首页 > 最新文献

Thinking & Reasoning最新文献

英文 中文
The mediating effect of reflective-analytic cognitive style on rational thought 反思分析型认知方式对理性思维的中介作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-07-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1634151
Ralph E. Viator, Nancy L. Harp, S. Rinaldo, Blair B. Marquardt
Abstract We investigate whether non-miserly cognitive styles mediate the effects of cognitive ability and thinking dispositions on rational thought. Specifically, we review relevant literature on two dimensions that define non-miserly cognitive style: reflection-impulsivity and analytic-intuitive. We use these two dimensions to identify a continuum of cognitive styles that vary from miserly (impulsive-intuitive) to non-miserly (reflective-analytic) and are congruent with tendencies to commit specific rational thinking errors. Further, we argue that this continuum, which we label reflective-analytic cognitive style, mediates the effect of cognitive ability and thinking dispositions on rational thought. We conduct an experiment testing our predictions and find evidence, via structural equation modelling, that reflective-analytic cognitive style does mediate the effect of cognitive ability (as measured by working memory capacity) and thinking dispositions (as measured by need for cognition and actively open-minded thinking) on responses to traditional rational thinking tasks.
摘要本研究探讨非吝啬型认知风格是否介导认知能力和思维倾向对理性思维的影响。具体来说,我们回顾了定义非吝啬认知风格的两个维度的相关文献:反思-冲动性和分析-直觉。我们使用这两个维度来识别从吝啬(冲动-直觉)到非吝啬(反思-分析)的连续认知风格,并与犯下特定理性思维错误的倾向一致。此外,我们认为,这种连续体,我们称之为反思-分析认知风格,调解了认知能力和思维倾向对理性思维的影响。我们进行了一项实验来检验我们的预测,并通过结构方程模型找到证据,证明反思分析型认知风格确实介导了认知能力(以工作记忆容量为衡量标准)和思维倾向(以认知需求和积极开放思维为衡量标准)对传统理性思维任务的反应的影响。
{"title":"The mediating effect of reflective-analytic cognitive style on rational thought","authors":"Ralph E. Viator, Nancy L. Harp, S. Rinaldo, Blair B. Marquardt","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1634151","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1634151","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract We investigate whether non-miserly cognitive styles mediate the effects of cognitive ability and thinking dispositions on rational thought. Specifically, we review relevant literature on two dimensions that define non-miserly cognitive style: reflection-impulsivity and analytic-intuitive. We use these two dimensions to identify a continuum of cognitive styles that vary from miserly (impulsive-intuitive) to non-miserly (reflective-analytic) and are congruent with tendencies to commit specific rational thinking errors. Further, we argue that this continuum, which we label reflective-analytic cognitive style, mediates the effect of cognitive ability and thinking dispositions on rational thought. We conduct an experiment testing our predictions and find evidence, via structural equation modelling, that reflective-analytic cognitive style does mediate the effect of cognitive ability (as measured by working memory capacity) and thinking dispositions (as measured by need for cognition and actively open-minded thinking) on responses to traditional rational thinking tasks.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"49 1","pages":"381 - 413"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89978874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
The relationship between anomalistic belief, misperception of chance and the base rate fallacy 异常信念、机会误解与基本率谬误之间的关系
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-07-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1653371
Toby Prike, Michelle M. Arnold, Paul Williamson
Abstract A poor understanding of probability may lead people to misinterpret every day coincidences and form anomalistic (e.g., paranormal) beliefs. We investigated the relationship between anomalistic belief (including type of belief) and misperception of chance and the base rate fallacy across both anomalistic and control (i.e., neutral) contexts. Greater anomalistic belief was associated with poorer performance for both types of items; however there were no significant interactions between belief and context. For misperception of chance items, only experiential (vs. theoretical) anomalistic beliefs predicted more errors. In contrast, overall anomalistic belief was positively related to the base rate fallacy but no specific subtype of anomalistic belief was a significant predictor. The results indicate misperception of chance may lead people to interpret coincidental events as having an anomalistic cause, and a poor understanding of base rates may make people more prone to forming anomalistic beliefs.
