首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Economic Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Risky and non-risky financial investments and cognition 风险与非风险金融投资及认知
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102677
Nicolau Martin-Bassols

Much policy attention has been placed on encouraging saving behaviours to avoid financial deprivation at older adulthood. However, optimising financial investments is highly dependent on cognitive capacity, which can be deteriorated by the natural ageing process. This paper explores the relationship between age-related cognitive deterioration with risky and non-risky financial investments. The data analysed comes from eight waves (2002 to 2019) of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) which, for a nationally representative sample, contains information about cognitive ability and their ownership of financial investments over time. Cognition emerges as one of the strongest predictors of the type and number of financial products that individuals hold. Specifically, the results show a positive relation of cognitive level with risky and non-risky financial investments. Even alongside other important factors – such as education, labour status, age, and household wealth – the explanatory power of cognition is found to be significant at the 0.1% significance level. The results are robust when accounting for unobservables and when using a genetic measure of cognition as main explanatory variable. More importantly, cognitive deterioration is only significantly associated with risky financial investments. That means, individuals reduce their risky financial investments when they start suffering old age related cognitive deterioration, while they do not change their holdings in non-risky ones in a significant manner. There are also no significant differences in the reactions to stock-market fluctuations due to cognitive level. In other words, individuals react to stock market fluctuations changing their financial holdings, but those reactions do not differ due to their cognitive level.

许多政策关注鼓励储蓄行为,以避免老年经济匮乏。然而,优化金融投资高度依赖于认知能力,而认知能力可能会因自然衰老过程而恶化。本文探讨了与年龄相关的认知退化与风险和非风险金融投资之间的关系。所分析的数据来自英国老龄化纵向研究(ELSA)的八波(2002年至2019年),作为一个具有全国代表性的样本,该研究包含了有关认知能力及其随时间推移的金融投资所有权的信息。认知成为预测个人持有的金融产品类型和数量的最强因素之一。具体而言,结果显示认知水平与风险和非风险金融投资呈正相关。即使与其他重要因素(如教育、劳动状况、年龄和家庭财富)一起,认知的解释力在0.1%的显著性水平上也是显著的。当考虑到不可观察的因素和使用认知的遗传测量作为主要解释变量时,结果是稳健的。更重要的是,认知退化只与高风险的金融投资显著相关。这意味着,当个人开始遭受与老年相关的认知衰退时,他们会减少高风险的金融投资,而不会大幅改变他们在非风险投资方面的持有。在对股市波动的反应上,认知水平也没有显著差异。换句话说,个人对股票市场波动改变其金融资产的反应,但这些反应并不因他们的认知水平而有所不同。
{"title":"Risky and non-risky financial investments and cognition","authors":"Nicolau Martin-Bassols","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102677","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2023.102677","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Much policy attention has been placed on encouraging saving behaviours to avoid financial deprivation at older adulthood. However, optimising financial investments is highly dependent on cognitive capacity, which can be deteriorated by the natural ageing process. This paper explores the relationship between age-related cognitive deterioration with risky and non-risky financial investments. The data analysed comes from eight waves (2002 to 2019) of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) which, for a nationally representative sample, contains information about cognitive ability and their ownership of financial investments over time. Cognition emerges as one of the strongest predictors of the type and number of financial products that individuals hold. Specifically, the results show a positive relation of cognitive level with risky and non-risky financial investments. Even alongside other important factors – such as education, labour status, age, and household wealth – the explanatory power of cognition is found to be significant at the 0.1% significance level. The results are robust when accounting for unobservables and when using a genetic measure of cognition as main explanatory variable. More importantly, cognitive deterioration is only significantly associated with risky financial investments. That means, individuals reduce their risky financial investments when they start suffering old age related cognitive deterioration, while they do not change their holdings in non-risky ones in a significant manner. There are also no significant differences in the reactions to stock-market fluctuations due to cognitive level. In other words, individuals react to stock market fluctuations changing their financial holdings, but those reactions do not differ due to their cognitive level.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"100 ","pages":"Article 102677"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91984574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How explicit expected value information affects tax compliance decisions and information acquisition 明确的期望值信息如何影响税务合规决策和信息获取
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102679
Martin Müller , Jerome Olsen , Erich Kirchler , Christoph Kogler

In a MouselabWEB experiment with 345 participants, we investigated whether different presentations of expected value information in tax compliance decisions increase conformity with classical deterrence models’ assumptions. Recording both choice and process data, we compare conditions of verbal explanation only, verbal explanation plus numerical cue, verbal explanation plus visual cue, and a control condition without expected value information. Only when the expected value was presented as a visual cue the option with the higher expected value (i.e., evasion) was chosen more often than the control condition (58.3% vs. 38.4%). Nevertheless, individuals were more compliant than predicted by the deterrence model. While we identified differences between the experimental conditions in information acquisition patterns and decision times, they do not suggest that one way of presenting expected value information was easier to process than the others or that the main behavioral effect can be explained by higher saliency of the visual cue. These results indicate that individuals' decisions are not predominantly driven by outcome maximization, even when explicit expected value information is provided.

