首页 > 最新文献

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations最新文献

英文 中文
Inspiring visibility: Exploring the roles of identification and solidarity for alleviating Black women’s invisibility in politics 鼓舞人心的能见度:探索认同和团结在减轻黑人妇女在政治中的隐形作用
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-07-06 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221105426
Amanda E. Mosier, Evava S. Pietri, India R. Johnson
We explored whether a Black female politician would alleviate feelings of invisibility among Black women even when they believed the politician deviated from the ingroup prototype by not supporting ingroup interests or by being low in ingroup solidarity. Study 1 demonstrated that relative to Black men and White men and women, Black women identified the most with Vice-President Kamala Harris and reported feeling the highest invisibility in politics immediately after Harris exited the Democratic primary election, but did not report higher support for Harris’s political platform. Study 2 further showed that a Black female politician who supported a policy that is viewed as harmful to Black Americans still helped alleviate feelings of invisibility for Black women because they strongly identified with the politician. However, a Black female politician opposing this policy was the most beneficial, demonstrating the importance of both identification and solidarity for inspiring visibility.
我们探讨了黑人女性政治家是否会减轻黑人女性被忽视的感觉,即使她们认为政治家偏离了内部群体的原型,不支持内部群体的利益或内部群体的团结程度较低。研究1表明,相对于黑人男性和白人男性和女性,黑人女性最认同副总统卡玛拉·哈里斯(Kamala Harris),并在哈里斯退出民主党初选后立即感到自己在政治上最不受关注,但她们对哈里斯的政治纲领并没有更高的支持。研究2进一步表明,支持一项被认为对美国黑人有害的政策的黑人女政治家,仍然有助于减轻黑人妇女被忽视的感觉,因为她们强烈地认同这位政治家。然而,一位反对这项政策的黑人女性政治家是最有益的,这表明了认同和团结对于激发知名度的重要性。
{"title":"Inspiring visibility: Exploring the roles of identification and solidarity for alleviating Black women’s invisibility in politics","authors":"Amanda E. Mosier, Evava S. Pietri, India R. Johnson","doi":"10.1177/13684302221105426","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221105426","url":null,"abstract":"We explored whether a Black female politician would alleviate feelings of invisibility among Black women even when they believed the politician deviated from the ingroup prototype by not supporting ingroup interests or by being low in ingroup solidarity. Study 1 demonstrated that relative to Black men and White men and women, Black women identified the most with Vice-President Kamala Harris and reported feeling the highest invisibility in politics immediately after Harris exited the Democratic primary election, but did not report higher support for Harris’s political platform. Study 2 further showed that a Black female politician who supported a policy that is viewed as harmful to Black Americans still helped alleviate feelings of invisibility for Black women because they strongly identified with the politician. However, a Black female politician opposing this policy was the most beneficial, demonstrating the importance of both identification and solidarity for inspiring visibility.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1351 - 1367"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48558598","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The negative secondary transfer effect: Comparing proposed mediation theories 消极的二次转移效应:对中介理论的比较
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-07-06 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221105822
N. Henschel, C. Derksen
The secondary transfer effect proposes that contact with an outgroup impacts attitudes towards another, secondary outgroup. For positive contact, three pathways have been identified for the effect: attitude generalization, multiculturalism, and ingroup reappraisal (deprovincialization hypothesis, operationalized here as national pride). Research on negative secondary transfer effects is still scarce. Using data from a German nationally representative survey, we investigated negative secondary transfer effects from foreigners to refugees. The three pathways were compared while considering positive and refugee contact. Negative and positive secondary transfer effects both occurred (partially) mediated via attitude generalization and multiculturalism but not via national pride. We conclude there might be a risk of generalizing prejudice from unrelated negative experiences via these two mechanisms. Research on forced migration and intergroup contact should further explore them with the ultimate goal of preventing negative secondary transfer effects. Longitudinal or experimental research is needed to address causality, ideally involving various outgroups.