对概率的不理解可能会导致人们误解日常的巧合,形成反常的(例如,超自然的)信仰。我们研究了异常信念(包括信念类型)与在异常和控制(即中性)两种情况下对机会和基本率谬误的误解之间的关系。异常信念越大,在这两类项目上的表现就越差;然而,信念与情境之间没有显著的交互作用。对于偶然性项目的误解,只有经验(相对于理论)异常信念预测更多的错误。总体异常信念与基本率谬论呈正相关,但没有特定亚型的异常信念是显著的预测因子。结果表明,对机会的误解可能会导致人们将巧合事件解释为具有异常原因,而对基本比率的理解不足可能会使人们更容易形成异常信念。
{"title":"The relationship between anomalistic belief, misperception of chance and the base rate fallacy","authors":"Toby Prike, Michelle M. Arnold, Paul Williamson","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1653371","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1653371","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract A poor understanding of probability may lead people to misinterpret every day coincidences and form anomalistic (e.g., paranormal) beliefs. We investigated the relationship between anomalistic belief (including type of belief) and misperception of chance and the base rate fallacy across both anomalistic and control (i.e., neutral) contexts. Greater anomalistic belief was associated with poorer performance for both types of items; however there were no significant interactions between belief and context. For misperception of chance items, only experiential (vs. theoretical) anomalistic beliefs predicted more errors. In contrast, overall anomalistic belief was positively related to the base rate fallacy but no specific subtype of anomalistic belief was a significant predictor. The results indicate misperception of chance may lead people to interpret coincidental events as having an anomalistic cause, and a poor understanding of base rates may make people more prone to forming anomalistic beliefs.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"20 1","pages":"447 - 477"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77616150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Four- to six-year-olds’ ratio reasoning-from 2D to 3D quantities 4 - 6岁儿童的比例推理——从2D到3D的数量
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-06-24 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1783362
Yingying Yang, Wei He
Abstract Recent research has suggested that young children may have primitive knowledge of ratio and proportions. However, it is unclear how precisely young children represent ratio magnitudes and how well they apply their ratio knowledge across different contexts. The current study examined 4- to 6-year-olds’ reasoning of ratio magnitudes. In the baseline task, children judged ratio relations between 2 D lengths. In the 3 D depth task, children judged ratio relations of two pictorial depths. In the 3 D cube task, children judged ratio relations of the volumes of two pictorially presented cubes. Linear regression models of estimated ratios on expected ratios were significant across all three tasks for all children groups. Children demonstrated a remarkable ability in understanding and expressing ratio magnitudes. However, relative to the baseline condition, the 3 D depth and cube tasks were associated with smaller explained variance and, hence, more noise. Children, particularly 4-year-olds, were unable to distinguish ratios of slightly different magnitudes in the 3 D cube task. Our study reveals both the potential and the limits of young children’s ratio reasoning abilities.
最近的研究表明,幼儿可能具有原始的比率和比例知识。然而,目前尚不清楚幼儿如何准确地表示比率大小,以及他们如何在不同的环境中应用比率知识。目前的研究考察了4到6岁儿童对比例大小的推理能力。在基线任务中,孩子们判断二维长度之间的比例关系。在三维深度任务中,儿童判断两个图形深度的比例关系。在三维立方体任务中,儿童判断两个图形呈现的立方体体积的比例关系。估计比率对期望比率的线性回归模型在所有儿童组的所有三个任务中都是显著的。孩子们在理解和表达比例方面表现出了显著的能力。然而,相对于基线条件,3d深度和立方体任务与较小的解释方差相关,因此,更多的噪声。在3d立方体任务中,孩子们,尤其是4岁的孩子,无法区分大小稍有不同的比例。我们的研究揭示了幼儿比例推理能力的潜力和局限性。
{"title":"Four- to six-year-olds’ ratio reasoning-from 2D to 3D quantities","authors":"Yingying Yang, Wei He","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1783362","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1783362","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Recent research has suggested that young children may have primitive knowledge of ratio and proportions. However, it is unclear how precisely young children represent ratio magnitudes and how well they apply their ratio knowledge across different contexts. The current study examined 4- to 6-year-olds’ reasoning of ratio magnitudes. In the baseline task, children judged ratio relations between 2 D lengths. In the 3 D depth task, children judged ratio relations of two pictorial depths. In the 3 D cube task, children judged ratio relations of the volumes of two pictorially presented cubes. Linear regression models of estimated ratios on expected ratios were significant across all three tasks for all children groups. Children demonstrated a remarkable ability in understanding and expressing ratio magnitudes. However, relative to the baseline condition, the 3 D depth and cube tasks were associated with smaller explained variance and, hence, more noise. Children, particularly 4-year-olds, were unable to distinguish ratios of slightly different magnitudes in the 3 D cube task. Our study reveals both the potential and the limits of young children’s ratio reasoning abilities.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"15 1","pages":"212 - 238"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88330872","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Errors, fast and slow: an analysis of response times in probability judgments 错误,快与慢:对概率判断中反应时间的分析
