首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Creative Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Creativity Research Overlooks the Study of Resilience among Young Children: A Bibliometric Network Review 创造力研究忽视了对幼儿复原力的研究:文献计量学网络综述
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-01-08 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.632
Nicolas B. Verger, Julie Roberts, Jane Guiller, Kareena McAloney-Kocaman

Creativity researchers are increasingly interested in understanding when, how, and for whom creativity can be beneficial. Previous reviews have demonstrated that creativity research largely ignores the study of its impact on factors that promote health, and well-being among populations of adults. It is unclear, in fact, whether this gap in research also extends to creativity research among young children. This paper addresses this issue. Early childhood is a crucial stage for the cognitive development of young children who remain highly sensitive to stress, and adversity. It is therefore essential to identify and promote factors that are beneficial to early childhood resilience, thereby contributing to documenting more of the effects of creative activities on positive outcomes. This paper presents a review with a bibliometric analysis of 1000 randomly selected articles from the Web of Science, without bias towards any specific peer-reviewed journal. The analysis of 454 included articles shows that approximately 80% of the included studies focus on creativity as an outcome (replicating previous findings with a larger sample), with only 3.78% investigating creative activities as predictors among young children. In this small percentage, most of the studies addressed creative activities in young children related to resilience outcomes.

创造力研究人员对了解创造力何时、如何以及对谁有益越来越感兴趣。之前的评论表明,创造力研究在很大程度上忽视了对促进成人健康和幸福的因素的影响的研究。事实上,目前还不清楚这一研究空白是否也延伸到了幼儿的创造力研究中。本文将探讨这一问题。幼儿期是幼儿认知发展的关键阶段,他们对压力和逆境仍然高度敏感。因此,有必要确定并促进有利于幼儿复原力的因素,从而有助于记录更多创造性活动对积极结果的影响。本文对从 "科学网"(Web of Science)上随机选取的 1000 篇文章进行了文献计量分析,不偏重于任何特定的同行评审期刊。对所收录的 454 篇文章进行的分析表明,所收录的研究中约有 80% 将创造力作为研究结果的重点(这与之前的研究结果相吻合,并采用了更大的样本),只有 3.78% 的研究将创造性活动作为幼儿的预测因素进行了调查。在这一小部分研究中,大多数研究都探讨了幼儿创造性活动与复原力结果的关系。
{"title":"Creativity Research Overlooks the Study of Resilience among Young Children: A Bibliometric Network Review","authors":"Nicolas B. Verger,&nbsp;Julie Roberts,&nbsp;Jane Guiller,&nbsp;Kareena McAloney-Kocaman","doi":"10.1002/jocb.632","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.632","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Creativity researchers are increasingly interested in understanding when, how, and for whom creativity can be beneficial. Previous reviews have demonstrated that creativity research largely ignores the study of its impact on factors that promote health, and well-being among populations of adults. It is unclear, in fact, whether this gap in research also extends to creativity research among young children. This paper addresses this issue. Early childhood is a crucial stage for the cognitive development of young children who remain highly sensitive to stress, and adversity. It is therefore essential to identify and promote factors that are beneficial to early childhood resilience, thereby contributing to documenting more of the effects of creative activities on positive outcomes. This paper presents a review with a bibliometric analysis of 1000 randomly selected articles from the Web of Science, without bias towards any specific peer-reviewed journal. The analysis of 454 included articles shows that approximately 80% of the included studies focus on creativity as an outcome (replicating previous findings with a larger sample), with only 3.78% investigating creative activities as predictors among young children. In this small percentage, most of the studies addressed creative activities in young children related to resilience outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"96-113"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-01-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.632","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139409542","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Never Ending Stories? Hebrew Writers' Creative Journey in the Second Half of Life 永无止境的故事?希伯来作家后半生的创作历程
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.631
Shlomit Aharoni Lir, Liat Ayalon

This qualitative study explored the relationship between creativity, time, age, and the literary world among 16 award-winning Israeli writers in the second half of their lives. Based on data collected through in-depth interviews with the participants, the findings indicate that the writers' creativity in the second half of life was linked to Preservation, Growth, and Decline, leading to four interrelated dimensions: (a) preserving youth through creativity, (b) enhancing creativity over time, (c) navigating creativity, acclaim, and ageism, and (d) managing creativity amidst a changing world. The study found that the writers' creativity was enhanced over time, as they gained experience and developed their skills. However, some writers experienced a decline in stamina or their ability to write long novels. The study also found that the writers' experiences of acceptance from the literary world and the audience changed over time. Some writers continued to receive acclaim and attention, while others experienced ageism and a decline in the audience's interest in their work.