次要转移效应认为,与外群体的接触会影响对另一个次要外群体的态度。对于积极接触,已经确定了三条途径:态度概括、多元文化主义和群体内重新评估(去地方化假设,在这里作为民族自豪感运作)。对负二次转移效应的研究还比较少。利用德国全国代表性调查的数据,我们调查了外国人对难民的负二次转移效应。在考虑积极接触和难民接触的情况下,对这三种途径进行了比较。消极和积极的二次迁移效应都(部分地)通过态度概括和多元文化主义介导,但不通过民族自豪感介导。我们的结论是,通过这两种机制,可能存在从不相关的负面经历中泛化偏见的风险。对强迫迁移和群体间接触的研究应进一步探索,最终目的是防止负面的二次迁移效应。需要纵向或实验研究来解决因果关系,理想情况下包括各种外群体。
{"title":"The negative secondary transfer effect: Comparing proposed mediation theories","authors":"N. Henschel, C. Derksen","doi":"10.1177/13684302221105822","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221105822","url":null,"abstract":"The secondary transfer effect proposes that contact with an outgroup impacts attitudes towards another, secondary outgroup. For positive contact, three pathways have been identified for the effect: attitude generalization, multiculturalism, and ingroup reappraisal (deprovincialization hypothesis, operationalized here as national pride). Research on negative secondary transfer effects is still scarce. Using data from a German nationally representative survey, we investigated negative secondary transfer effects from foreigners to refugees. The three pathways were compared while considering positive and refugee contact. Negative and positive secondary transfer effects both occurred (partially) mediated via attitude generalization and multiculturalism but not via national pride. We conclude there might be a risk of generalizing prejudice from unrelated negative experiences via these two mechanisms. Research on forced migration and intergroup contact should further explore them with the ultimate goal of preventing negative secondary transfer effects. Longitudinal or experimental research is needed to address causality, ideally involving various outgroups.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1271 - 1288"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45944073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
People of Color are People of Action: Asian American Participation in Own-Group and African American-Oriented Collective Action 有色人种是行动的人:亚裔美国人参与自己的群体和以非裔美国人为导向的集体行动
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-27 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221103962
Adam Y. Kim, R. M. Lee
Racially marginalized communities are socially and politically active, yet there is limited work that examines the psychological forces underlying how People of Color engage in cross-racial solidarity and collective action. We propose a model of politicized racial identity and collective action to Asian American participation in own-group collective action and African American collective action. In Study 1, we tested the model using correlational data. In Study 2, we used an experiment to explore whether politicized identities predict collective action. Results support the relation between politicized identities and collective action. Politicized Person of Color identity predicted Asian American engagement in both own-group-oriented collective action (Study 2) and African American-oriented (Study 1, Study 2) collective action. Further, politicized Asian American identity predicted Asian American engagement in own-group collective action (Study 1). These findings provide empirical evidence for the role of politicized identities in predicting collective action, including cross-racial solidarity with African Americans.