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-06-19 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1781691
Jonas Ludwig, Fabian K. Ahrens, A. Achtziger
Abstract Based on the Dual-Process Diffusion Model, we tested three hypotheses about response times of errors and correct responses in probability judgments. We predicted that correct responses were (1) slower than errors in the case of conflicting decision processes but (2) faster than errors in the case of alignment; and that they were (3) slower in the case of conflict than in the case of alignment. A binary-choice experiment was conducted in which three types of decision problems elicited conflict or alignment of a deliberative decision process and a heuristic decision process. Consistent with the traditional dual-process architecture, the former captured computational-normative decision strategies and the latter described intuitive-affective aspects of decision making. The hypotheses (1) and (3) were supported, while no statistically significant evidence was found for (2). Implications for the generalisability of the Dual-Process Diffusion Model to slow probability judgments are discussed.
摘要基于双过程扩散模型,对概率判断中错误反应时间和正确反应时间的三个假设进行了检验。我们预测正确的反应(1)在冲突决策过程中比错误的反应慢,但(2)在一致的情况下比错误的反应快;他们(3)在冲突的情况下比在结盟的情况下慢。在三种类型的决策问题中,审慎决策过程和启发式决策过程相互冲突或一致,进行了二元选择实验。与传统的双进程架构一致,前者捕获计算规范的决策策略,后者描述决策的直觉-情感方面。假设(1)和(3)得到了支持,而(2)没有发现统计学上显著的证据。本文讨论了双过程扩散模型对慢概率判断的通用性的影响。
{"title":"Errors, fast and slow: an analysis of response times in probability judgments","authors":"Jonas Ludwig, Fabian K. Ahrens, A. Achtziger","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1781691","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1781691","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Based on the Dual-Process Diffusion Model, we tested three hypotheses about response times of errors and correct responses in probability judgments. We predicted that correct responses were (1) slower than errors in the case of conflicting decision processes but (2) faster than errors in the case of alignment; and that they were (3) slower in the case of conflict than in the case of alignment. A binary-choice experiment was conducted in which three types of decision problems elicited conflict or alignment of a deliberative decision process and a heuristic decision process. Consistent with the traditional dual-process architecture, the former captured computational-normative decision strategies and the latter described intuitive-affective aspects of decision making. The hypotheses (1) and (3) were supported, while no statistically significant evidence was found for (2). Implications for the generalisability of the Dual-Process Diffusion Model to slow probability judgments are discussed.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"90 1","pages":"627 - 639"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77908498","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes 对受污染意识的三个领域的潜在维度结构的考察:超自然信仰、阴谋信仰和反科学态度
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-05-05 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1759688
Jala Rizeq, D. Flora, M. Toplak
Abstract The concept of contaminated mindware provides one conceptualization for measuring beliefs and attitudes about three domains that have evaluation-disabling properties in the context of reasoning: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. We tested the underlying structure of individual differences in these three domains of contaminated mindware and their predictors in a sample of 321 Canadian undergraduate students. The predictors included cognitive ability, cognitive reflection, the dispositional tendency of actively open-minded thinking, and ontological confusions. A hierarchical model with three correlated general factors of paranormal, conspiracy, and anti-science beliefs and attitudes and four specific paranormal factors (i.e., psi, superstition, spiritualism, and precognition) was optimal. While all predictors were significantly correlated with the contaminated mindware domains, structural equation modeling results supported the unique effects of ontological confusions and actively open-minded thinking. The current results support the multidimensional nature of contaminated mindware domains and highlight some of its correlates and unique predictors. Providing a structure and theoretical framework for unwarranted beliefs and attitudes will be useful for measuring their potential impact on the processes of human reasoning.
