首页 > 最新文献

Applied Psycholinguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Bilingual oral language development among dual language immersion students: Use of a Bayesian approach with language learning progressions 双语沉浸式学生的双语口语发展:贝叶斯方法与语言学习进展的应用
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000067
Ève Ryan, Preston Botter, Michelle L. Luna, Alison L. Bailey, Clémence Darriet
The dual language development of dual language immersion (DLI) students, although often examined at the domain level (e.g., listening or reading), remains understudied for more specific skills (e.g., word, sentence, or discourse). This study examines the eleven-month progression of oral language skills in a picture description task in two languages (French and English) for early-elementary (Transitional Kindergarten through first grade) DLI students (N = 42). Using Bayesian methods, which estimate parameters using both the data and prior information, we describe French and English growth patterns as measured by learning progressions whose focus is on language features at the word, sentence, and discourse levels. For French oral language, we found evidence of meaningful positive linear growth for all language features, whereas for English oral language, meaningful linear positive growth was only detected for sophistication of topic vocabulary. Overall, coming from a French-speaking household was associated with steeper French oral language trajectories, but coming from an English-only household did not specifically impact English oral language trajectories. In both languages, grade level influenced the trajectories of some—but not all—features. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications, advocating for a language progression approach in instruction and research on bilingualism.
浸入式双语教学(DLI)学生的双语发展虽然经常在领域层面(如听力或阅读)进行研究,但对更具体的技能(如单词、句子或话语)的研究仍然不足。本研究以两种语言(法语和英语)对小学低年级(幼儿园过渡班至一年级)DLI 学生(42 人)进行图片描述任务,考察其口语技能在 11 个月内的进步情况。我们采用贝叶斯方法(利用数据和先验信息估计参数),描述了法语和英语的成长模式,这些模式是通过学习进度来衡量的,其重点是单词、句子和话语层面的语言特点。就法语口语而言,我们发现所有语言特征都出现了有意义的线性正增长,而就英语口语而言,只有主题词汇的复杂性出现了有意义的线性正增长。总体而言,来自讲法语的家庭与更陡峭的法语口语发展轨迹有关,但来自纯英语家庭对英语口语发展轨迹没有具体影响。在两种语言中,年级水平都会影响部分(而非全部)特征的发展轨迹。最后,我们提出了理论和实践意义,主张在双语教学和研究中采用语言渐进方法。
{"title":"Bilingual oral language development among dual language immersion students: Use of a Bayesian approach with language learning progressions","authors":"Ève Ryan, Preston Botter, Michelle L. Luna, Alison L. Bailey, Clémence Darriet","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000067","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000067","url":null,"abstract":"The dual language development of dual language immersion (DLI) students, although often examined at the domain level (e.g., listening or reading), remains understudied for more specific skills (e.g., word, sentence, or discourse). This study examines the eleven-month progression of oral language skills in a picture description task in two languages (French and English) for early-elementary (Transitional Kindergarten through first grade) DLI students (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 42). Using Bayesian methods, which estimate parameters using both the data and prior information, we describe French and English growth patterns as measured by learning progressions whose focus is on language features at the word, sentence, and discourse levels. For French oral language, we found evidence of meaningful positive linear growth for all language features, whereas for English oral language, meaningful linear positive growth was only detected for sophistication of topic vocabulary. Overall, coming from a French-speaking household was associated with steeper French oral language trajectories, but coming from an English-only household did not specifically impact English oral language trajectories. In both languages, grade level influenced the trajectories of some—but not all—features. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications, advocating for a language progression approach in instruction and research on bilingualism.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140587682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How well do schoolchildren and adolescents know the form and meaning of different derivational suffixes? Evidence from a cross-sectional study 学龄儿童和青少年对不同派生后缀的形式和含义了解多少?一项横断面研究提供的证据
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000043
Dalia Martinez, Danielle Colenbrander, Tomohiro Inoue, George K. Georgiou
As children advance through school, derived words become increasingly common in their reading materials. Previous studies have shown that children’s knowledge of derivational morphology develops relatively slowly, but there is more to learn about this development. This study examined differences in knowledge of the form and meaning of suffixes across grade levels (Grades 3, 5, and 8) and different types of derivational suffixes (adjectives and nominals). We assessed 309 English-speaking children on word reading and receptive vocabulary tests and two tasks designed to assess the form (orthographic knowledge) and meaning (semantic knowledge) of 28 derivational suffixes (14 adjectives and 14 nominals). Overall, our findings showed a significant improvement in identifying and understanding derivational suffixes from Grade 3 to Grade 5 and a smaller, but still significant, improvement from Grade 5 to Grade 8. Our findings regarding suffix types were mixed. While written forms of adjectives were identified more accurately than nominals across all grades, this advantage did not extend to the students’ understanding of the meaning of the suffixes. These results highlight the distinction between the identification of suffixes and the understanding of their meaning. We discuss our results in relation to suffix frequency in children’s reading materials.