种族边缘化社区在社会和政治上都很活跃,但研究有色人种如何参与跨种族团结和集体行动的心理力量的工作有限。我们提出了一种政治化的种族认同和集体行动模式,即亚裔美国人参与自己的集体行动和非裔美国人的集体行动。在研究1中,我们使用相关数据对模型进行了测试。在研究2中,我们使用了一个实验来探索政治化身份是否能预测集体行动。结果支持政治化身份与集体行动之间的关系。政治化的有色人种身份预测了亚裔美国人参与以自己的群体为导向的集体行动(研究2)和以非裔美国人为导向的(研究1,研究2)集体行动。此外,政治化的亚裔美国人身份预测了亚裔美国人参与自己的集体行动(研究1)。这些发现为政治化身份在预测集体行动中的作用提供了经验证据,包括与非裔美国人的跨种族团结。
{"title":"People of Color are People of Action: Asian American Participation in Own-Group and African American-Oriented Collective Action","authors":"Adam Y. Kim, R. M. Lee","doi":"10.1177/13684302221103962","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221103962","url":null,"abstract":"Racially marginalized communities are socially and politically active, yet there is limited work that examines the psychological forces underlying how People of Color engage in cross-racial solidarity and collective action. We propose a model of politicized racial identity and collective action to Asian American participation in own-group collective action and African American collective action. In Study 1, we tested the model using correlational data. In Study 2, we used an experiment to explore whether politicized identities predict collective action. Results support the relation between politicized identities and collective action. Politicized Person of Color identity predicted Asian American engagement in both own-group-oriented collective action (Study 2) and African American-oriented (Study 1, Study 2) collective action. Further, politicized Asian American identity predicted Asian American engagement in own-group collective action (Study 1). These findings provide empirical evidence for the role of politicized identities in predicting collective action, including cross-racial solidarity with African Americans.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1368 - 1388"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45873050","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sequential models of intergroup contact and social categorization: An experimental field test of integrated models 群体间接触和社会分类的序列模型:综合模型的实验场检验
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-27 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221104921
L. Vezzali, E. Trifiletti, Ralf Wölfer, G. A. di Bernardo, S. Stathi, V. Cocco, A. Cadamuro, S. E. Shamloo, M. Hewstone
Research has proposed different models of how contact situations should be structured to maximize contact effects, focusing in particular on the role of categorization during contact. We conducted two experimental field interventions (Ns = 247 and 247) to test models that integrate different levels of categorization. Each of the tested models was contrasted against a no-intervention control condition. In both studies, we assessed effects shortly after the intervention (1 week later; i.e., posttest) and then after approximately 6 months (i.e., follow-up). In the first study, results generally supported the model where categorization precedes decategorization, showing effects on major dependent measures highlighted in research on intergroup contact: quantity and quality of contact, cross-group friendships, intergroup anxiety (marginal effect at follow-up), outgroup attitudes (only at follow-up). Evidence for follow-up effects for this model was, however, weaker in Study 2, where the delayed effects of the intervention emerged only indirectly, via changes in contact quality, outgroup attitudes, and approach behavioral intentions at posttest. Comparisons of the other two models (decategorization then categorization; and simultaneous categorization and decategorization) with the control condition (only in Study 1) provided weaker and inconsistent results.
研究提出了如何构建接触情境以最大限度地发挥接触效果的不同模型,特别关注分类在接触过程中的作用。我们进行了两项实验性现场干预(Ns=247和247),以测试整合不同分类水平的模型。将每个测试模型与无干预控制条件进行对比。在这两项研究中,我们在干预后不久(1周后,即后测)和大约6个月后(即随访)评估了效果。在第一项研究中,结果普遍支持分类先于非分类的模型,显示了对群体间接触研究中强调的主要依赖性指标的影响:接触的数量和质量、跨群体友谊、群体间焦虑(随访时的边际效应)、群外态度(仅随访时)。然而,该模型后续效果的证据在研究2中较弱,在研究2中,干预的延迟效应仅通过接触质量、外群体态度和后测时接近行为意图的变化间接出现。将其他两个模型(先去类别化,然后再分类;同时进行分类和去类别化)与对照条件(仅在研究1中)进行比较,结果较弱且不一致。