这项定性研究探讨了 16 位以色列获奖作家后半生的创造力、时间、年龄和文学世界之间的关系。根据对参与者进行深入访谈收集到的数据,研究结果表明,作家们后半生的创造力与 "保存"、"成长 "和 "衰退 "相关联,从而形成了四个相互关联的维度:(a)通过创造力保存青春,(b)随着时间的推移增强创造力,(c)在创造力、赞誉和年龄歧视之间游刃有余,以及(d)在不断变化的世界中驾驭创造力。研究发现,随着时间的推移,作家们的创造力得到了提升,因为他们积累了经验,发展了技能。然而,一些作家的耐力或撰写长篇小说的能力有所下降。研究还发现,随着时间的推移,作家们被文学界和读者接受的经历也发生了变化。一些作家继续受到好评和关注,而另一些作家则遭遇了年龄歧视,观众对其作品的兴趣也有所下降。
{"title":"Never Ending Stories? Hebrew Writers' Creative Journey in the Second Half of Life","authors":"Shlomit Aharoni Lir,&nbsp;Liat Ayalon","doi":"10.1002/jocb.631","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.631","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This qualitative study explored the relationship between creativity, time, age, and the literary world among 16 award-winning Israeli writers in the second half of their lives. Based on data collected through in-depth interviews with the participants, the findings indicate that the writers' creativity in the second half of life was linked to <i>Preservation</i>, <i>Growth</i>, and <i>Decline</i>, leading to four interrelated dimensions: (a) preserving youth through creativity, (b) enhancing creativity over time, (c) navigating creativity, acclaim, and ageism, and (d) managing creativity amidst a changing world. The study found that the writers' creativity was enhanced over time, as they gained experience and developed their skills. However, some writers experienced a decline in stamina or their ability to write long novels. The study also found that the writers' experiences of acceptance from the literary world and the audience changed over time. Some writers continued to receive acclaim and attention, while others experienced ageism and a decline in the audience's interest in their work.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"82-95"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-01-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.631","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139373996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Loose and Tight: Creative Formation but Rigid Use of Nominal Compounds in Conspiracist Texts 松与紧:阴谋论文本中名称复合词的创造性形成与僵化使用
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-01-04 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.633
Alessandro Miani, Lonneke van der Plas, Adrian Bangerter

Conspiracy theories (CTs) are spectacular narratives, widely spread, that pose societal threats. We test whether CTs might be linguistically creative products, which would facilitate their transmission and thereby account for their widespread popularity. We analyzed nominal compounds (e.g., mind control, carbon dioxide; N = 1,713,568) from a large corpus of conspiracist and mainstream texts matched by topic. In conspiracist texts, compounds showed greater originality, divergence, and sophistication, but they were used with lower frequency and were more often repeated in different contexts. This pattern suggests a creative aspect in the generation of compounds, coupled with rigidity in their use. We interpret these findings as an effect of loosely defined conceptual boundaries among conspiracist writers, in conjunction with social functions like status or group signaling. Our findings not only contribute to the discourse on creativity in CTs but also provide insights into the communicative advantage of CTs.

阴谋论(CT)是一种广为流传的壮观叙事,对社会构成威胁。我们测试了 "阴谋论 "是否可能是语言上的创造性产物,这将有助于其传播,从而解释其广为流行的原因。我们分析了大量按主题匹配的阴谋论文本和主流文本语料库中的名词化合物(如精神控制、二氧化碳;N = 1,713,568)。在阴谋论文本中,复合词表现出更高的独创性、差异性和复杂性,但其使用频率较低,在不同语境中重复出现的频率更高。这种模式表明,复合词的产生有其创造性的一面,同时其使用也有一定的局限性。我们将这些发现解释为阴谋论作家之间松散定义的概念界限与社会功能(如地位或群体信号)相结合的影响。我们的研究结果不仅有助于讨论共谋语中的创造性,还为共谋语的交际优势提供了见解。
{"title":"Loose and Tight: Creative Formation but Rigid Use of Nominal Compounds in Conspiracist Texts","authors":"Alessandro Miani,&nbsp;Lonneke van der Plas,&nbsp;Adrian Bangerter","doi":"10.1002/jocb.633","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.633","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Conspiracy theories (CTs) are spectacular narratives, widely spread, that pose societal threats. We test whether CTs might be linguistically creative products, which would facilitate their transmission and thereby account for their widespread popularity. We analyzed nominal compounds (e.g., <i>mind control</i>, <i>carbon dioxide</i>; <i>N</i> = 1,713,568) from a large corpus of conspiracist and mainstream texts matched by topic. In conspiracist texts, compounds showed greater originality, divergence, and sophistication, but they were used with lower frequency and were more often repeated in different contexts. This pattern suggests a creative aspect in the generation of compounds, coupled with rigidity in their use. We interpret these findings as an effect of loosely defined conceptual boundaries among conspiracist writers, in conjunction with social functions like status or group signaling. Our findings not only contribute to the discourse on creativity in CTs but also provide insights into the communicative advantage of CTs.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"114-127"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.633","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139373834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Creative Subprocess Frequencies and Their Relation to Personal Characteristics and Product Creativity: Insights from a Drawing Task Think Aloud Study 创意子过程频率及其与个人特征和产品创意的关系:绘画任务朗读思考研究的启示
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-12-19 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.629
Gregory T. Boldt, James C. Kaufman

Most research on the creative process has focused on idea generation, and the prevalence and influence of many other creative subprocesses remain poorly understood. To clarify different subprocesses' respective roles in creative work, this study investigated their frequencies and associations with creativity-related personal characteristics and product creativity. Undergraduate students (n = 266) articulated their creative thought processes via the think aloud method while completing an open-ended drawing task and completed questionnaires assessing creativity-relevant personal characteristics. Drawings were rated for creativity by quasi-expert judges. Transcripts were coded using an a priori coding scheme informed by models of the creative process, including generation (14%), elaboration (26%), association (9%), selection (4%), evaluation (13%), anchoring (13%), metacognition (7%), and filler (14%). Subprocess frequencies correlated minimally with measures of personal characteristics, although several significant, albeit weak, relationships emerged. A hierarchical regression analysis indicated that the best predictors of drawing creativity were aesthetic fluency, the frequency of anchoring, time spent on task, and, marginally, the frequency of generation. Together, these results provide insight into how relevant personal characteristics and subprocess engagement contribute to drawing creativity and highlight the particular importance of anchoring, which involves monitoring and refining task-related goals and constraints, throughout the creative process.