随着儿童学龄的增长,派生词在他们的阅读材料中变得越来越常见。以往的研究表明,儿童对派生词词形的认识发展相对缓慢,但这种发展还有待进一步了解。本研究考察了不同年级(3 年级、5 年级和 8 年级)和不同类型派生后缀(形容词和名词)对后缀形式和意义的认知差异。我们对 309 名英语儿童进行了单词阅读和接受性词汇测试,以及两项旨在评估 28 个派生后缀(14 个形容词和 14 个名词)的形式(正字法知识)和意义(语义知识)的任务。总体而言,我们的研究结果表明,从三年级到五年级,学生在识别和理解派生后缀方面有了显著的进步,而从五年级到八年级,学生的进步虽然较小,但仍然很明显。我们对后缀类型的研究结果喜忧参半。虽然在所有年级中,形容词的书面形式都比名词识别得更准确,但这种优势并没有延伸到学生对后缀意义的理解上。这些结果突出了识别后缀和理解其含义之间的区别。我们将结合儿童阅读材料中的后缀频率来讨论我们的结果。
{"title":"How well do schoolchildren and adolescents know the form and meaning of different derivational suffixes? Evidence from a cross-sectional study","authors":"Dalia Martinez, Danielle Colenbrander, Tomohiro Inoue, George K. Georgiou","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000043","url":null,"abstract":"As children advance through school, derived words become increasingly common in their reading materials. Previous studies have shown that children’s knowledge of derivational morphology develops relatively slowly, but there is more to learn about this development. This study examined differences in knowledge of the form and meaning of suffixes across grade levels (Grades 3, 5, and 8) and different types of derivational suffixes (adjectives and nominals). We assessed 309 English-speaking children on word reading and receptive vocabulary tests and two tasks designed to assess the form (orthographic knowledge) and meaning (semantic knowledge) of 28 derivational suffixes (14 adjectives and 14 nominals). Overall, our findings showed a significant improvement in identifying and understanding derivational suffixes from Grade 3 to Grade 5 and a smaller, but still significant, improvement from Grade 5 to Grade 8. Our findings regarding suffix types were mixed. While written forms of adjectives were identified more accurately than nominals across all grades, this advantage did not extend to the students’ understanding of the meaning of the suffixes. These results highlight the distinction between the identification of suffixes and the understanding of their meaning. We discuss our results in relation to suffix frequency in children’s reading materials.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140587686","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Audio-visual Stroop matching task with first- and second-language color words and color associates 用第一语言和第二语言颜色词与颜色联想进行视听 Stroop 匹配任务
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000055
Iva Šaban, James R. Schmidt
In the audio-visual Stroop matching task, participants compare one Stroop stimulus dimension (e.g., the color of a written word) to a second stimulus (e.g., a spoken word) and indicate whether these two stimuli match or mismatch. Slower responses on certain trials can be due to conflict which occurs between color representations (semantic conflict) or due to conflict between responses evoked by task comparisons (response conflict). The contribution of these conflicts has been investigated with color word distracters. This is the first study which explores how two types of first- and second-language words affect audio-visual matching. Native French speakers performed a bilingual Stroop matching task with intermixed French (L1) and English (L2) color words (Experiment 1) and color associates (Experiment 2) presented in congruent and incongruent colors simultaneously with spoken French color words. Participants were instructed to indicate whether the spoken word “matches” or “mismatches” the font color, while ignoring written word meaning. Interestingly, the results were similar for the critical “mismatch” trials for both French and English words. The responses were the fastest on trials in which task comparisons activate fewer response alternatives, supporting the assumption of the response conflict account.