{"title":"Sequential models of intergroup contact and social categorization: An experimental field test of integrated models","authors":"L. Vezzali, E. Trifiletti, Ralf Wölfer, G. A. di Bernardo, S. Stathi, V. Cocco, A. Cadamuro, S. E. Shamloo, M. Hewstone","doi":"10.1177/13684302221104921","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221104921","url":null,"abstract":"Research has proposed different models of how contact situations should be structured to maximize contact effects, focusing in particular on the role of categorization during contact. We conducted two experimental field interventions (Ns = 247 and 247) to test models that integrate different levels of categorization. Each of the tested models was contrasted against a no-intervention control condition. In both studies, we assessed effects shortly after the intervention (1 week later; i.e., posttest) and then after approximately 6 months (i.e., follow-up). In the first study, results generally supported the model where categorization precedes decategorization, showing effects on major dependent measures highlighted in research on intergroup contact: quantity and quality of contact, cross-group friendships, intergroup anxiety (marginal effect at follow-up), outgroup attitudes (only at follow-up). Evidence for follow-up effects for this model was, however, weaker in Study 2, where the delayed effects of the intervention emerged only indirectly, via changes in contact quality, outgroup attitudes, and approach behavioral intentions at posttest. Comparisons of the other two models (decategorization then categorization; and simultaneous categorization and decategorization) with the control condition (only in Study 1) provided weaker and inconsistent results.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1181 - 1204"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44630683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Media representation matters: The effects of exposure to counter-stereotypical gay male characters on heterosexual men’s expressions of discrimination 媒体表现很重要:接触反刻板印象的男同性恋角色对异性恋男性表达歧视的影响
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-27 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221102874
Silvia Galdi, Francesca Guizzo, F. Fasoli
Presence of gay men in mainstream media may have a positive impact on viewers’ attitude change. However, gay male characters are often stereotypically portrayed as feminine, and no research has yet explored audiences’ reactions to counter-stereotypical gay characters. Heterosexual Italian men (N = 158) were exposed to a clip portraying (i) a stereotypical feminine gay male character, (ii) a counter-stereotypical masculine gay male character, or (iii) a nature documentary. Compared to the other conditions, exposure to the counter-stereotypical gay character increased discrimination toward gay men, in the form of anti-gay jokes, the higher the level of participants’ prejudice against gay men. Results further demonstrated that this effect was explained by reduced perceived stereotypicality of the character. Findings are in line with the social identity theory prediction that when gay men (outgroup) are perceived as too similar, and potentially threaten the group identity, heterosexual men would attempt to restore ingroup distinctiveness.
主流媒体中男同性恋的出现可能会对观众的态度转变产生积极影响。然而,男同性恋角色通常被刻板地描绘成女性,目前还没有研究探讨观众对反刻板的同性恋角色的反应。异性恋的意大利男性(N=158)接触了一个片段,该片段描绘了(i)一个刻板的女性男同性恋角色,(ii)一个反刻板的男性男同性恋角色或(iii)一部自然纪录片。与其他条件相比,暴露在反刻板印象的同性恋性格中会增加对男同性恋的歧视,表现为反同性恋笑话,参与者对男同性恋者的偏见程度越高。结果进一步表明,这种影响可以通过减少对角色的刻板印象来解释。研究结果与社会认同理论的预测一致,即当男同性恋(群体外)被认为过于相似,并可能威胁到群体认同时,异性恋男性会试图恢复群体内的独特性。
{"title":"Media representation matters: The effects of exposure to counter-stereotypical gay male characters on heterosexual men’s expressions of discrimination","authors":"Silvia Galdi, Francesca Guizzo, F. Fasoli","doi":"10.1177/13684302221102874","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221102874","url":null,"abstract":"Presence of gay men in mainstream media may have a positive impact on viewers’ attitude change. However, gay male characters are often stereotypically portrayed as feminine, and no research has yet explored audiences’ reactions to counter-stereotypical gay characters. Heterosexual Italian men (N = 158) were exposed to a clip portraying (i) a stereotypical feminine gay male character, (ii) a counter-stereotypical masculine gay male character, or (iii) a nature documentary. Compared to the other conditions, exposure to the counter-stereotypical gay character increased discrimination toward gay men, in the form of anti-gay jokes, the higher the level of participants’ prejudice against gay men. Results further demonstrated that this effect was explained by reduced perceived stereotypicality of the character. Findings are in line with the social identity theory prediction that when gay men (outgroup) are perceived as too similar, and potentially threaten the group identity, heterosexual men would attempt to restore ingroup distinctiveness.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1329 - 1350"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43235743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dominance motivated delusions: Whites with high social dominance orientation perceive equal amounts of institutional racism between Blacks and Whites 支配性错觉:具有高度社会支配倾向的白人认为黑人和白人之间的制度性种族主义是等量的