污染思维的概念提供了一种概念,用于测量在推理背景下具有评估致残属性的三个领域的信念和态度:超自然信念、阴谋信念和反科学态度。我们以321名加拿大本科生为样本,测试了这三个污染思维领域的个体差异及其预测因素的潜在结构。预测因子包括认知能力、认知反思、主动开放思维倾向和本体论困惑。具有超自然、阴谋和反科学信仰和态度三个相关的一般因素和四个特定超自然因素(即psi、迷信、唯心论和预知)的层次模型是最优的。虽然所有预测因子都与受污染的思维域显著相关,但结构方程模型结果支持本体论混淆和积极开放思维的独特影响。目前的结果支持受污染的思维域的多维性,并强调了一些相关因素和独特的预测因素。为无根据的信念和态度提供一个结构和理论框架,将有助于衡量它们对人类推理过程的潜在影响。
{"title":"An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes","authors":"Jala Rizeq, D. Flora, M. Toplak","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1759688","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1759688","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The concept of contaminated mindware provides one conceptualization for measuring beliefs and attitudes about three domains that have evaluation-disabling properties in the context of reasoning: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. We tested the underlying structure of individual differences in these three domains of contaminated mindware and their predictors in a sample of 321 Canadian undergraduate students. The predictors included cognitive ability, cognitive reflection, the dispositional tendency of actively open-minded thinking, and ontological confusions. A hierarchical model with three correlated general factors of paranormal, conspiracy, and anti-science beliefs and attitudes and four specific paranormal factors (i.e., psi, superstition, spiritualism, and precognition) was optimal. While all predictors were significantly correlated with the contaminated mindware domains, structural equation modeling results supported the unique effects of ontological confusions and actively open-minded thinking. The current results support the multidimensional nature of contaminated mindware domains and highlight some of its correlates and unique predictors. Providing a structure and theoretical framework for unwarranted beliefs and attitudes will be useful for measuring their potential impact on the processes of human reasoning.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"8 1","pages":"187 - 211"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-05-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86043887","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 47
Disfluent fonts do not help people to solve math and non-math problems regardless of their numeracy 不流畅的字体不能帮助人们解决数学和非数学问题,不管他们的计算能力如何
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1759689
M. Sirota, Andriana Theodoropoulou, Marie Juanchich
Abstract Prior research has suggested that perceptual disfluency activates analytical processing and increases the solution rate of mathematical problems with appealing but incorrect answers (i.e., the Cognitive Reflection Test, hereafter CRT). However, a recent meta-analysis does not support such a conclusion. We tested here whether insufficient numerical ability can account for this discrepancy. We found strong evidence against the disfluency effect on the problem-solving rate for the Numerical CRT problems regardless of participants’ numeracy and for the Verbal CRT non-math problems (n = 310, Exp. 1) even though simple instructions to pay attention to and reflect upon the Verbal CRT problems substantially increased their solution rate (n = 311, Exp. 2). The updated meta-analysis (k = 18) yielded close-to-zero effect, Hedge’s g = −0.01, 95% CI[-0.05, 0.03] and decisive evidence against the disfluency effect on math problems, BF0+ = 151.6. Thus, perceptual disfluency does not activate analytical processing.