有关创意过程的大多数研究都集中在创意的产生上,对许多其他创意子过程的普遍性和影响仍知之甚少。为了澄清不同子过程在创意工作中的各自作用,本研究调查了它们的频率以及与创意相关的个人特征和产品创意之间的关联。本科生(n = 266)在完成开放式绘画任务的同时,通过大声思考法表达了他们的创意思维过程,并填写了评估创意相关个人特征的问卷。绘画作品由准专家评委进行创意评分。笔录采用先验编码方案进行编码,该方案参考了创意过程模型,包括生成(14%)、阐述(26%)、联想(9%)、选择(4%)、评价(13%)、锚定(13%)、元认知(7%)和填充(14%)。子过程的频率与个人特征的测量相关性很小,尽管出现了几种重要的关系,但都很微弱。分层回归分析表明,最能预测绘画创造力的因素是审美流畅性、锚定频率、任务花费的时间,以及少量的生成频率。这些结果共同揭示了相关个人特征和子过程参与如何促进绘画创造力,并强调了锚定的特殊重要性,即在整个创造过程中监控和完善与任务相关的目标和限制。
{"title":"Creative Subprocess Frequencies and Their Relation to Personal Characteristics and Product Creativity: Insights from a Drawing Task Think Aloud Study","authors":"Gregory T. Boldt,&nbsp;James C. Kaufman","doi":"10.1002/jocb.629","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.629","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Most research on the creative process has focused on idea generation, and the prevalence and influence of many other creative subprocesses remain poorly understood. To clarify different subprocesses' respective roles in creative work, this study investigated their frequencies and associations with creativity-related personal characteristics and product creativity. Undergraduate students (<i>n</i> = 266) articulated their creative thought processes via the think aloud method while completing an open-ended drawing task and completed questionnaires assessing creativity-relevant personal characteristics. Drawings were rated for creativity by quasi-expert judges. Transcripts were coded using an a priori coding scheme informed by models of the creative process, including generation (14%), elaboration (26%), association (9%), selection (4%), evaluation (13%), anchoring (13%), metacognition (7%), and filler (14%). Subprocess frequencies correlated minimally with measures of personal characteristics, although several significant, albeit weak, relationships emerged. A hierarchical regression analysis indicated that the best predictors of drawing creativity were aesthetic fluency, the frequency of anchoring, time spent on task, and, marginally, the frequency of generation. Together, these results provide insight into how relevant personal characteristics and subprocess engagement contribute to drawing creativity and highlight the particular importance of anchoring, which involves monitoring and refining task-related goals and constraints, throughout the creative process.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"59 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138825201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to “The Machines Take over: A Comparison of Various Supervised Learning Approaches for Automated Scoring of Divergent Thinking Tasks” 更正 "机器接管:用于发散性思维任务自动评分的各种监督学习方法的比较"
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.627
<p>Buczak, P., Huang, H., Forthmann, B., & Doebler, P. (2023). The machines take over: A comparison of various supervised learning approaches for automated scoring of divergent thinking tasks. <i>The Journal of Creative Behavior</i>, <i>57</i>, 17–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.559 </p><p>Due to an error in our code for the data preprocessing, we only utilized the word embeddings (WEs) of the last word in sentences instead of aggregating all WEs. This affected the use of WEs as model features and resulted in incomplete data usage. Our program code has been fixed and we have rerun our simulations.</p><p>We apologize for this error.</p><p>To highlight the most important differences between the findings reported in Buczak et al. (2023) and the corrected findings, Figure 1 shows the changes in terms of RMSE for the three learners on the Hofelich-Mohr et al. (2016) data set when applied in the setting of our first simulation study using two different semantic spaces. Overall, support vector regression (SVR), extreme gradient boosting (XGB), and random forest (RF) models profited greatly from the updated WEs whenever these were directly included as features. As the “Meta” feature set did not contain WEs in any form, the corresponding models and their results remained unaffected by the change. For the “Meta- + WE-based” feature set, however, the model's performance declined slightly. Regarding the learners, the updated WEs mostly led to a noticeable jump in performance for SVR when using a low-dimensional semantic space (i.e., 50 or 100 dimensions). In these cases, SVR even outperformed XGB and RF. However, SVR suffered when using high-dimensional semantic spaces. Apart from SVR, only subtle differences emerged regarding the choice of semantic space for XGB and RF.</p><p>Similar patterns of change were observed in the results for the Silvia et al. (2008) data, and when looking at correlations instead of the prediction RMSE for both data sets (figures were excluded for the sake of brevity). For the cross-sample prediction setup in our second simulation study, this mostly held up as well. However, when training on Hofelich-Mohr et al. (2016) and predicting Silvia et al. (2008), the performance of SVR increased with increasing semantic space dimensionality when studying the RMSE. For correlation scores, SVR's performance, again, suffered when a semantic space of high dimensionality was used.</p><p>As for the models using the WEs directly, the improved performance was to be expected since the aggregated WEs of all words capture more information about the answer than the WEs of a single word. However, the reasons behind the performance decrease for the models using WE-based features seem less clear-cut. Perhaps, WE-based features such as cosine similarity, WE norm, and the number of high-loading WEs degraded slightly in quality because we added word vectors from single words, including words almost unrelated to the idea described.</p><p>Alt