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-23 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221103984
Christopher K. Marshburn, Brandon A. Reinkensmeyer, E. Knowles
Whites display an asymmetry when detecting discrimination—disparate treatment from high-status groups directed toward low-status groups constitutes discrimination but not the opposite. Whites also believe they experience just as much racial discrimination as Blacks. This latter pattern could be especially true for Whites with higher social dominance orientation (SDO)—preference for intergroup dominance and inequality. Three studies (including one pilot study) investigated whether Whites with higher (vs. lower) SDO perceived examples of institutional and individual instances of racial discrimination as evidence of racism when happening to White (vs. Black) victims. Results revealed partial support for our prediction that the asymmetry in discrimination detection for Whites with higher (vs. lower) SDO would be opposite to the previously identified asymmetry, especially for examples of institutional (vs. individual) discrimination. Implications are discussed in the context of the 2020 Black Lives Matter protests and 2021 Capitol siege.
白人在察觉歧视时表现出一种不对称——高地位群体对低地位群体的差别待遇构成歧视,而不是相反。白人还认为,他们遭受的种族歧视和黑人一样多。后一种模式尤其适用于具有较高社会支配倾向(SDO)的白人——偏好群体间支配和不平等。三项研究(包括一项试点研究)调查了当发生在白人(与黑人)受害者身上时,具有较高(与较低)SDO的白人是否将机构和个人种族歧视的例子视为种族主义的证据。结果部分支持了我们的预测,即高(低)SDO的白人在歧视检测中的不对称性与之前发现的不对称性相反,特别是对于机构(与个人)歧视的例子。在2020年“黑人的命也重要”抗议活动和2021年国会大厦围困的背景下,讨论了其影响。
{"title":"Dominance motivated delusions: Whites with high social dominance orientation perceive equal amounts of institutional racism between Blacks and Whites","authors":"Christopher K. Marshburn, Brandon A. Reinkensmeyer, E. Knowles","doi":"10.1177/13684302221103984","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221103984","url":null,"abstract":"Whites display an asymmetry when detecting discrimination—disparate treatment from high-status groups directed toward low-status groups constitutes discrimination but not the opposite. Whites also believe they experience just as much racial discrimination as Blacks. This latter pattern could be especially true for Whites with higher social dominance orientation (SDO)—preference for intergroup dominance and inequality. Three studies (including one pilot study) investigated whether Whites with higher (vs. lower) SDO perceived examples of institutional and individual instances of racial discrimination as evidence of racism when happening to White (vs. Black) victims. Results revealed partial support for our prediction that the asymmetry in discrimination detection for Whites with higher (vs. lower) SDO would be opposite to the previously identified asymmetry, especially for examples of institutional (vs. individual) discrimination. Implications are discussed in the context of the 2020 Black Lives Matter protests and 2021 Capitol siege.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1244 - 1270"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42675221","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Flexible minds make more moderate views: Subtractive counterfactuals mitigate strong views about immigrants’ trustworthiness 灵活的思维产生更温和的观点:减法反事实减轻了对移民可信度的强烈看法
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-22 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221102876
Kevin Winter, Annika Scholl, K. Sassenberg
Public discourse on immigration has seemed to polarize over recent years—with some people strongly trusting, but others strongly distrusting immigrants. We examined whether a cognitive strategy could mitigate these biased outgroup judgments. Given that subtractive counterfactual thoughts (“If only I had not done X. . .”) facilitate cognitive flexibility and especially a relational processing style, we hypothesized that these thoughts (vs. additive counterfactuals “If only I had done X. . .” and no counterfactuals) would weaken the relationship between people’s political orientation and the perceived trustworthiness of immigrants. In five experiments (two preregistered; total N = 1,189), we found that inducing subtractive (but not additive) counterfactuals—either via rhetorical questions in a political speech or via mindset priming—had the predicted debiasing effect. Taken together, subtle means such as using subtractive counterfactual questions in political communication seem to be a promising way to reduce biased outgroup judgments in heated public debates.