先前的研究表明,知觉不流畅激活了分析加工,并增加了具有吸引人但答案错误的数学问题(即认知反射测试,以下简称CRT)的解题率。然而,最近的一项荟萃分析并不支持这样的结论。我们在这里测试了计算能力不足是否可以解释这种差异。我们发现了强有力的证据,证明无论参与者的计算能力如何,在数值CRT问题和口头CRT非数学问题(n = 310,实验1)中,尽管注意和反思口头CRT问题的简单指示大大提高了他们的解决率(n = 311,实验2)。更新的元分析(k = 18)产生了接近于零的影响,Hedge ' s g = - 0.01, 95% CI[-0.05]。0.03]对数学问题的不流畅效应有决定性的证据,BF0+ = 151.6。因此,知觉不流畅并不会激活分析加工。
{"title":"Disfluent fonts do not help people to solve math and non-math problems regardless of their numeracy","authors":"M. Sirota, Andriana Theodoropoulou, Marie Juanchich","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1759689","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1759689","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Prior research has suggested that perceptual disfluency activates analytical processing and increases the solution rate of mathematical problems with appealing but incorrect answers (i.e., the Cognitive Reflection Test, hereafter CRT). However, a recent meta-analysis does not support such a conclusion. We tested here whether insufficient numerical ability can account for this discrepancy. We found strong evidence against the disfluency effect on the problem-solving rate for the Numerical CRT problems regardless of participants’ numeracy and for the Verbal CRT non-math problems (n = 310, Exp. 1) even though simple instructions to pay attention to and reflect upon the Verbal CRT problems substantially increased their solution rate (n = 311, Exp. 2). The updated meta-analysis (k = 18) yielded close-to-zero effect, Hedge’s g = −0.01, 95% CI[-0.05, 0.03] and decisive evidence against the disfluency effect on math problems, BF0+ = 151.6. Thus, perceptual disfluency does not activate analytical processing.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"67 1","pages":"142 - 159"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79206998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Limits of the foreign language effect: intertemporal choice 外语效应的限制:跨期选择
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-04-03 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2021.1934899
Michał J. Białek, A. Domurat, M. Paruzel-Czachura, Rafał Muda
Abstract Intertemporal choice requires one to decide between smaller sooner and larger later payoffs and is captured by discount rates. Across two preregistered experiments testing three language pairs (Polish vs. English, Spanish, and German; Experiment 1) and with incentivized participants (Experiment 2), we found no evidence that using a foreign language decreased the strength or increased the consistency of intertemporal choices. On the contrary, there was some evidence of stronger discounting when a foreign language was used. We confirmed prior findings that more reflective individuals discount less strongly but observed that they were just as (un)affected by using foreign language as less reflective individuals. Thus, we provide preliminary evidence that the foreign language effect is robust to individual differences in cognitive reflection.
跨期选择要求人们在较小的较早收益和较大的较晚收益之间做出选择,并由贴现率捕获。在两个预先注册的实验中,测试了三种语言对(波兰语与英语、西班牙语和德语;在实验1和实验2中,我们发现没有证据表明使用外语会降低跨期选择的强度或增加跨期选择的一致性。相反,有一些证据表明,当使用外语时,折扣更大。我们证实了先前的发现,更多反思的人不那么强烈地折扣,但观察到他们和较少反思的人一样(不)受使用外语的影响。因此,我们提供了初步的证据,证明外语效应对认知反射的个体差异是稳健的。
{"title":"Limits of the foreign language effect: intertemporal choice","authors":"Michał J. Białek, A. Domurat, M. Paruzel-Czachura, Rafał Muda","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2021.1934899","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2021.1934899","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Intertemporal choice requires one to decide between smaller sooner and larger later payoffs and is captured by discount rates. Across two preregistered experiments testing three language pairs (Polish vs. English, Spanish, and German; Experiment 1) and with incentivized participants (Experiment 2), we found no evidence that using a foreign language decreased the strength or increased the consistency of intertemporal choices. On the contrary, there was some evidence of stronger discounting when a foreign language was used. We confirmed prior findings that more reflective individuals discount less strongly but observed that they were just as (un)affected by using foreign language as less reflective individuals. Thus, we provide preliminary evidence that the foreign language effect is robust to individual differences in cognitive reflection.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"8 1","pages":"97 - 124"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84232982","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Erroneous gambling-related beliefs emerge from broader beliefs during problem-solving: a critical review and classification scheme 在解决问题的过程中,错误的赌博相关信念从更广泛的信念中产生:一个批判性的审查和分类方案
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-04-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1590233
Anastasia Ejova, K. Ohtsuka
Abstract Erroneous gambling-related beliefs (EGRBs) can be defined as beliefs that imply a failure to recognise how commercial gambling activities are designed to generate a guaranteed loss to players. In theorising about how EGRBs develop, previous reviews have proposed that EGRBs are extensions of decision-making heuristics and associated biases. We propose an alternative generative mechanism: one in which gambling games make substantial wins seem possible through problem-solving and eventual correct strategic action. EGRBs are then beliefs in the possibility of correct strategic action (illusions of control) that develop as players trial candidate strategies—strategies selected based on various broader beliefs. We further propose that EGRBs can be classified based on what is theorised in cognitive science about categories of general human beliefs about the world. For example, it has been theorised that human beliefs about supernatural forces and randomness have certain similarities across cultures, and so we propose that there exists a category of supernatural EGRBs, as well as a category of EGRBs based on broader beliefs about the nature of randomness. We review evidence for this classification scheme and discuss how it can be applied in researching and treating gambling disorder.