在一项有345名参与者的MouselabWEB实验中,我们研究了税收合规决策中期望值信息的不同表达是否会增加对经典威慑模型假设的遵从性。记录选择和过程数据,我们比较了仅口头解释、口头解释加数字提示、口头解释加视觉提示和没有期望值信息的控制条件。只有当期望值作为视觉提示呈现时,期望值较高的选项(即逃避)比控制条件更常被选择(58.3%对38.4%)。然而,个体比威慑模型预测的更顺从。虽然我们发现了信息获取模式和决策时间的实验条件之间的差异,但这并不意味着呈现预期价值信息的一种方式比其他方式更容易处理,也不意味着主要的行为效应可以用视觉线索的更高显着性来解释。这些结果表明,即使提供了明确的期望值信息,个人的决策也不是主要由结果最大化驱动的。
{"title":"How explicit expected value information affects tax compliance decisions and information acquisition","authors":"Martin Müller ,&nbsp;Jerome Olsen ,&nbsp;Erich Kirchler ,&nbsp;Christoph Kogler","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102679","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2023.102679","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In a MouselabWEB experiment with 345 participants, we investigated whether different presentations of expected value information in tax compliance decisions increase conformity with classical deterrence models’ assumptions. Recording both choice and process data, we compare conditions of verbal explanation only, verbal explanation plus numerical cue, verbal explanation plus visual cue, and a control condition without expected value information. Only when the expected value was presented as a visual cue the option with the higher expected value (i.e., evasion) was chosen more often than the control condition (58.3% vs. 38.4%). Nevertheless, individuals were more compliant than predicted by the deterrence model. While we identified differences between the experimental conditions in information acquisition patterns and decision times, they do not suggest that one way of presenting expected value information was easier to process than the others or that the main behavioral effect can be explained by higher saliency of the visual cue. These results indicate that individuals' decisions are not predominantly driven by outcome maximization, even when explicit expected value information is provided.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 102679"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167487023000806/pdfft?md5=c02ede694d3e4e5ffc01586ead56b062&pid=1-s2.0-S0167487023000806-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91964471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of perceived similarity and social proximity on the formation of prosocial preferences 感知相似性和社会接近度对亲社会偏好形成的影响
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102678
Christoph A. Schütt

Homophily, the tendency of interacting with similar others, has been found to be an important determinant of the existence and stability of social groups, whereas social relationships within these groups vary in the degree of social proximity. In this paper, I investigate how perceived similarity affects social proximity and care towards a partner and altruistic giving. In a between-subjects design, subjects are matched with either a similar or a dissimilar partner and play a dictator game. Similarity is induced via objective similarities on pre-elicited personality profiles based on Big-Five personality traits. I show that perceiving someone as more similar increases giving in a dictator game. A mediation analysis shows that social proximity and care towards the partner mediate the treatment effect. That is, subjects in the similar treatment feel a higher degree of social proximity to their partner, leading to a higher degree of care towards the latter and therefore, give more in the dictator game.

同性恋,即与相似他人互动的倾向,已被发现是社会群体存在和稳定的重要决定因素,而这些群体内的社会关系因社会接近程度而异。在这篇论文中,我研究了感知相似性如何影响社会接近度和对伴侣的关心以及无私奉献。在受试者之间的设计中,受试者与相似或不同的伴侣配对,玩独裁者游戏。相似性是通过基于五大人格特征的预先引发的人格特征的客观相似性来诱导的。我展示了在独裁者游戏中,将某人视为更相似的增加付出。中介分析表明,社会接近度和对伴侣的关心对治疗效果起中介作用。也就是说,接受类似治疗的受试者感觉到与伴侣的社交距离更高,从而对后者产生更高程度的关心,因此在独裁者游戏中给予更多。
{"title":"The effect of perceived similarity and social proximity on the formation of prosocial preferences","authors":"Christoph A. Schütt","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102678","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2023.102678","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Homophily, the tendency of interacting with similar others, has been found to be an important determinant of the existence and stability of social groups, whereas social relationships within these groups vary in the degree of social proximity. In this paper, I investigate how perceived similarity affects social proximity and care towards a partner and altruistic giving. In a between-subjects design, subjects are matched with either a similar or a dissimilar partner and play a dictator game. Similarity is induced via objective similarities on pre-elicited personality profiles based on Big-Five personality traits. I show that perceiving someone as more similar increases giving in a dictator game. A mediation analysis shows that social proximity and care towards the partner mediate the treatment effect. That is, subjects in the similar treatment feel a higher degree of social proximity to their partner, leading to a higher degree of care towards the latter and therefore, give more in the dictator game.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 102678"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49870856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Leadership heuristic 领导启发式
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102661
Carina Cavalcanti , Philip J. Grossman , Elias L. Khalil