在视听 Stroop 匹配任务中,受试者将一个 Stroop 刺激维度(如一个书面单词的颜色)与第二个刺激(如一个口头单词)进行比较,并指出这两个刺激是匹配还是不匹配。某些试验中反应较慢的原因可能是颜色表征之间发生了冲突(语义冲突),也可能是任务比较引起的反应之间发生了冲突(反应冲突)。这些冲突的作用已通过颜色单词干扰物进行了研究。本研究首次探讨了两种第一语言和第二语言单词如何影响视听匹配。以法语为母语的受试者在进行双语 Stroop 匹配任务时,会在法语(第一语言)和英语(第二语言)颜色词(实验 1)和颜色联想(实验 2)中同时出现同色和不同色法语颜色词。参与者被要求指出口语单词与字体颜色是 "匹配 "还是 "不匹配",而忽略书面单词的含义。有趣的是,在关键的 "不匹配 "试验中,法语和英语单词的结果相似。在任务比较激活较少反应选择的试验中,受试者的反应速度最快,这支持了反应冲突理论的假设。
{"title":"Audio-visual Stroop matching task with first- and second-language color words and color associates","authors":"Iva Šaban, James R. Schmidt","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000055","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000055","url":null,"abstract":"In the audio-visual Stroop matching task, participants compare one Stroop stimulus dimension (e.g., the color of a written word) to a second stimulus (e.g., a spoken word) and indicate whether these two stimuli match or mismatch. Slower responses on certain trials can be due to conflict which occurs between color representations (<jats:italic>semantic conflict</jats:italic>) or due to conflict between responses evoked by task comparisons (<jats:italic>response conflict</jats:italic>). The contribution of these conflicts has been investigated with color word distracters. This is the first study which explores how two types of first- and second-language words affect audio-visual matching. Native French speakers performed a bilingual Stroop matching task with intermixed French (L1) and English (L2) color words (Experiment 1) and color associates (Experiment 2) presented in congruent and incongruent colors simultaneously with spoken French color words. Participants were instructed to indicate whether the spoken word “matches” or “mismatches” the font color, while ignoring written word meaning. Interestingly, the results were similar for the critical “mismatch” trials for both French and English words. The responses were the fastest on trials in which task comparisons activate fewer response alternatives, supporting the assumption of the response conflict account.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140587669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of speech production training on memory across short and long delays in 5- and 6-year-olds: A pre-registered study 语音制作训练对 5 岁和 6 岁儿童短时和长时延迟记忆的影响:预注册研究
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000109
Belén López Assef, Margarethe McDonald, Amélie Bernard, Tania S. Zamuner

Studies on the role of speech production on learning have found a memory benefit from production labeled the “Production Effect.” While research with adults has generally shown a robust memory advantage for produced words, children show more mixed results, and the advantage is affected by age, cognitive, and linguistic factors. With adults, the Production Effect is not restricted to the immediate context but is also found after a delay. So far, no studies have investigated the effect of delayed recall on the Production Effect with children. Children aged 5 and 6 years old (n = 60) participated in two sessions. Children were trained on familiar words and images, which were heard (Listen) or produced aloud (Say). Children then performed a free recall task. One week later, children repeated the recall task and an additional recognition task. At immediate testing, there was a recency effect on words recalled from the different training conditions and a recall advantage for words produced over words heard; however, this no longer held after a 1-week delay in either the recall or recognition task. Exploratory analysis showed that vocabulary did not predict the Production Effect. Findings indicate that unlike adults, the Production Effect is not as robust in children after a delay.