尽管在低维语义空间中,SVR 的部分性能略优于 XGB 和 RF,但我们仍然坚持最初推荐的 XGB 和 RF,因为它们对语义空间选择的敏感度较低。不过,如果要使用 SVR,降低 WE 的维度(例如通过主成分分析)仍然是一个可行的选择。此外,由于新的结果在很大程度上缩小了我们最初分析中存在的预测性能差距,因此我们关于使用加法构词法而非填充法的建议得到了进一步加强。校正过程中的另一个结果是,我们观察到,典型的英语交替使用任务反应通常会将一个名词作为句子末尾所要表达的思想的中心,因此,最后一个词的 WEs 被证明具有令人惊讶的预测性。除此以外,原讨论内容未受影响。
{"title":"Correction to “The Machines Take over: A Comparison of Various Supervised Learning Approaches for Automated Scoring of Divergent Thinking Tasks”","authors":"","doi":"10.1002/jocb.627","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.627","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;Buczak, P., Huang, H., Forthmann, B., &amp; Doebler, P. (2023). The machines take over: A comparison of various supervised learning approaches for automated scoring of divergent thinking tasks. &lt;i&gt;The Journal of Creative Behavior&lt;/i&gt;, &lt;i&gt;57&lt;/i&gt;, 17–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.559\u0000 &lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Due to an error in our code for the data preprocessing, we only utilized the word embeddings (WEs) of the last word in sentences instead of aggregating all WEs. This affected the use of WEs as model features and resulted in incomplete data usage. Our program code has been fixed and we have rerun our simulations.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;We apologize for this error.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;To highlight the most important differences between the findings reported in Buczak et al. (2023) and the corrected findings, Figure 1 shows the changes in terms of RMSE for the three learners on the Hofelich-Mohr et al. (2016) data set when applied in the setting of our first simulation study using two different semantic spaces. Overall, support vector regression (SVR), extreme gradient boosting (XGB), and random forest (RF) models profited greatly from the updated WEs whenever these were directly included as features. As the “Meta” feature set did not contain WEs in any form, the corresponding models and their results remained unaffected by the change. For the “Meta- + WE-based” feature set, however, the model's performance declined slightly. Regarding the learners, the updated WEs mostly led to a noticeable jump in performance for SVR when using a low-dimensional semantic space (i.e., 50 or 100 dimensions). In these cases, SVR even outperformed XGB and RF. However, SVR suffered when using high-dimensional semantic spaces. Apart from SVR, only subtle differences emerged regarding the choice of semantic space for XGB and RF.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Similar patterns of change were observed in the results for the Silvia et al. (2008) data, and when looking at correlations instead of the prediction RMSE for both data sets (figures were excluded for the sake of brevity). For the cross-sample prediction setup in our second simulation study, this mostly held up as well. However, when training on Hofelich-Mohr et al. (2016) and predicting Silvia et al. (2008), the performance of SVR increased with increasing semantic space dimensionality when studying the RMSE. For correlation scores, SVR's performance, again, suffered when a semantic space of high dimensionality was used.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;As for the models using the WEs directly, the improved performance was to be expected since the aggregated WEs of all words capture more information about the answer than the WEs of a single word. However, the reasons behind the performance decrease for the models using WE-based features seem less clear-cut. Perhaps, WE-based features such as cosine similarity, WE norm, and the number of high-loading WEs degraded slightly in quality because we added word vectors from single words, including words almost unrelated to the idea described.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Alt","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 3","pages":"530-532"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.627","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138567220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Associations among Challenge Stress, Hindrance Stress, and Employees' Innovative Work Behavior: Mediation Effects of Thriving at Work and Emotional Exhaustion 挑战压力、阻碍压力与员工创新工作行为之间的关系:工作欣欣向荣与情感枯竭的中介效应
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-29 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.624
Jinyan Xie, Zhonglin Wen, Yiming Ma, Baozhen Cai, Xiqin Liu