The economics literature offers at least two main explanations of why individuals adopt the heuristic of following their leader’s suggestion: First, the leader has information or a talent relevant to the task at hand and, second, the leader’s suggestion helps to reduce uncertainty and to coordinate the group on one choice. The psychology literature offers another explanation: The leader, acting as an “ethical example,” helps to increase job satisfaction, performance, and prosocial behavior. Both lines of literature, although in different ways, assume rational choice on the part of followers. Neither literature addresses the question: Would people adopt the leadership heuristic if the leader lacks any relevant information, talent advantage, ethical character, or other desirable traits? We report experimental evidence that suggests the answer is yes. In our experiment, leaders suggest the outcome of a fair “coin toss.” Leaders vary in (irrelevant) “information” and (irrelevant) “ability” possessed. Although there is no feedback after each period, we find that one-third of all the decisions of the participants heuristically follow the leader’s choice. This is surprising given that, first, the leader’s choice is irrelevant and, second, to follow it would be payoff-reducing. Payoff-reducing choices of subjects are more frequent with irrelevantly informed leaders than with irrelevantly talented leaders. Crucially, we also show that the findings are not driven by lack of understanding of random events. In short, neither the hot-hand and gambler’s fallacies nor attributes that might inspire trust/loyalty can explain subjects’ choices.

经济学文献提供了至少两种主要的解释,来解释为什么个人会接受启发式的领导建议:首先,领导者拥有与手头任务相关的信息或才能;其次,领导者的建议有助于减少不确定性,并在一个选择上协调团队。心理学文献提供了另一种解释:领导者作为“道德榜样”,有助于提高工作满意度、绩效和亲社会行为。这两种文学流派虽然方式不同,但都假定追随者有理性的选择。两篇文献都没有解决这个问题:如果领导者缺乏任何相关信息、人才优势、道德品质或其他可取的品质,人们会采用领导启发式吗?我们报告的实验证据表明,答案是肯定的。在我们的实验中,领导者提出公平的“抛硬币”的结果。领导者在(不相关的)“信息”和(不相关的)“能力”上各不相同。虽然在每个周期后都没有反馈,但我们发现参与者的所有决策中有三分之一是启发式地遵循领导者的选择。这是令人惊讶的,因为首先,领导者的选择是无关的,其次,遵循它会减少回报。与不相关的有才能的领导者相比,信息不相关的领导者更容易选择降低收益的科目。至关重要的是,我们还表明,这些发现并不是由于缺乏对随机事件的理解而产生的。简而言之,无论是热手和赌徒的谬论,还是可能激发信任/忠诚的属性,都不能解释受试者的选择。
{"title":"Leadership heuristic","authors":"Carina Cavalcanti ,&nbsp;Philip J. Grossman ,&nbsp;Elias L. Khalil","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102661","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102661","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The economics literature offers at least two main explanations of why individuals adopt the heuristic of following their leader’s suggestion: First, the leader has information or a talent relevant to the task at hand and, second, the leader’s suggestion helps to reduce uncertainty and to coordinate the group on one choice. The psychology literature offers another explanation: The leader, acting as an “ethical example,” helps to increase job satisfaction, performance, and prosocial behavior. Both lines of literature, although in different ways, assume rational choice on the part of followers. Neither literature addresses the question: Would people adopt the leadership heuristic if the leader lacks any relevant information, talent advantage, ethical character, or other desirable traits? We report experimental evidence that suggests the answer is yes. In our experiment, leaders suggest the outcome of a fair “coin toss.” Leaders vary in (irrelevant) “information” and (irrelevant) “ability” possessed. Although there is no feedback after each period, we find that one-third of all the decisions of the participants heuristically follow the leader’s choice. This is surprising given that, first, the leader’s choice is irrelevant and, second, to follow it would be payoff-reducing. Payoff-reducing choices of subjects are more frequent with irrelevantly informed leaders than with irrelevantly talented leaders. Crucially, we also show that the findings are not driven by lack of understanding of random events. In short, neither the hot-hand and gambler’s fallacies nor attributes that might inspire trust/loyalty can explain subjects’ choices.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102661"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44657089","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Challenges in studying the interplay of genes and environment. A study of childhood financial distress moderating genetic predisposition for peak smoking 研究基因与环境相互作用的挑战。儿童经济困难对吸烟高峰遗传易感性的调节研究
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102636
Laura Bierut , Pietro Biroli , Titus J. Galama , Kevin Thom