错误的赌博相关信念(egrb)可以定义为信念,暗示未能认识到商业赌博活动是如何设计的,以产生保证的损失给玩家。在理论化egrb的发展过程中,以前的评论提出egrb是决策启发式和相关偏见的延伸。我们提出了另一种生成机制:赌博游戏通过解决问题和最终正确的战略行动使实质性的胜利看起来是可能的。egrb是对正确战略行动可能性的信念(控制幻觉),这种信念是在玩家尝试候选策略(基于各种更广泛的信念选择的策略)时形成的。我们进一步提出,egrb可以根据认知科学中关于人类对世界的一般信念类别的理论进行分类。例如,有理论认为,人类对超自然力量和随机性的信仰在不同文化中具有一定的相似性,因此我们提出存在一类超自然的egrb,以及一类基于对随机性本质的更广泛信仰的egrb。我们回顾了这种分类方案的证据,并讨论了如何将其应用于研究和治疗赌博障碍。
{"title":"Erroneous gambling-related beliefs emerge from broader beliefs during problem-solving: a critical review and classification scheme","authors":"Anastasia Ejova, K. Ohtsuka","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1590233","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1590233","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Erroneous gambling-related beliefs (EGRBs) can be defined as beliefs that imply a failure to recognise how commercial gambling activities are designed to generate a guaranteed loss to players. In theorising about how EGRBs develop, previous reviews have proposed that EGRBs are extensions of decision-making heuristics and associated biases. We propose an alternative generative mechanism: one in which gambling games make substantial wins seem possible through problem-solving and eventual correct strategic action. EGRBs are then beliefs in the possibility of correct strategic action (illusions of control) that develop as players trial candidate strategies—strategies selected based on various broader beliefs. We further propose that EGRBs can be classified based on what is theorised in cognitive science about categories of general human beliefs about the world. For example, it has been theorised that human beliefs about supernatural forces and randomness have certain similarities across cultures, and so we propose that there exists a category of supernatural EGRBs, as well as a category of EGRBs based on broader beliefs about the nature of randomness. We review evidence for this classification scheme and discuss how it can be applied in researching and treating gambling disorder.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"12 1","pages":"159 - 187"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78435108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
Ideological belief bias with political syllogisms 意识形态信仰偏向政治三段论
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-04-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1688188
D. Calvillo, Alexander B. Swan, Abraham M. Rutchick
Abstract The belief bias in reasoning occurs when individuals are more willing to accept conclusions that are consistent with their beliefs than conclusions that are inconsistent. The present study examined a belief bias in syllogisms containing political content. In two experiments, participants judged whether conclusions were valid, completed political ideology measures, and completed a cognitive reflection test. The conclusions varied in validity and in their political ideology (conservative or liberal). Participants were sensitive to syllogisms’ validity and conservatism. Overall, they showed a liberal bias, accepting more liberal than conservative conclusions. Furthermore, conservative participants accepted more conservative conclusions than liberal conclusions, whereas liberal participants showed the opposite pattern. Cognitive reflection did not magnify this effect as predicted by a motivated system 2 reasoning account of motivated ideological reasoning. These results suggest that people with different ideologies may accept different conclusions from the same evidence.
