaigh","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100315","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Aberrant attentional salience has been implicated in the cannabis-psychosis association. Here, history and frequency of cannabis use were examined against changes in overshadowing (OS), a cue competition paradigm that involves salience processing. Additionally, we examined the association between OS and alternative measures of aberrant salience, as well as schizotypy, in a non-clinical adult sample.</p><p>280 participants completed an online geometry learning-based OS task, while a subset (<em>N</em> = 149) also completed the Salience Attribution Task (SAT) measure of aberrant salience. All completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ), Aberrant Salience Inventory (ASI), and the modified Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQmv). Differences across OS and SAT performance stages and between cannabis use groups were assessed using mixed ANOVAs. Multiple regression and correlational analyses assessed the relationships between OS and SAT task metrics and SPQ and ASI subscale scores.</p><p>Current cannabis users had significantly lower OS scores during the testing phase relative to those who do not use cannabis, at medium effect sizes. Schizotypy or ASI scores did not mediate this relationship. In the SAT, current cannabis users presented significantly higher implicit aberrant salience relative to non-users. Scores in the first training phase of the OS task significantly predicted higher explicit aberrant and adaptive salience scores in the SAT.</p><p>These data indicate an association between regular cannabis use and abnormalities in cue competition effects in a healthy adult sample. Comparisons of OS and SAT cast new light on putative overlapping mechanisms underlying performance across different measures of salience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100315"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000167/pdfft?md5=6fba5df38d9195c49dd128eb914dfdc4&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000167-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140905701","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-09DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100316
J.S. Toimela , A.H. Halt , M. Kerkelä , O. Kampman , J. Suvisaari , T. Kieseppä , M. Lähteenvuo , J. Tiihonen , A. Ahola-Olli , J. Veijola , M. Holm , The SUPER researchers listed in the Acknowledgements
Background
Both overweight and cognitive deficits are common among people with schizophrenia (SZ) and schizoaffective disorder. The results in earlier studies have been inconsistent on whether overweight is associated with cognitive deficits in psychotic disorders.
Aims
Our aim in this study was to detect possible associations between obesity and cognitive deficits among study participants with SZ and schizoaffective disorder.
Methods
The study sample included 5382 participants with a clinical diagnosis of SZ or schizoaffective disorder selected from the Finnish SUPER study. Obesity was measured both with body-mass index and waist circumference. The cognitive performance was evaluated with two tests from the Cambridge automated neuropsychological test battery: Reaction time was evaluated with the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Visual memory was evaluated with the paired associative learning test. The final analysis included a total sample of 4498 participants applicable for the analysis of the reaction time and 3967 participants for the analysis of the visual memory.
Results
Obesity measured with body-mass index was associated with better performance in reaction time task among both female and male participants. Among male participants, overweight was associated with better performance in the visual memory test. The waist circumference was not associated with cognitive measures.
Conclusions
The results suggest that obesity in people with SZ or schizoaffective disorder might not be associated with cognitive deficits but instead with better cognitive performance. The results were opposite from earlier literature on the general population. More research is required to better understand whether the results might be partly caused by the differences in the etiology of obesity between the general population and people with SZ.