近年来,关于移民的公共话语似乎两极分化——一些人非常信任移民,但另一些人则非常不信任移民。我们研究了认知策略是否可以减轻这些偏见的外群体判断。考虑到减法反事实思维(“如果我没有做x就好了……”)促进了认知灵活性,尤其是关系处理风格,我们假设这些思维(相对于“如果我做了x就好了……”)。(没有反事实)会削弱人们的政治取向与移民的可信赖性之间的关系。在五个实验中(两个预注册;总N = 1189),我们发现,诱导减法(而不是加法)反事实——无论是通过政治演讲中的修辞问题还是通过心态启动——都具有预期的消除偏见的效果。综上所述,在政治沟通中使用减法反事实问题等微妙手段似乎是一种很有希望的方法,可以减少激烈的公共辩论中有偏见的外群体判断。
{"title":"Flexible minds make more moderate views: Subtractive counterfactuals mitigate strong views about immigrants’ trustworthiness","authors":"Kevin Winter, Annika Scholl, K. Sassenberg","doi":"10.1177/13684302221102876","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221102876","url":null,"abstract":"Public discourse on immigration has seemed to polarize over recent years—with some people strongly trusting, but others strongly distrusting immigrants. We examined whether a cognitive strategy could mitigate these biased outgroup judgments. Given that subtractive counterfactual thoughts (“If only I had not done X. . .”) facilitate cognitive flexibility and especially a relational processing style, we hypothesized that these thoughts (vs. additive counterfactuals “If only I had done X. . .” and no counterfactuals) would weaken the relationship between people’s political orientation and the perceived trustworthiness of immigrants. In five experiments (two preregistered; total N = 1,189), we found that inducing subtractive (but not additive) counterfactuals—either via rhetorical questions in a political speech or via mindset priming—had the predicted debiasing effect. Taken together, subtle means such as using subtractive counterfactual questions in political communication seem to be a promising way to reduce biased outgroup judgments in heated public debates.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1310 - 1328"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41431651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Like the cool kids? The role of popular classmates in the development of anti-immigrant attitudes in adolescence 喜欢那些酷孩子吗?受欢迎的同学在青少年反移民态度发展中的作用
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-13 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221099444
Andrea Bohman, Aleš Kudrnáč
While classmates have been identified as important socializing agents in relation to adolescents’ prejudice, there is limited understanding of how popularity status plays into classroom transmission of prejudicial attitudes. Drawing on theories of social influence, we used a three-wave panel of Swedish adolescents (N = 941, aged 13–15) to examine the role of sociometric and prestige popular classmates in the development of adolescents’ anti-immigrant attitudes. Multilevel repeated measurement models revealed positive relationships between popular and individual prejudice; between sociometric prejudice and the level and rate of change; and between prestige prejudice and wave-to-wave shifts in individual prejudice. Overall, we found sociometrically popular classmates to be more influential in relation to adolescents’ prejudice. Additionally, we found the effect of sociometric prejudice to be more pronounced if political issues were frequently discussed in the classroom.