关于语音生成对学习的作用的研究发现,语音生成会给记忆带来好处,这种好处被称为 "生成效应"。对成人的研究普遍表明,发音产生的单词具有强大的记忆优势,而对儿童的研究则结果不一,而且这种优势受到年龄、认知和语言因素的影响。对于成人来说,生词效应并不局限于即时语境,在延迟后也会出现。迄今为止,还没有研究调查过儿童延迟回忆对生产效应的影响。5 岁和 6 岁的儿童(n = 60)参加了两次训练。儿童先接受熟悉单词和图像的训练,然后听(听)或大声说(说)。然后,儿童进行自由回忆任务。一周后,儿童重复进行回忆任务和额外的识别任务。在即时测试中,从不同的训练条件中回忆出的单词存在回忆效应,而说出的单词比听到的单词具有回忆优势;但是,在回忆或识别任务延迟一周后,这种优势就不再存在了。探索性分析表明,词汇量并不能预测生产效应。研究结果表明,与成人不同,儿童的生产效应在延迟后并不那么强烈。
{"title":"Effects of speech production training on memory across short and long delays in 5- and 6-year-olds: A pre-registered study","authors":"Belén López Assef, Margarethe McDonald, Amélie Bernard, Tania S. Zamuner","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000109","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000109","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies on the role of speech production on learning have found a memory benefit from production labeled the “Production Effect.” While research with adults has generally shown a robust memory advantage for produced words, children show more mixed results, and the advantage is affected by age, cognitive, and linguistic factors. With adults, the Production Effect is not restricted to the immediate context but is also found after a delay. So far, no studies have investigated the effect of delayed recall on the Production Effect with children. Children aged 5 and 6 years old (<span>n</span> = 60) participated in two sessions. Children were trained on familiar words and images, which were heard (<span>Listen</span>) or produced aloud (<span>Say</span>). Children then performed a free recall task. One week later, children repeated the recall task and an additional recognition task. At immediate testing, there was a recency effect on words recalled from the different training conditions and a recall advantage for words produced over words heard; however, this no longer held after a 1-week delay in either the recall or recognition task. Exploratory analysis showed that vocabulary did not predict the Production Effect. Findings indicate that unlike adults, the Production Effect is not as robust in children after a delay.</p>","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140587684","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phonology, homophony, and eyes-closed rest in Mandarin novel word learning: An eye-tracking study in adult native and non-native speakers 普通话新词学习中的语音、同音和闭眼休息:对母语和非母语成人的眼动跟踪研究
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000031
Wenfu Bao, Anja Arnhold, Juhani Järvikivi
This study used the visual world paradigm to investigate novel word learning in adults from different language backgrounds and the effects of phonology, homophony, and rest on the outcome. We created Mandarin novel words varied by types of phonological contrasts and homophone status. During the experiment, native (n = 34) and non-native speakers (English; n = 30) learned pairs of novel words and were tested twice with a 15-minute break in between, which was spent either resting or gaming. In the post-break test of novel word recognition, an interaction appeared between language backgrounds, phonology, and homophony: non-native speakers performed less accurately than native speakers only on non-homophones learned in pairs with tone contrasts. Eye movement data indicated that non-native speakers’ processing of tones may be more effortful than their processing of segments while learning homophones, as demonstrated by the time course. Interestingly, no significant effects of rest were observed across language groups; yet after gaming, native speakers achieved higher accuracy than non-native speakers. Overall, this study suggests that Mandarin novel word learning can be affected by participants’ language backgrounds and phonological and homophonous features of words. However, the role of short periods of rest in novel word learning requires further investigation.
本研究采用视觉世界范式来研究不同语言背景的成年人的新词学习情况,以及语音、同音和休息对学习结果的影响。我们根据语音对比类型和同音状态创造了不同的普通话新词。在实验过程中,母语者(n = 34)和非母语者(英语;n = 30)学习了成对的新词,并接受了两次测试,中间有 15 分钟的休息时间,可以用来休息或游戏。在休息后进行的新词识别测试中,语言背景、语音和同音字之间出现了交互作用:非母语者仅在学习带有音调对比的非同音字时的准确率低于母语者。眼动数据表明,在学习同音字时,非母语者对音调的处理可能比对音段的处理更费力,这一点从时间进程中可以看出。有趣的是,在不同的语言组中,没有观察到休息的明显影响;但在游戏之后,母语者比非母语者获得了更高的准确性。总之,本研究表明,普通话新词学习会受到参与者的语言背景以及词的语音和同音特征的影响。然而,短时休息在新词学习中的作用还需要进一步研究。
{"title":"Phonology, homophony, and eyes-closed rest in Mandarin novel word learning: An eye-tracking study in adult native and non-native speakers","authors":"Wenfu Bao, Anja Arnhold, Juhani Järvikivi","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000031","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000031","url":null,"abstract":"This study used the visual world paradigm to investigate novel word learning in adults from different language backgrounds and the effects of phonology, homophony, and rest on the outcome. We created Mandarin novel words varied by types of phonological contrasts and homophone status. During the experiment, native (<jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 34) and non-native speakers (English; <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 30) learned pairs of novel words and were tested twice with a 15-minute break in between, which was spent either resting or gaming. In the post-break test of novel word recognition, an interaction appeared between language backgrounds, phonology, and homophony: non-native speakers performed less accurately than native speakers only on non-homophones learned in pairs with tone contrasts. Eye movement data indicated that non-native speakers’ processing of tones may be more effortful than their processing of segments while learning homophones, as demonstrated by the time course. Interestingly, no significant effects of rest were observed across language groups; yet after gaming, native speakers achieved higher accuracy than non-native speakers. Overall, this study suggests that Mandarin novel word learning can be affected by participants’ language backgrounds and phonological and homophonous features of words. However, the role of short periods of rest in novel word learning requires further investigation.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140037655","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of postverbal subjects in L2 Italian: A multifactorial corpus analysis 第二语言意大利语中后动词主语的发展:多因素语料分析