当个体更愿意接受与自己的信念一致的结论,而不是不一致的结论时,就会出现推理中的信念偏差。本研究考察了包含政治内容的三段论中的信念偏差。在两个实验中,参与者判断结论是否有效,完成政治意识形态测试,并完成认知反射测试。这些结论在有效性和政治意识形态(保守派或自由派)上各不相同。参与者对三段论的有效性和保守性敏感。总的来说,他们表现出自由主义的偏见,接受更自由的结论而不是保守的结论。此外,保守的参与者比自由的参与者更容易接受保守的结论,而自由的参与者则表现出相反的模式。认知反射并没有像被动机系统2推理对被动机意识形态推理的解释所预测的那样放大这种效应。这些结果表明,不同意识形态的人可能会从相同的证据中接受不同的结论。
{"title":"Ideological belief bias with political syllogisms","authors":"D. Calvillo, Alexander B. Swan, Abraham M. Rutchick","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1688188","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1688188","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The belief bias in reasoning occurs when individuals are more willing to accept conclusions that are consistent with their beliefs than conclusions that are inconsistent. The present study examined a belief bias in syllogisms containing political content. In two experiments, participants judged whether conclusions were valid, completed political ideology measures, and completed a cognitive reflection test. The conclusions varied in validity and in their political ideology (conservative or liberal). Participants were sensitive to syllogisms’ validity and conservatism. Overall, they showed a liberal bias, accepting more liberal than conservative conclusions. Furthermore, conservative participants accepted more conservative conclusions than liberal conclusions, whereas liberal participants showed the opposite pattern. Cognitive reflection did not magnify this effect as predicted by a motivated system 2 reasoning account of motivated ideological reasoning. These results suggest that people with different ideologies may accept different conclusions from the same evidence.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"34 1","pages":"291 - 310"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81054277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
A comparison of information processing and dynamical systems perspectives on problem solving 信息处理与动态系统在问题解决方面的比较
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-04-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1605930
Stephen K. Reed, Robin R. Vallacher
Abstract This article compares the information processing and dynamical systems perspectives on problem solving. Key theoretical constructs of the information-processing perspective include “searching” a “problem space” by using “heuristics” that produce “incremental” changes such as reaching a “subgoal” to solve a puzzle. Key theoretical constructs of the dynamical-systems perspective include “positive attractors”, “negative attractors”, and “latent attractors” that can cause large “nonincremental” changes in the possibility of a solution through the “emergence” of new ideas and beliefs that can resolve a conflict. The proposed alignment maps dynamical-system constructs to information-processing constructs: state space to problem space, negative attractor to impasse, positive attractor to productive subgoal, latent attractor to implicit cognition, and nonincremental change to insight. The purpose of the mapping is to explore similarities and differences between these constructs. Research from cognitive and social psychology illustrates how using constructs from both perspectives is helpful. The concluding section on Future Directions recommends an agenda based on three objectives: (1) create ontologies to organise current knowledge, (2) conduct research to obtain new knowledge, and (3) provide education to inform students about this knowledge.
摘要本文比较了信息处理和动力系统两种解决问题的方法。信息处理视角的关键理论结构包括通过使用“启发式”来“搜索”一个“问题空间”,从而产生“增量”变化,比如达到一个“子目标”来解决一个难题。动态系统视角的关键理论结构包括“积极吸引子”、“消极吸引子”和“潜在吸引子”,它们可以通过解决冲突的新想法和信念的“出现”,在解决方案的可能性中引起巨大的“非增量”变化。提出的对齐将动态系统结构映射到信息处理结构:状态空间映射到问题空间,消极吸引子映射到僵局,积极吸引子映射到生产性子目标,潜在吸引子映射到内隐认知,非增量变化映射到洞察力。映射的目的是探索这些构造之间的异同。认知心理学和社会心理学的研究表明,从这两个角度使用构念是有帮助的。关于未来方向的结语部分建议了一个基于三个目标的议程:(1)创建本体来组织当前知识,(2)进行研究以获得新知识,(3)提供教育以告知学生这些知识。
{"title":"A comparison of information processing and dynamical systems perspectives on problem solving","authors":"Stephen K. Reed, Robin R. Vallacher","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1605930","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1605930","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article compares the information processing and dynamical systems perspectives on problem solving. Key theoretical constructs of the information-processing perspective include “searching” a “problem space” by using “heuristics” that produce “incremental” changes such as reaching a “subgoal” to solve a puzzle. Key theoretical constructs of the dynamical-systems perspective include “positive attractors”, “negative attractors”, and “latent attractors” that can cause large “nonincremental” changes in the possibility of a solution through the “emergence” of new ideas and beliefs that can resolve a conflict. The proposed alignment maps dynamical-system constructs to information-processing constructs: state space to problem space, negative attractor to impasse, positive attractor to productive subgoal, latent attractor to implicit cognition, and nonincremental change to insight. The purpose of the mapping is to explore similarities and differences between these constructs. Research from cognitive and social psychology illustrates how using constructs from both perspectives is helpful. The concluding section on Future Directions recommends an agenda based on three objectives: (1) create ontologies to organise current knowledge, (2) conduct research to obtain new knowledge, and (3) provide education to inform students about this knowledge.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"8 1","pages":"254 - 290"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82971158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
Thinking & Reasoning
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1