虽然同学已被确定为与青少年偏见有关的重要社交媒介,但人们对受欢迎程度如何影响偏见态度的课堂传播的理解有限。根据社会影响理论,我们使用瑞典青少年(N = 941, 13-15岁)的三波面板来检验社会计量学和声望受欢迎的同学在青少年反移民态度发展中的作用。多层次重复测量模型揭示了大众偏见与个体偏见之间的正相关关系;社会计量偏见与变化水平和速度之间的关系在声望偏见和个人偏见的波动之间。总的来说,我们发现在社会计量学上受欢迎的同学对青少年的偏见更有影响。此外,我们发现,如果在课堂上经常讨论政治问题,社会计量偏见的影响会更加明显。
{"title":"Like the cool kids? The role of popular classmates in the development of anti-immigrant attitudes in adolescence","authors":"Andrea Bohman, Aleš Kudrnáč","doi":"10.1177/13684302221099444","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221099444","url":null,"abstract":"While classmates have been identified as important socializing agents in relation to adolescents’ prejudice, there is limited understanding of how popularity status plays into classroom transmission of prejudicial attitudes. Drawing on theories of social influence, we used a three-wave panel of Swedish adolescents (N = 941, aged 13–15) to examine the role of sociometric and prestige popular classmates in the development of adolescents’ anti-immigrant attitudes. Multilevel repeated measurement models revealed positive relationships between popular and individual prejudice; between sociometric prejudice and the level and rate of change; and between prestige prejudice and wave-to-wave shifts in individual prejudice. Overall, we found sociometrically popular classmates to be more influential in relation to adolescents’ prejudice. Additionally, we found the effect of sociometric prejudice to be more pronounced if political issues were frequently discussed in the classroom.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1010 - 1031"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46273785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
United we stand? Perceived loyalty of dual nationals, multiracial people, and dual state residents 团结一致?双重国籍、多种族和双重国籍居民的感知忠诚
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-09 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221096322
R. Fine, Nour S. Kteily, Jacqueline M. Chen, Steven O. Roberts, Arnold K. Ho
As multiracial children of foreign-born parents, Kamala Harris and Barack Obama embody diversity in politics for many perceivers. Yet some have also questioned their loyalty to their respective groups. We explored perceptions of dual group members’ (DGM; dual nationals, multiracial people, and dual state residents) loyalty among first-party (those who share a group membership with a DGM target) and third-party perceivers (those who do not). Studies 1a–2b showed that first- and third-party perceivers rated DGMs as less loyal than their single group member (SGM) counterparts. However, only first- (Studies 2a–2b) but not third-party (Studies 1a–1b) perceivers preferred SGMs to DGMs for loyalty-dependent roles. Study 3 revealed that perceivers who were higher in patriotism supported DGMs for loyalty-dependent roles less when they were first- (vs. third) party observers. These studies suggest that perceivers readily intuit that DGMs are less loyal than SGMs and, under some conditions, this may lead to discrimination.
卡玛拉·哈里斯(Kamala Harris)和巴拉克·奥巴马(Barack Obama)作为父母在外国出生的多种族子女,在许多人看来体现了政治的多样性。然而,一些人也质疑他们对各自团体的忠诚度。我们探讨了第一方(与DGM目标共享团体成员的人)和第三方感知者(不共享团体成员)对双重团体成员(DGM;双重国民、多种族人群和双重州居民)忠诚的看法。研究1a–2b表明,第一和第三方感知者认为DGM的忠诚度低于其单一群体成员(SGM)。然而,只有第一(研究2a-2b)而不是第三方(研究1a-1b)的感知者更喜欢SGM而不是DGM来扮演忠诚度依赖的角色。研究3显示,爱国主义情绪较高的感知者在担任第一方(与第三方)观察员时,对DGM忠诚依赖角色的支持较少。这些研究表明,感知者很容易直觉到DGM不如SGM忠诚,在某些情况下,这可能会导致歧视。