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1017/s014271642400002x
Andrea Listanti, Jacopo Torregrossa
Most studies on the acquisition of postverbal subjects (VS) in L2 Italian focus on a limited number of linguistic factors that tend to be associated with the production of VS in L1 (e.g., verb class and subject discourse status). Moreover, they analyze homogeneous groups of learners in terms of proficiency, mostly through controlled experiments. In this paper, we present a cross-sectional corpus study based on a multifactorial analysis of the L2 use of VS structures in semi-spontaneous speech. We analyze the production of VSs by learners of different levels of proficiency (A1-C2), considering linguistic factors that trigger the production of VS in L1, but have been unaccounted for in L2 studies (e.g., agentivity of the subject, syntactic configuration of the sentence, contrastive focus). We use a cumulative link mixed model to show how the features of verbs and subjects in VS structures change across proficiency levels. The results indicate learners’ progressive mastery of the mechanisms of assignment of the subject function to the postverbal constituent and increasing sensitivity to contrastive focus as a feature relevant for the use of VS. Furthermore, we observe that psychological verbs associated with the use of VS are produced from the earliest stages of L2 acquisition.
大多数关于意大利语第二语言后置动词(VS)习得的研究都集中在有限的语言因素上,这些因素往往与第一语言中VS的产生有关(如动词类别和主语话语地位)。此外,这些研究主要通过对照实验对同质的学习者群体进行熟练程度分析。在本文中,我们基于对 L2 在半自发言语中使用 VS 结构的多因素分析,介绍了一项横断面语料库研究。我们分析了不同水平(A1-C2)学习者的 VS 生成情况,考虑了在 L1 中引发 VS 生成,但在 L2 研究中却没有考虑到的语言因素(如主语的代理性、句子的句法结构、对比焦点)。我们使用累积链接混合模型来说明 VS 结构中动词和主语的特征在不同水平的学习者中是如何变化的。结果表明,学习者逐渐掌握了将主语功能分配给后置动词成分的机制,并对作为 VS 使用相关特征的对比焦点越来越敏感。此外,我们还观察到,与使用 VS 相关的心理动词是在学习 L2 的最初阶段产生的。
{"title":"The development of postverbal subjects in L2 Italian: A multifactorial corpus analysis","authors":"Andrea Listanti, Jacopo Torregrossa","doi":"10.1017/s014271642400002x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s014271642400002x","url":null,"abstract":"Most studies on the acquisition of postverbal subjects (VS) in L2 Italian focus on a limited number of linguistic factors that tend to be associated with the production of VS in L1 (e.g., verb class and subject discourse status). Moreover, they analyze homogeneous groups of learners in terms of proficiency, mostly through controlled experiments. In this paper, we present a cross-sectional corpus study based on a multifactorial analysis of the L2 use of VS structures in semi-spontaneous speech. We analyze the production of VSs by learners of different levels of proficiency (A1-C2), considering linguistic factors that trigger the production of VS in L1, but have been unaccounted for in L2 studies (e.g., agentivity of the subject, syntactic configuration of the sentence, contrastive focus). We use a cumulative link mixed model to show how the features of verbs and subjects in VS structures change across proficiency levels. The results indicate learners’ progressive mastery of the mechanisms of assignment of the subject function to the postverbal constituent and increasing sensitivity to contrastive focus as a feature relevant for the use of VS. Furthermore, we observe that psychological verbs associated with the use of VS are produced from the earliest stages of L2 acquisition.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139769823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Affective and sensory–motor norms for idioms by L1 and L2 English speakers 第一语言和第二语言英语使用者对习语的情感和感觉运动规范
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716423000504
Mahsa Morid, Laura Sabourin
In the present study, we developed affective (valence and arousal) and sensory–motor (concreteness and imageability) norms for 210 English idioms rated by native English speakers (L1) and English second-language speakers (L2). Based on internal consistency analyses, the ratings were found to be highly reliable. Furthermore, we explored various relations within the collected measures (valence, arousal, concreteness, and imageability) and between these measures and some available psycholinguistic norms (familiarity, literal plausibility, and decomposability) for the same set of idioms. The primary findings were that (i) valence and arousal showed the typical U-shape relation, for both L1 and L2 data; (ii) idioms with more negative valence were rated as more arousing; (iii) the majority of idioms were rated as either positive or negative with only 4 being rated as neutral; (iv) familiarity correlated positively with valence and arousal; (v) concreteness and imageability showed a strong positive correlation; and (vi) the ratings of L1 and L2 speakers significantly differed for arousal and concreteness, but not for valence and imageability. We discuss our interpretation of these observations with reference to the literature on figurative language processing (both single words and idioms).