Employees' innovative work behavior (IWB) is one of the key factors in improving organizational competitiveness. Previous studies show that challenge and hindrance stress can impact employees' IWB, but our understanding of the exact mechanism underlying the impact is still limited. The present study employed four scales (Challenge and Hindrance Stress Scale, Thriving at Work Scale, Chinese Emotional Exhaustion Scale, and Employee Innovative Behavior Scale) to collect questionnaire data from 789 employees in diverse organizations via an online platform. A two-path mediation model was constructed. The results show that: (a) challenge stress positively predicted thriving at work and IWB; (b) thriving at work played a partial mediation effect between challenge stress and IWB; (c) hindrance stress negatively predicted thriving at work and positively predicted emotional exhaustion; and (4) hindrance stress did not directly impact IWB while thriving at work and emotional exhaustion were main mediators in the relationship between hindrance stress and IWB. These findings suggest that employees should sensibly cope with different work stresses, while managers should plan work tasks scientifically and give employees adequate opportunities to learn and rest in order to keep them in a positive state to solve problems and work creatively.

员工的创新工作行为(IWB)是提高组织竞争力的关键因素之一。以往的研究表明,挑战和阻碍压力会影响员工的创新工作行为,但我们对其确切影响机制的了解仍然有限。本研究采用了四个量表(挑战与阻碍压力量表、工作欣欣向荣量表、中国式情绪耗竭量表和员工创新行为量表),通过在线平台收集了不同组织中 789 名员工的问卷数据。研究构建了一个双路径中介模型。结果表明(a) 挑战压力正向预测了工作欣欣向荣和 IWB;(b) 工作欣欣向荣在挑战压力和 IWB 之间起到了部分中介作用;(c) 阻碍压力负向预测了工作欣欣向荣,正向预测了情绪衰竭;(4) 阻碍压力不直接影响 IWB,而工作欣欣向荣和情绪衰竭是阻碍压力和 IWB 之间关系的主要中介。这些研究结果表明,员工应理智地应对不同的工作压力,而管理者则应科学地规划工作任务,并给予员工充分的学习和休息机会,以保持员工在解决问题和创造性工作时的积极状态。
{"title":"Associations among Challenge Stress, Hindrance Stress, and Employees' Innovative Work Behavior: Mediation Effects of Thriving at Work and Emotional Exhaustion","authors":"Jinyan Xie,&nbsp;Zhonglin Wen,&nbsp;Yiming Ma,&nbsp;Baozhen Cai,&nbsp;Xiqin Liu","doi":"10.1002/jocb.624","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.624","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Employees' innovative work behavior (IWB) is one of the key factors in improving organizational competitiveness. Previous studies show that challenge and hindrance stress can impact employees' IWB, but our understanding of the exact mechanism underlying the impact is still limited. The present study employed four scales (Challenge and Hindrance Stress Scale, Thriving at Work Scale, Chinese Emotional Exhaustion Scale, and Employee Innovative Behavior Scale) to collect questionnaire data from 789 employees in diverse organizations via an online platform. A two-path mediation model was constructed. The results show that: (a) challenge stress positively predicted thriving at work and IWB; (b) thriving at work played a partial mediation effect between challenge stress and IWB; (c) hindrance stress negatively predicted thriving at work and positively predicted emotional exhaustion; and (4) hindrance stress did not directly impact IWB while thriving at work and emotional exhaustion were main mediators in the relationship between hindrance stress and IWB. These findings suggest that employees should sensibly cope with different work stresses, while managers should plan work tasks scientifically and give employees adequate opportunities to learn and rest in order to keep them in a positive state to solve problems and work creatively.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"66-81"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2023-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140343116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Don't Let it be! Creative Co-regulation and Socially Shared Regulation in a Case Study of the Beatles: Get Back 不要让它发生!创造性共同监管和社会共享监管——以披头士乐队为例:回归
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-29 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.626
Kamila Urban, Marek Urban

Criticism has been raised about creativity researchers narrowing their focus solely to the cognitive processes within individuals. To address these concerns, this case study seeks to expand the scope of creativity research by delving into metacognitive, motivational, and emotional processes involved in collaborative creative problem-solving. The present study applies the concepts of self-regulation, co-regulation, and socially shared regulation to a real-world case study: the documentary The Beatles: Get Back. Observational footage of the band rehearsing and creating new songs for their live show is analyzed within a socially shared regulation of learning (SSRL) framework. Theory-driven coding identified individual facets of self-regulation, co-regulation, and socially shared regulation during collaborative creative problem-solving. The analysis revealed initial problems with self-regulation, especially motivation and emotion regulation, inhibiting collaboration. However, co-regulatory prompts between group members facilitated a shift toward more productive socially shared regulation where the band collectively negotiated goals, plans and ideas. An examination of spontaneous interactions highlighted the interplay between individual self-regulation and socially constructed regulation shaping real-world creative collaboration. Individual metacognitive skills, motivation, and emotions, as well as socially shared group dynamics enabled the members to regulate uncertainty and obstacles in collaborative creative problem-solving.