{"title":"Association of obesity to reaction time and visual memory in schizophrenia","authors":"J.S. Toimela , A.H. Halt , M. Kerkelä , O. Kampman , J. Suvisaari , T. Kieseppä , M. Lähteenvuo , J. Tiihonen , A. Ahola-Olli , J. Veijola , M. Holm , The SUPER researchers listed in the Acknowledgements","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100316","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Both overweight and cognitive deficits are common among people with schizophrenia (SZ) and schizoaffective disorder. The results in earlier studies have been inconsistent on whether overweight is associated with cognitive deficits in psychotic disorders.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>Our aim in this study was to detect possible associations between obesity and cognitive deficits among study participants with SZ and schizoaffective disorder.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The study sample included 5382 participants with a clinical diagnosis of SZ or schizoaffective disorder selected from the Finnish SUPER study. Obesity was measured both with body-mass index and waist circumference. The cognitive performance was evaluated with two tests from the Cambridge automated neuropsychological test battery: Reaction time was evaluated with the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Visual memory was evaluated with the paired associative learning test. The final analysis included a total sample of 4498 participants applicable for the analysis of the reaction time and 3967 participants for the analysis of the visual memory.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Obesity measured with body-mass index was associated with better performance in reaction time task among both female and male participants. Among male participants, overweight was associated with better performance in the visual memory test. The waist circumference was not associated with cognitive measures.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The results suggest that obesity in people with SZ or schizoaffective disorder might not be associated with cognitive deficits but instead with better cognitive performance. The results were opposite from earlier literature on the general population. More research is required to better understand whether the results might be partly caused by the differences in the etiology of obesity between the general population and people with SZ.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100316"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000179/pdfft?md5=da13f3602a529e35e900a92853f33bfd&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000179-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140894992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-09DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100314
Xiaoyu Dong , Bridget Shovestul , Abhishek Saxena , Emily Dudek , Stephanie Reda , J. Steven Lamberti , David Dodell-Feder
Schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) are associated with pervasive cognitive impairments, including deficits in decision-making under risk. However, there is inconclusive evidence regarding specific mechanisms underlying altered decision-making patterns. In this study, participants (33 SSD and 28 non-SSD) completed the Columbia Card Task, an explicit risk-taking task, to better understand risk preference and adjustment in dynamic decision-making. We found that while there is no group difference in overall risk-taking, risk preference, or optimal decision-making, risk adjustment to contextual factors (e.g., loss probability) is blunted in SSD. We also found associations between risk-taking/suboptimal decision-making and disorganized symptoms, excited symptoms, and role functioning, but no associations between decision-making and working memory. These results suggest that during a complex, dynamic risk-taking task, individuals with SSD exhibit less adaption to changing information about risk, which may reflect risk imperception.
{"title":"Decision-making under risk and its correlates in schizophrenia","authors":"Xiaoyu Dong , Bridget Shovestul , Abhishek Saxena , Emily Dudek , Stephanie Reda , J. Steven Lamberti , David Dodell-Feder","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100314","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) are associated with pervasive cognitive impairments, including deficits in decision-making under risk. However, there is inconclusive evidence regarding specific mechanisms underlying altered decision-making patterns. In this study, participants (33 SSD and 28 non-SSD) completed the Columbia Card Task, an explicit risk-taking task, to better understand risk preference and adjustment in dynamic decision-making. We found that while there is no group difference in overall risk-taking, risk preference, or optimal decision-making, risk adjustment to contextual factors (e.g., loss probability) is blunted in SSD. We also found associations between risk-taking/suboptimal decision-making and disorganized symptoms, excited symptoms, and role functioning, but no associations between decision-making and working memory. These results suggest that during a complex, dynamic risk-taking task, individuals with SSD exhibit less adaption to changing information about risk, which may reflect risk imperception.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100314"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000155/pdfft?md5=43747da0409467127db61b6f8313dc18&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000155-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140894990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-24DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100312
Seung Ho Lee , Malik Ekhdoura , Sihyun Baek , Naista Zhand
{"title":"Social cognition among clinical subtypes of schizophrenia","authors":"Seung Ho Lee , Malik Ekhdoura , Sihyun Baek , Naista Zhand","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100312","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100312","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100312"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000131/pdfft?md5=e5ff67d8eb86660965ac92a86d42428e&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000131-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140640970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-20DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100313
Joanna M. Fiszdon , Kaicheng Wang , Karen Lê , Lori Parente , Jimmy Choi
While cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) and compensatory strategy training both have large literature bases supporting their efficacy on both proximal and distal outcomes, the research base on stand-alone cognitive training (CT) is smaller and less consistent, with little information about factors associated with better outcomes. In this study, we examined the efficacy of CT on training task, cognitive, symptom, and functional ability measures as well as the impact of motivational interviewing (MI), motivation level, and session attendance on treatment outcomes. Adults with psychotic spectrum disorders (n = 114) were randomized to MI or a sham control interview (CI), followed by 4 months of computerized CT. In whole sample analyses, participants improved on training tasks, cognitive performance, and psychiatric symptoms, but self-reported cognition, self-reported depression, and functional ability did not change. Compared to CI, MI was associated with greater reductions in self-reported depressive symptoms. Motivation level and session attendance did not significantly influence outcomes. Findings support the efficacy of CT on several key outcomes, and its simplicity may be advantageous in uptake in community clinics with limited staffing. The lack of functional gains underscores the need to incorporate treatment ingredients that promote generalization and real-world implementation of learned skills. We also speculate that engagement during course of training may be a better predictor of training success than baseline task-specific motivation.