{"title":"United we stand? Perceived loyalty of dual nationals, multiracial people, and dual state residents","authors":"R. Fine, Nour S. Kteily, Jacqueline M. Chen, Steven O. Roberts, Arnold K. Ho","doi":"10.1177/13684302221096322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221096322","url":null,"abstract":"As multiracial children of foreign-born parents, Kamala Harris and Barack Obama embody diversity in politics for many perceivers. Yet some have also questioned their loyalty to their respective groups. We explored perceptions of dual group members’ (DGM; dual nationals, multiracial people, and dual state residents) loyalty among first-party (those who share a group membership with a DGM target) and third-party perceivers (those who do not). Studies 1a–2b showed that first- and third-party perceivers rated DGMs as less loyal than their single group member (SGM) counterparts. However, only first- (Studies 2a–2b) but not third-party (Studies 1a–1b) perceivers preferred SGMs to DGMs for loyalty-dependent roles. Study 3 revealed that perceivers who were higher in patriotism supported DGMs for loyalty-dependent roles less when they were first- (vs. third) party observers. These studies suggest that perceivers readily intuit that DGMs are less loyal than SGMs and, under some conditions, this may lead to discrimination.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"913 - 933"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41451918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Economic inequality shapes the agency–communion content of gender stereotypes 经济不平等塑造了机构——性别刻板印象的交流内容
IF 4.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-02 DOI: 10.1177/13684302221095338
Eva Moreno‐Bella, G. Willis, Angélica Quiroga-Garza, Miguel Moya
Economic inequality is a main issue in current societies, and it affects people’s psychological processes. In this research, we propose that perceived economic inequality might affect how people perceive men and women. In two experiments carried out in Spain (N = 170) and Mexico (N = 215), we tested whether high (vs. low) economic inequality leads to changes in the perceived agency and communion of both men and women. Our findings suggest that when economic inequality is high (vs. low), the communal content in social perceptions of both men and women decreases. Specifically, under high (vs. low) inequality, the difference in agency and communion ascribed to a man becomes greater (i.e., men are perceived as even more agentic than communal), whereas this difference becomes smaller for women (i.e., women are still perceived as more communal than agentic, but this difference is smaller). We discuss these findings’ implications regarding the psychosocial effects of economic inequality.
经济不平等是当今社会的一个主要问题,它影响着人们的心理过程。在这项研究中,我们提出,感知到的经济不平等可能会影响人们对男性和女性的感知。在西班牙(N=170)和墨西哥(N=215)进行的两项实验中,我们测试了高(与低)经济不平等是否会导致男性和女性感知的代理和交流发生变化。我们的研究结果表明,当经济不平等程度较高(相对于较低)时,男性和女性社会认知中的公共内容都会减少。具体而言,在高(与低)不平等的情况下,男性在代理和交流方面的差异变得更大(即,男性被认为比社区更具代理性),而女性的这种差异变得更小(即,女性仍然被认为比代理更具社区性,但这种差异更小)。我们讨论了这些发现对经济不平等的心理社会影响的影响。
{"title":"Economic inequality shapes the agency–communion content of gender stereotypes","authors":"Eva Moreno‐Bella, G. Willis, Angélica Quiroga-Garza, Miguel Moya","doi":"10.1177/13684302221095338","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221095338","url":null,"abstract":"Economic inequality is a main issue in current societies, and it affects people’s psychological processes. In this research, we propose that perceived economic inequality might affect how people perceive men and women. In two experiments carried out in Spain (N = 170) and Mexico (N = 215), we tested whether high (vs. low) economic inequality leads to changes in the perceived agency and communion of both men and women. Our findings suggest that when economic inequality is high (vs. low), the communal content in social perceptions of both men and women decreases. Specifically, under high (vs. low) inequality, the difference in agency and communion ascribed to a man becomes greater (i.e., men are perceived as even more agentic than communal), whereas this difference becomes smaller for women (i.e., women are still perceived as more communal than agentic, but this difference is smaller). We discuss these findings’ implications regarding the psychosocial effects of economic inequality.","PeriodicalId":48099,"journal":{"name":"Group Processes & Intergroup Relations","volume":"26 1","pages":"1075 - 1098"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44549506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1