有人批评创造力研究者将他们的研究焦点仅仅局限于个体的认知过程。为了解决这些问题,本案例研究试图通过深入研究涉及协作创造性问题解决的元认知、动机和情感过程来扩大创造力研究的范围。本研究将自我监管、共同监管和社会共享监管的概念应用于现实世界的案例研究:纪录片《披头士:回归》。乐队排练和为现场表演创作新歌的观察镜头在社会共享学习调节(SSRL)框架内进行分析。理论驱动编码确定了协作创造性问题解决过程中自我调节、共同调节和社会共享调节的各个方面。分析揭示了自我调节的最初问题,特别是动机和情绪调节,阻碍了合作。然而,团体成员之间的共同监管促进了向更富有成效的社会共享监管的转变,在这种监管中,乐队集体协商目标、计划和想法。对自发互动的研究强调了个人自我调节和社会构建调节之间的相互作用,形成了现实世界的创造性合作。个体元认知技能、动机和情绪以及社会共享的群体动力使成员能够调节协作创造性问题解决中的不确定性和障碍。
{"title":"Don't Let it be! Creative Co-regulation and Socially Shared Regulation in a Case Study of the Beatles: Get Back","authors":"Kamila Urban,&nbsp;Marek Urban","doi":"10.1002/jocb.626","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.626","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Criticism has been raised about creativity researchers narrowing their focus solely to the cognitive processes within individuals. To address these concerns, this case study seeks to expand the scope of creativity research by delving into metacognitive, motivational, and emotional processes involved in collaborative creative problem-solving. The present study applies the concepts of self-regulation, co-regulation, and socially shared regulation to a real-world case study: the documentary <i>The Beatles: Get Back</i>. Observational footage of the band rehearsing and creating new songs for their live show is analyzed within a socially shared regulation of learning (SSRL) framework. Theory-driven coding identified individual facets of self-regulation, co-regulation, and socially shared regulation during collaborative creative problem-solving. The analysis revealed initial problems with self-regulation, especially motivation and emotion regulation, inhibiting collaboration. However, co-regulatory prompts between group members facilitated a shift toward more productive socially shared regulation where the band collectively negotiated goals, plans and ideas. An examination of spontaneous interactions highlighted the interplay between individual self-regulation and socially constructed regulation shaping real-world creative collaboration. Individual metacognitive skills, motivation, and emotions, as well as socially shared group dynamics enabled the members to regulate uncertainty and obstacles in collaborative creative problem-solving.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"59 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138505737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mechanisms for Affect Communication from Dance: A Mixed Methods Study 舞蹈情感交流的机制:一项混合方法研究
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-29 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.622
Megan G. Stutesman, Thalia R. Goldstein

Dance is a multifaceted creative act that has been used to communicate emotions throughout human evolution. Despite this history, there has been no empirical exploration of components of dance that allow it to communicate emotion. We address this with a mixed methods study in which a quantitative study with dance viewers builds upon qualitative findings with dance artists. Qualitative analysis revealed proposed components of dance responsible for emotion communication are narrative content, social interaction, emotion portrayal intent, and textural quality of movement. These elements were then manipulated using dance video stimuli to quantitatively test viewers' emotion perception accuracy. Results revealed emotion portrayal intent (i.e., expression intent in dance creation) is an important factor for viewers to accurately perceive emotion, regardless of narrative content. While prior studies have focused on the perceiver's role in emotion perception from dance, this is the first study to demonstrate emotions are perceived from dance based on intent during dance creation. Findings highlight the nuanced structure of emotion communication in dance, for which we pose a theoretical framework. Together, dance holds possibilities for studying distinct mechanisms for creative communication that may be important for the study of generalized affect communication outside of creative arts contexts.

舞蹈是一种多方面的创造性行为,在整个人类进化过程中一直被用来交流情感。尽管有这样的历史,但还没有对舞蹈的组成部分进行实证探索,使其能够交流情感。我们通过混合方法研究来解决这个问题,其中对舞蹈观众进行定量研究,建立在对舞蹈艺术家进行定性研究的基础上。定性分析表明,舞蹈中负责情感交流的成分包括叙事内容、社会互动、情感刻画意图和动作的质感。然后使用舞蹈视频刺激来操纵这些元素,以定量测试观众的情绪感知准确性。结果表明,无论叙事内容如何,情感刻画意图(即舞蹈创作中的表达意图)都是观众准确感知情感的重要因素。虽然之前的研究主要集中在感知者在舞蹈情感感知中的作用,但这是第一个证明在舞蹈创作过程中基于意图从舞蹈中感知情感的研究。研究结果强调了舞蹈中情感交流的微妙结构,为此我们提出了一个理论框架。总之,舞蹈为研究创造性交流的不同机制提供了可能性,这对于研究创造性艺术背景之外的广义情感交流可能很重要。
{"title":"Mechanisms for Affect Communication from Dance: A Mixed Methods Study","authors":"Megan G. Stutesman,&nbsp;Thalia R. Goldstein","doi":"10.1002/jocb.622","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.622","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dance is a multifaceted creative act that has been used to communicate emotions throughout human evolution. Despite this history, there has been no empirical exploration of components of dance that allow it to communicate emotion. We address this with a mixed methods study in which a quantitative study with dance viewers builds upon qualitative findings with dance artists. Qualitative analysis revealed proposed components of dance responsible for emotion communication are <i>narrative content, social interaction, emotion portrayal intent</i>, and <i>textural quality of movement.</i> These elements were then manipulated using dance video stimuli to quantitatively test viewers' emotion perception accuracy. Results revealed <i>emotion portrayal intent</i> (i.e., expression intent in dance creation) is an important factor for viewers to accurately perceive emotion, regardless of <i>narrative content</i>. While prior studies have focused on the perceiver's role in emotion perception from dance, this is the first study to demonstrate emotions are perceived from dance based on <i>intent</i> during dance creation. Findings highlight the nuanced structure of emotion communication in dance, for which we pose a theoretical framework. Together, dance holds possibilities for studying distinct mechanisms for creative communication that may be important for the study of generalized affect communication outside of creative arts contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"28-46"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2023-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.622","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138505723","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nothing But Stereotypes? Negligible Sex Differences Across Creativity Measures in Science, Arts, and Sports Adolescent High Achievers 除了刻板印象什么都没有?在科学、艺术和体育方面,青少年高成就者的创造力测量中的性别差异可以忽略不计
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-23 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.623
Vlada Repeykova, Teemu Toivainen, Maxim Likhanov, Kim van Broekhoven, Yulia Kovas