虽然认知矫正治疗(CRT)和补偿策略训练都有大量文献支持其对近端和远端结果的疗效,但关于独立认知训练(CT)的研究基础较小且不太一致,几乎没有关于与更好结果相关的因素的信息。在这项研究中,我们考察了 CT 在训练任务、认知、症状和功能能力测量方面的疗效,以及动机访谈(MI)、动机水平和疗程出席率对治疗结果的影响。患有精神谱系障碍的成年人(n = 114)被随机分配到MI或假对照访谈(CI),然后接受为期4个月的计算机化CT治疗。在全样本分析中,参与者的训练任务、认知表现和精神症状都有所改善,但自我认知、自我抑郁和功能能力没有变化。与 CI 相比,多元智能对自述抑郁症状的缓解作用更大。动机水平和疗程出席率对疗效没有显著影响。研究结果支持 CT 在几个关键结果上的疗效,其简便性可能有利于在人员有限的社区诊所中推广。缺乏功能性收益突出表明,有必要在治疗中加入促进所学技能普遍化和实际应用的成分。我们还推测,训练过程中的参与可能比特定任务的基线动机更能预测训练的成功与否。
{"title":"Efficacy of cognitive training on cognition, symptoms and functioning: Impact of motivation and attendance","authors":"Joanna M. Fiszdon , Kaicheng Wang , Karen Lê , Lori Parente , Jimmy Choi","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100313","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>While cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) and compensatory strategy training both have large literature bases supporting their efficacy on both proximal and distal outcomes, the research base on stand-alone cognitive training (CT) is smaller and less consistent, with little information about factors associated with better outcomes. In this study, we examined the efficacy of CT on training task, cognitive, symptom, and functional ability measures as well as the impact of motivational interviewing (MI), motivation level, and session attendance on treatment outcomes. Adults with psychotic spectrum disorders (<em>n</em> = 114) were randomized to MI or a sham control interview (CI), followed by 4 months of computerized CT. In whole sample analyses, participants improved on training tasks, cognitive performance, and psychiatric symptoms, but self-reported cognition, self-reported depression, and functional ability did not change. Compared to CI, MI was associated with greater reductions in self-reported depressive symptoms. Motivation level and session attendance did not significantly influence outcomes. Findings support the efficacy of CT on several key outcomes, and its simplicity may be advantageous in uptake in community clinics with limited staffing. The lack of functional gains underscores the need to incorporate treatment ingredients that promote generalization and real-world implementation of learned skills. We also speculate that engagement during course of training may be a better predictor of training success than baseline task-specific motivation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100313"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000143/pdfft?md5=adca5fe9fc957e603b91b1e59533cb8a&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000143-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140620718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Acute and transient psychotic disorder (ATPD) is characterized by acute onset of psychotic symptoms and early recovery. Contrastingly, schizophrenia (SZ) is a chronic mental disorder characterized by impaired functioning including a deficit in cognition. In SZ, the cognitive deficit is among the core symptoms, but in ATPDs, the existing evidence brings mixed results. Our primary aim was to compare three core cognitive domains (executive functioning/abstraction, speed of processing and working memory) of patients diagnosed with ATPD and SZ over a 12-month period. Moreover, we explored how these diagnostic subgroups differed in their clinical characteristics. We recruited 39 patients with a diagnosis of SZ and 31 with ATPD with schizophrenic symptoms. All patients completed clinical and neuropsychological assessments. At baseline, we used a one-way ANCOVA model with a group as the between-subjects factor. Mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVAs with time as the within-subjects factor and group as the between-subjects factor were run to test the overtime differences. At baseline, we did not find any differences in cognition - with sex, education and age as covariates - between ATPDs and SZ. After one year, all patients showed an improvement in all three domains, however, there were no significant overtime changes between ATPDs and SZ. Regarding clinical profiles, ATPDs demonstrated less severe psychopathology and better functioning compared to SZ both at baseline and after 12 months. The medication dosage differed at retest, but not at baseline between the groups. Our findings suggest clinical differences and a similar trajectory of cognitive performance between these diagnostic subgroups.