在本研究中,我们为以英语为母语者(L1)和以英语为第二语言者(L2)评定的 210 个英语习语制定了情感(价值和唤醒)和感觉运动(具体性和形象性)规范。根据内部一致性分析,我们发现这些评分非常可靠。此外,我们还探讨了所收集的测量指标(情感、唤醒、具体性和形象性)之间的各种关系,以及这些测量指标与一些心理语言学规范(熟悉度、字面似然性和可分解性)之间的关系。主要的研究结果是:(i) 在 L1 和 L2 数据中,价值和唤醒呈现典型的 U 型关系;(ii) 具有更消极价值的成语被评为更唤醒;(iii) 大多数成语被评为积极或消极,只有 4 个成语被评为中性;(iv) 熟悉程度与价值性和唤醒性呈正相关;(v) 具体性和形象性呈强正相关;(vi) L1 和 L2 说话者对唤醒性和具体性的评价有显著差异,但对价值性和形象性的评价没有显著差异。我们将参考有关具象语言处理(包括单词和成语)的文献,讨论我们对这些观察结果的解释。
{"title":"Affective and sensory–motor norms for idioms by L1 and L2 English speakers","authors":"Mahsa Morid, Laura Sabourin","doi":"10.1017/s0142716423000504","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716423000504","url":null,"abstract":"In the present study, we developed affective (valence and arousal) and sensory–motor (concreteness and imageability) norms for 210 English idioms rated by native English speakers (L1) and English second-language speakers (L2). Based on internal consistency analyses, the ratings were found to be highly reliable. Furthermore, we explored various relations within the collected measures (valence, arousal, concreteness, and imageability) and between these measures and some available psycholinguistic norms (familiarity, literal plausibility, and decomposability) for the same set of idioms. The primary findings were that (i) valence and arousal showed the typical U-shape relation, for both L1 and L2 data; (ii) idioms with more negative valence were rated as more arousing; (iii) the majority of idioms were rated as either positive or negative with only 4 being rated as neutral; (iv) familiarity correlated positively with valence and arousal; (v) concreteness and imageability showed a strong positive correlation; and (vi) the ratings of L1 and L2 speakers significantly differed for arousal and concreteness, but not for valence and imageability. We discuss our interpretation of these observations with reference to the literature on figurative language processing (both single words and idioms).","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139677965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the glottal stop as a mark of gender-inclusive language in German 研究作为德语中性别包容性语言标志的喉塞音
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716424000018
Anita Körner, Sarah Glim, Ralf Rummer
Grammatical gender form influences readers’ mental gender representations. Previous research demonstrates that the generic masculine form leads to male-biased representations, while some alternative forms lead to female-biased representations. The present research examines the recently introduced glottal stop form in spoken language in German, where a glottal stop (similar to a short pause), meant to represent all gender identities, is inserted before the gender-specific ending. In two experiments (total N = 1188), participants listened to sentences in the glottal stop, the generic masculine, or the generic feminine form and classified whether a second sentence about women or men was a sensible continuation. The generic feminine and the glottal stop led to female biases (fewer errors in sentences about women vs. men) and the generic masculine led to a male bias. The biases were smaller for the glottal stop and the generic masculine than for the generic feminine, indicating that the former two are more readily understood as representing both women and men.