Previous research has focused on understanding when, why, and how sex differences in creativity occur, as results vary across samples, measures, and methodologies. In the current study we investigated sex differences in creativity among 984 high achieving adolescents in three expertise areas: Sciences, Arts, and Sports. Eight creativity indicators were analyzed: Alternative uses task (AUT) fluency; creative self-efficacy (CSE); intraindividual strengths (difference between CSE and AUT Fluency); five self-reported creativity scales: Self/everyday, scholarly, performance, mechanical/scientific, artistic. The results showed negligible sex differences (np2 = .01), with females performing better in AUT Fluency and males self-rating their CSE higher. No sex differences were found in self/everyday, scholarly and performance creativity. Males self-rated their mechanical/scientific creativity (np2 = .06) higher than females; while females self-rated their artistic creativity (np2 = .02) higher in comparison to males. Our results extend the existing literature by finding negligible sex differences in adolescent expert groups. However, some stereotypical differences emerged, for example, females with Sciences expertise rated their mechanical/scientific creativity lower than males with and even without Sciences expertise. Results call for further investigation into the links between sex differences, expertise, and specific creativity domains.

之前的研究主要集中在理解创造力的性别差异发生的时间、原因和方式,因为不同样本、测量方法和方法的结果不同。在目前的研究中,我们调查了984名在科学、艺术和体育三个专业领域取得优异成绩的青少年在创造力方面的性别差异。分析了八项创造力指标:替代性使用任务(AUT)流畅性;创造性自我效能(CSE);个人优势(CSE和AUT流利度的差异);五个自我报告的创造力量表:自我/日常、学术、表演、机械/科学、艺术。结果显示,性别差异可以忽略不计(np2 $$ {n}_{mathrm{p}}^2 $$ = 0.01),女性在AUT流畅性方面表现较好,而男性对其CSE的自我评价较高。在自我/日常、学术和表演创造力方面没有发现性别差异。男性自评机械/科学创造力(np2 $$ {n}_{mathrm{p}}^2 $$ = .06)高于女性;而女性对自己艺术创造力的自我评价(np2 $$ {n}_{mathrm{p}}^2 $$ = .02)则高于男性。我们的研究结果扩展了现有的文献,在青少年专家群体中发现了可以忽略不计的性别差异。然而,出现了一些刻板的差异,例如,具有科学专业知识的女性对自己的机械/科学创造力的评价低于具有甚至没有科学专业知识的男性。结果要求进一步调查性别差异、专业知识和特定创造力领域之间的联系。
{"title":"Nothing But Stereotypes? Negligible Sex Differences Across Creativity Measures in Science, Arts, and Sports Adolescent High Achievers","authors":"Vlada Repeykova,&nbsp;Teemu Toivainen,&nbsp;Maxim Likhanov,&nbsp;Kim van Broekhoven,&nbsp;Yulia Kovas","doi":"10.1002/jocb.623","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.623","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research has focused on understanding when, why, and how sex differences in creativity occur, as results vary across samples, measures, and methodologies. In the current study we investigated sex differences in creativity among 984 high achieving adolescents in three expertise areas: Sciences, Arts, and Sports. Eight creativity indicators were analyzed: Alternative uses task (AUT) fluency; creative self-efficacy (CSE); intraindividual strengths (difference between CSE and AUT Fluency); five self-reported creativity scales: Self/everyday, scholarly, performance, mechanical/scientific, artistic. The results showed negligible sex differences (<span></span><math>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msubsup>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mi>p</mi>\u0000 <mn>2</mn>\u0000 </msubsup>\u0000 </mrow></math> = .01), with females performing better in AUT Fluency and males self-rating their CSE higher. No sex differences were found in self/everyday, scholarly and performance creativity. Males self-rated their mechanical/scientific creativity (<span></span><math>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msubsup>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mi>p</mi>\u0000 <mn>2</mn>\u0000 </msubsup>\u0000 </mrow></math> = .06) higher than females; while females self-rated their artistic creativity (<span></span><math>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msubsup>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mi>p</mi>\u0000 <mn>2</mn>\u0000 </msubsup>\u0000 </mrow></math> = .02) higher in comparison to males. Our results extend the existing literature by finding negligible sex differences in adolescent expert groups. However, some stereotypical differences emerged, for example, females with Sciences expertise rated their mechanical/scientific creativity lower than males with and even without Sciences expertise. Results call for further investigation into the links between sex differences, expertise, and specific creativity domains.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"58 1","pages":"47-65"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2023-11-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.623","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138505724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Reliability Generalization Meta-Analysis of the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale 创造力量表考夫曼域的信度概化元分析
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-20 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.620
Sedat Sen, Süreyya Yörük

The Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS) is a self-reported rating scale that measures creative behaviors in five areas. Despite the vast amount of research on the scale, the internal consistency reliability of K-DOCS scores have not been examined. Specifically, there is no study on the overall reliability coefficients, the variation in the reliability of scores, and reliability induction. In the present study, reliability generalization meta-analyses were conducted to address these issues. The sample consisted of 56 studies that produced 60 Cronbach's alpha coefficients in total. The pooled alpha values were estimated to be .904 (total scale), .825 (Self/Everyday subscale), .858 (Scholarly subscale), .887 (Performance subscale), .867 (Scientific subscale), and .861 (Artistic subscale). The moderator analyses showed that the reliability estimates of K-DOCS total scores and Self/Everyday subscale scores did not differ with respect to any of the moderator variables. On the other hand, certain variables affected the alpha coefficients for Scholarly (educational level, SD of the age, and mean age), Performance (continent, educational level, ethnicity, Caucasian percentage, SD of the age, and mean age), Scientific (language, test version, continent, country, ethnicity, SD of the age, and mean age), and Artistic (continent, language, country, mean age, and total mean score) subscale scores. Finally, the reliability induction rate was 39.62%, and there were no statistically significant differences between the inducing and reporting studies with respect to the continuous variables (mean of the total score, means and SDs of the sample age, and percentages of female and Caucasian). Our findings indicate that the K-DOCS provides highly reliable scores. However, certain variables contribute to systematic errors in Scholarly, Performance, Scientific, and Artistic subscale scores. Hence, scores on these subscales should be interpreted with caution. Due to a high amount of variation in the reliability coefficients, reliability induction is not advised for the K-DOCS.

考夫曼创造力领域量表(K-DOCS)是一个自我报告的评分量表,衡量五个领域的创造性行为。尽管对该量表进行了大量的研究,但尚未对K-DOCS分数的内部一致性信度进行检验。具体而言,没有对总体信度系数、分数信度变异和信度诱导进行研究。本研究采用信度概化元分析来解决这些问题。样本包括56项研究,总共产生60个克朗巴赫α系数。合并alpha值估计为0.904(总量表)、0.825(自我/日常量表)、0.858(学术量表)、0.887(表现量表)、0.867(科学量表)和0.861(艺术量表)。调节分析显示,K-DOCS总分和自我/日常分量表得分的信度估计在任何调节变量上都没有差异。另一方面,某些变量影响了学术(教育水平、年龄标准差和平均年龄)、表现(大陆、教育水平、种族、高加索百分比、年龄标准差和平均年龄)、科学(语言、测试版本、大陆、国家、种族、年龄标准差和平均年龄)和艺术(大陆、语言、国家、平均年龄和总平均分)子量表得分的α系数。最后,信度诱导率为39.62%,诱导研究和报告研究在连续变量(总分均值、样本年龄均值和标准差、女性和高加索人百分比)上差异无统计学意义。我们的研究结果表明,K-DOCS提供了高度可靠的评分。然而,某些变量会导致学术、表演、科学和艺术分量表得分的系统性错误。因此,在这些子量表上的分数应该谨慎解释。由于可靠性系数的大量变化,不建议对K-DOCS进行可靠性诱导。
{"title":"A Reliability Generalization Meta-Analysis of the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale","authors":"Sedat Sen,&nbsp;Süreyya Yörük","doi":"10.1002/jocb.620","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.620","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS) is a self-reported rating scale that measures creative behaviors in five areas. Despite the vast amount of research on the scale, the internal consistency reliability of K-DOCS scores have not been examined. Specifically, there is no study on the overall reliability coefficients, the variation in the reliability of scores, and reliability induction. In the present study, reliability generalization meta-analyses were conducted to address these issues. The sample consisted of 56 studies that produced 60 Cronbach's alpha coefficients in total. The pooled alpha values were estimated to be .904 (total scale), .825 (Self/Everyday subscale), .858 (Scholarly subscale), .887 (Performance subscale), .867 (Scientific subscale), and .861 (Artistic subscale). The moderator analyses showed that the reliability estimates of K-DOCS total scores and Self/Everyday subscale scores did not differ with respect to any of the moderator variables. On the other hand, certain variables affected the alpha coefficients for Scholarly (educational level, <i>SD</i> of the age, and mean age), Performance (continent, educational level, ethnicity, Caucasian percentage, <i>SD</i> of the age, and mean age), Scientific (language, test version, continent, country, ethnicity, <i>SD</i> of the age, and mean age), and Artistic (continent, language, country, mean age, and total mean score) subscale scores. Finally, the reliability induction rate was 39.62%, and there were no statistically significant differences between the inducing and reporting studies with respect to the continuous variables (mean of the total score, means and <i>SD</i>s of the sample age, and percentages of female and Caucasian). Our findings indicate that the K-DOCS provides highly reliable scores. However, certain variables contribute to systematic errors in Scholarly, Performance, Scientific, and Artistic subscale scores. Hence, scores on these subscales should be interpreted with caution. Due to a high amount of variation in the reliability coefficients, reliability induction is not advised for the K-DOCS.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"57 4","pages":"812-837"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.620","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138505738","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Creative Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1