{"title":"Longitudinal course of core cognitive domains in first-episode acute and transient psychotic disorders compared with schizophrenia","authors":"Karolína Knížková , Barbora Keřková , Monika Večeřová , Petra Šustová , Juraj Jonáš , Aneta Siroňová , Aleš Hrubý , Mabel Rodriguez","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100311","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100311","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Acute and transient psychotic disorder (ATPD) is characterized by acute onset of psychotic symptoms and early recovery. Contrastingly, schizophrenia (SZ) is a chronic mental disorder characterized by impaired functioning including a deficit in cognition. In SZ, the cognitive deficit is among the core symptoms, but in ATPDs, the existing evidence brings mixed results. Our primary aim was to compare three core cognitive domains (executive functioning/abstraction, speed of processing and working memory) of patients diagnosed with ATPD and SZ over a 12-month period. Moreover, we explored how these diagnostic subgroups differed in their clinical characteristics. We recruited 39 patients with a diagnosis of SZ and 31 with ATPD with schizophrenic symptoms. All patients completed clinical and neuropsychological assessments. At baseline, we used a one-way ANCOVA model with a group as the between-subjects factor. Mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVAs with time as the within-subjects factor and group as the between-subjects factor were run to test the overtime differences. At baseline, we did not find any differences in cognition - with sex, education and age as covariates - between ATPDs and SZ. After one year, all patients showed an improvement in all three domains, however, there were no significant overtime changes between ATPDs and SZ. Regarding clinical profiles, ATPDs demonstrated less severe psychopathology and better functioning compared to SZ both at baseline and after 12 months. The medication dosage differed at retest, but not at baseline between the groups. Our findings suggest clinical differences and a similar trajectory of cognitive performance between these diagnostic subgroups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100311"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S221500132400012X/pdfft?md5=9616e6eec27d43ffad73560284e01d8b&pid=1-s2.0-S221500132400012X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140347087","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-03-28DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100310
George Gifford , Alexis E. Cullen , Sandra Vieira , Anja Searle , Robert A. McCutcheon , Gemma Modinos , William S. Stone , Emily Hird , Jennifer Barnett , Hendrika H. van Hell , Ana Catalan , Edward Millgate , Nick Taptiklis , Francesca Cormack , Margot E. Slot , Paola Dazzan , Arija Maat , Lieuwe de Haan , Benedicto Crespo Facorro , Birte Glenthøj , Philip McGuire
Despite the functional impact of cognitive deficit in people with psychosis, objective cognitive assessment is not typically part of routine clinical care. This is partly due to the length of traditional assessments and the need for a highly trained administrator. Brief, automated computerised assessments could help to address this issue. We present data from an evaluation of PsyCog, a computerised, non-verbal, mini battery of cognitive tests. Healthy Control (HC) (N = 135), Clinical High Risk (CHR) (N = 233), and First Episode Psychosis (FEP) (N = 301) participants from a multi-centre prospective study were assessed at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. PsyCog was used to assess cognitive performance at baseline and at up to two follow-up timepoints. Mean total testing time was 35.95 min (SD = 2.87). Relative to HCs, effect sizes of performance impairments were medium to large in FEP patients (composite score G = 1.21, subtest range = 0.52–0.88) and small to medium in CHR patients (composite score G = 0.59, subtest range = 0.18–0.49). Site effects were minimal, and test-retest reliability of the PsyCog composite was good (ICC = 0.82–0.89), though some practice effects and differences in data completion between groups were found. The present implementation of PsyCog shows it to be a useful tool for assessing cognitive function in people with psychosis. Computerised cognitive assessments have the potential to facilitate the evaluation of cognition in psychosis in both research and in clinical care, though caution should still be taken in terms of implementation and study design.