语法性别形式会影响读者的心理性别表征。以往的研究表明,通用的男性形式会导致男性偏向的表征,而一些替代形式则会导致女性偏向的表征。本研究考察了最近在德语口语中引入的喉塞音形式,即在特定性别结尾之前插入一个喉塞音(类似于短暂停顿),意在代表所有性别身份。在两项实验中(共 1188 人),受试者聆听了喉塞音、一般男性或一般女性形式的句子,并对第二个关于女性或男性的句子是否是合理的延续进行了分类。一般阴性和喉塞音导致了女性偏向(在有关女性的句子中错误相对于男性较少),而一般阳性则导致了男性偏向。喉塞音和通用男性比通用女性的偏差更小,这表明前两者更容易被理解为同时代表女性和男性。
{"title":"Examining the glottal stop as a mark of gender-inclusive language in German","authors":"Anita Körner, Sarah Glim, Ralf Rummer","doi":"10.1017/s0142716424000018","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716424000018","url":null,"abstract":"Grammatical gender form influences readers’ mental gender representations. Previous research demonstrates that the generic masculine form leads to male-biased representations, while some alternative forms lead to female-biased representations. The present research examines the recently introduced glottal stop form in spoken language in German, where a glottal stop (similar to a short pause), meant to represent all gender identities, is inserted before the gender-specific ending. In two experiments (total <jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 1188), participants listened to sentences in the glottal stop, the generic masculine, or the generic feminine form and classified whether a second sentence about women or men was a sensible continuation. The generic feminine and the glottal stop led to female biases (fewer errors in sentences about women vs. men) and the generic masculine led to a male bias. The biases were smaller for the glottal stop and the generic masculine than for the generic feminine, indicating that the former two are more readily understood as representing both women and men.","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139677552","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of verb surprisal on the acquisition of second language syntactic structures in adults: An artificial language learning study 动词惊奇对成人学习第二语言句法结构的影响:人工语言学习研究
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716423000334
Giulia Bovolenta, Emma Marsden

Inverse probability adaptation effects (the finding that encountering a verb in an unexpected structure increases long-term priming for that structure) have been observed in both L1 and L2 speakers. However, participants in these studies all had established representations of the syntactic structures to be primed. It therefore remains an open question whether inverse probability adaptation effects could take place with newly encountered L2 structures. In a pre-registered experiment, we exposed participants (n = 84) to an artificial language with active and passive constructions. Training on Day 1 established expectations for specific co-occurrence patterns between verbs and structures. On Day 2, established patterns were violated for the surprisal group (n = 42), but not for the control group (n = 42). We observed no immediate priming effects from exposure to high-surprisal items. On Day 3, however, we observed an effect of input variation on comprehension of verb meaning in an auditory grammaticality judgment task. The surprisal group showed higher accuracy for passive structures in both tasks, suggesting that experiencing variation during learning had promoted the recognition of optionality in the target language.

在 L1 和 L2 说话者中都观察到了反向概率适应效应(在意外结构中遇到动词会增加对该结构的长期引物)。然而,这些研究的参与者都已经建立了被引诱的句法结构的表征。因此,新遇到的 L2 结构是否会产生反向概率适应效应仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。在一项预先登记的实验中,我们让参与者(n = 84)接触一种包含主动和被动结构的人工语言。第一天的训练建立了对动词和结构之间特定共现模式的预期。第 2 天,出其不意组(n = 42)违反了既定模式,而对照组(n = 42)则没有。我们没有观察到暴露于高惊奇项目的直接引物效应。然而,在第三天,我们在听觉语法判断任务中观察到了输入变化对动词意义理解的影响。在这两项任务中,惊奇组对被动结构的判断准确率更高,这表明在学习过程中经历的变异促进了对目标语言中可选择性的识别。
{"title":"The effect of verb surprisal on the acquisition of second language syntactic structures in adults: An artificial language learning study","authors":"Giulia Bovolenta, Emma Marsden","doi":"10.1017/s0142716423000334","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716423000334","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Inverse probability adaptation effects (the finding that encountering a verb in an unexpected structure increases long-term priming for that structure) have been observed in both L1 and L2 speakers. However, participants in these studies all had established representations of the syntactic structures to be primed. It therefore remains an open question whether inverse probability adaptation effects could take place with newly encountered L2 structures. In a pre-registered experiment, we exposed participants (<span>n</span> = 84) to an artificial language with active and passive constructions. Training on Day 1 established expectations for specific co-occurrence patterns between verbs and structures. On Day 2, established patterns were violated for the surprisal group (<span>n =</span> 42), but not for the control group (<span>n</span> = 42). We observed no immediate priming effects from exposure to high-surprisal items. On Day 3, however, we observed an effect of input variation on comprehension of verb meaning in an auditory grammaticality judgment task. The surprisal group showed higher accuracy for passive structures in both tasks, suggesting that experiencing variation during learning had promoted the recognition of optionality in the target language.