{"title":"PsyCog: A computerised mini battery for assessing cognition in psychosis","authors":"George Gifford , Alexis E. Cullen , Sandra Vieira , Anja Searle , Robert A. McCutcheon , Gemma Modinos , William S. Stone , Emily Hird , Jennifer Barnett , Hendrika H. van Hell , Ana Catalan , Edward Millgate , Nick Taptiklis , Francesca Cormack , Margot E. Slot , Paola Dazzan , Arija Maat , Lieuwe de Haan , Benedicto Crespo Facorro , Birte Glenthøj , Philip McGuire","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100310","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100310","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite the functional impact of cognitive deficit in people with psychosis, objective cognitive assessment is not typically part of routine clinical care. This is partly due to the length of traditional assessments and the need for a highly trained administrator. Brief, automated computerised assessments could help to address this issue. We present data from an evaluation of PsyCog, a computerised, non-verbal, mini battery of cognitive tests. Healthy Control (HC) (<em>N</em> = 135), Clinical High Risk (CHR) (<em>N</em> = 233), and First Episode Psychosis (FEP) (<em>N</em> = 301) participants from a multi-centre prospective study were assessed at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. PsyCog was used to assess cognitive performance at baseline and at up to two follow-up timepoints. Mean total testing time was 35.95 min (SD = 2.87). Relative to HCs, effect sizes of performance impairments were medium to large in FEP patients (composite score G = 1.21, subtest range = 0.52–0.88) and small to medium in CHR patients (composite score G = 0.59, subtest range = 0.18–0.49). Site effects were minimal, and test-retest reliability of the PsyCog composite was good (ICC = 0.82–0.89), though some practice effects and differences in data completion between groups were found. The present implementation of PsyCog shows it to be a useful tool for assessing cognitive function in people with psychosis. Computerised cognitive assessments have the potential to facilitate the evaluation of cognition in psychosis in both research and in clinical care, though caution should still be taken in terms of implementation and study design.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100310"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000118/pdfft?md5=40d27cb9dafcbf41a42a125575e4427c&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000118-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140309715","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-03-19DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100309
Erlend Lane , Devayani Joshi , Synthia Guimond , Raeanne Moore , Anthony O. Ahmed , Olusola Ajilore , John Torous
Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are associated with cognitive deficits that contribute significantly to disability. However, traditional in-lab cognitive assessments are time-consuming and not optimized for remote administration. Recent advancements in smartphone technology enable momentary cognitive assessments in a real-world context. This brief report reviews recent research in momentary cognitive assessments in individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder through reviewing mobile platforms and cognitive assessments studied. A total of 14 experimental articles were reviewed, focusing on cognitive domains including visual working memory, processing speed, executive function, verbal fluency, verbal memory, social cognition, and typing patterns. The review highlights the feasibility of remote cognitive assessment with smartphones, and provides a layout of domains studied in this context, but illustrates a low volume of current research, the need for additional studies, and the potential for innovations like digital phenotyping.
{"title":"Exploring current smartphone-based cognitive assessments in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder","authors":"Erlend Lane , Devayani Joshi , Synthia Guimond , Raeanne Moore , Anthony O. Ahmed , Olusola Ajilore , John Torous","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100309","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2024.100309","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are associated with cognitive deficits that contribute significantly to disability. However, traditional in-lab cognitive assessments are time-consuming and not optimized for remote administration. Recent advancements in smartphone technology enable momentary cognitive assessments in a real-world context. This brief report reviews recent research in momentary cognitive assessments in individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder through reviewing mobile platforms and cognitive assessments studied. A total of 14 experimental articles were reviewed, focusing on cognitive domains including visual working memory, processing speed, executive function, verbal fluency, verbal memory, social cognition, and typing patterns. The review highlights the feasibility of remote cognitive assessment with smartphones, and provides a layout of domains studied in this context, but illustrates a low volume of current research, the need for additional studies, and the potential for innovations like digital phenotyping.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":"37 ","pages":"Article 100309"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000106/pdfft?md5=6951c970913c9a16b2d361fd66ba123d&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000106-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140162693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}