</p>","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138826089","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Subject-verb dependency formation and semantic interference in native and non-native language comprehension 母语和非母语理解中的主谓依存关系形成和语义干扰
IF 2.1 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716423000498
Hiroki Fujita, Ian Cunnings

Differences between native (L1) and non-native (L2) comprehension have been debated. This study explores whether a source of potential L1/L2 differences lies in susceptibility to memory-based interference during dependency formation. Interference effects are known to occur in sentences like The key to the cabinets were rusty, where ungrammaticality results from a number mismatch between the sentence subject and verb. Such sentences are sometimes misperceived as grammatical due to the presence of a number-matching “distractor” (“the cabinets”). Interference has been well-examined in a number agreement. However, whether and how forming thematic relations is susceptible to interference remains underexplored in L1 and L2 language comprehension. In six preregistered experiments, we investigated semantic interference in language comprehension and explored whether potential L1/L2 differences can be attributed to different degrees of susceptibility to interference. The results did not show that L2 speakers are more susceptible to interference than L1 speakers. Also, the observed interference patterns were only partially consistent with existing theories of memory retrieval during comprehension. We discuss how these theories may be reconciled with our findings and argue our results suggest that similar processes are involved in L1 and L2 subject-verb dependency formation.

母语(L1)和非母语(L2)理解能力之间的差异一直备受争议。本研究探讨了 L1/L2 差异的潜在根源是否在于依赖关系形成过程中对基于记忆的干扰的易感性。众所周知,干扰效应会出现在 The key to the cabinets were rusty 这样的句子中,句子主语和动词之间的数量不匹配会导致句子不合语法。由于存在一个数字匹配的 "干扰项"("the cabinets"),这类句子有时会被误认为是符合语法的。干扰因素已经在数字一致中得到了很好的研究。然而,在 L1 和 L2 语言理解中,形成主题关系是否会受到干扰以及如何受到干扰的研究仍然不足。在六个预先登记的实验中,我们研究了语言理解中的语义干扰,并探讨了 L1/L2 的潜在差异是否可归因于不同程度的易受干扰性。结果并未显示 L2 说话者比 L1 说话者更容易受到干扰。此外,观察到的干扰模式与理解过程中记忆检索的现有理论只有部分一致。我们讨论了如何将这些理论与我们的研究结果相协调,并认为我们的研究结果表明,L1 和 L2 主谓依存关系的形成过程相似。
{"title":"Subject-verb dependency formation and semantic interference in native and non-native language comprehension","authors":"Hiroki Fujita, Ian Cunnings","doi":"10.1017/s0142716423000498","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716423000498","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Differences between native (L1) and non-native (L2) comprehension have been debated. This study explores whether a source of potential L1/L2 differences lies in susceptibility to memory-based interference during dependency formation. Interference effects are known to occur in sentences like <span>The key to the cabinets were rusty</span>, where ungrammaticality results from a number mismatch between the sentence subject and verb. Such sentences are sometimes misperceived as grammatical due to the presence of a number-matching “distractor” (“the cabinets”). Interference has been well-examined in a number agreement. However, whether and how forming thematic relations is susceptible to interference remains underexplored in L1 and L2 language comprehension. In six preregistered experiments, we investigated semantic interference in language comprehension and explored whether potential L1/L2 differences can be attributed to different degrees of susceptibility to interference. The results did not show that L2 speakers are more susceptible to interference than L1 speakers. Also, the observed interference patterns were only partially consistent with existing theories of memory retrieval during comprehension. We discuss how these theories may be reconciled with our findings and argue our results suggest that similar processes are involved in L1 and L2 subject-verb dependency formation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48065,"journal":{"name":"Applied Psycholinguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138715121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Psycholinguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1