Smoking is one of the leading causes of preventable disease and death in the U.S., and it is strongly influenced both by genetic predisposition and childhood adversity. Using polygenic indices (PGIs) of predisposition to smoking, we evaluate whether childhood financial distress (CFD; a composite measure of financial adversity) moderates genetic risk in explaining peak-cigarette consumption in adulthood. Using the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), we find a substantial reduction in the relationship between genetic risk and peak smoking for those who did not suffer financial adversity in childhood. Among adult smokers who grew up in high-CFD households, a one standard deviation higher PGI is associated with 2.9 more cigarettes smoked per day at peak. By contrast, among smokers who grew up in low-CFD households, this gradient is reduced by 37 percent (or 1.1 fewer). These results are robust to controlling for a host of prime confounders. By contrast, we find no evidence of interactions between the PGI and typical measures of childhood SES such as parental education - a null result that we replicate in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) and the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA). This suggests the role of childhood financial distress in the relationship with peak smoking is distinct from that of low childhood SES, with high CFD potentially reflecting more acute distress than do measures of low childhood SES. Our evidence also suggests low childhood SES is a weaker proxy for acute distress, providing an alternative explanation for the childhood SES null result.

在美国,吸烟是导致可预防疾病和死亡的主要原因之一,它受到遗传易感性和童年逆境的强烈影响。使用吸烟倾向的多基因指数(PGIs),我们评估儿童经济困难(CFD;在解释成年期香烟消费高峰时,一种财务逆境的综合测量方法缓和了遗传风险。利用健康与退休研究(HRS),我们发现,对于那些在童年时期没有遭受经济困难的人来说,遗传风险和吸烟高峰之间的关系大幅降低。在高cfd家庭中长大的成年吸烟者中,PGI每高一个标准差,峰值时每天多抽2.9支烟。相比之下,在低cfd家庭中长大的吸烟者,这一梯度降低了37%(或1.1)。这些结果对于控制大量主要混杂因素是稳健的。相比之下,我们没有发现PGI与典型的儿童社会经济地位测量(如父母教育)之间相互作用的证据——我们在威斯康星纵向研究(WLS)和英国老龄化纵向研究(ELSA)中重复了一个无效结果。这表明童年经济困难在与吸烟高峰的关系中的作用与童年低经济地位的关系不同,高CFD可能比童年低经济地位的测量反映出更严重的痛苦。我们的证据还表明,低童年SES是急性痛苦的弱代理,为童年SES无效的结果提供了另一种解释。
{"title":"Challenges in studying the interplay of genes and environment. A study of childhood financial distress moderating genetic predisposition for peak smoking","authors":"Laura Bierut ,&nbsp;Pietro Biroli ,&nbsp;Titus J. Galama ,&nbsp;Kevin Thom","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102636","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102636","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Smoking is one of the leading causes of preventable disease and death in the U.S., and it is strongly influenced both by genetic predisposition and childhood adversity. Using polygenic indices (PGIs) of predisposition to smoking, we evaluate whether childhood financial distress (CFD; a composite measure of financial adversity) moderates genetic risk in explaining peak-cigarette consumption in adulthood. Using the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), we find a substantial reduction in the relationship between genetic risk and peak smoking for those who did not suffer financial adversity in childhood. Among adult smokers who grew up in high-CFD households, a one standard deviation higher PGI is associated with 2.9 more cigarettes smoked per day at peak. By contrast, among smokers who grew up in low-CFD households, this gradient is reduced by 37 percent (or 1.1 fewer). These results are robust to controlling for a host of prime confounders. By contrast, we find no evidence of interactions between the PGI and typical measures of childhood SES such as parental education - a null result that we replicate in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) and the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA). This suggests the role of childhood financial distress in the relationship with peak smoking is distinct from that of low childhood SES, with high CFD potentially reflecting more acute distress than do measures of low childhood SES. Our evidence also suggests low childhood SES is a weaker proxy for acute distress, providing an alternative explanation for the childhood SES null result.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102636"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10358858/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9866651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Smiles behind a mask are detectable and affect judgments of attractiveness, trustworthiness, and competence 面具后的微笑是可察觉的,会影响对吸引力、可信度和能力的判断
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102660
Astrid Hopfensitz , César Mantilla

Smiling is a popular and powerful facial signal used to influence how we are judged and evaluated by others. The recent COVID pandemic made the use of face masks common around the world. Since face masks, when properly worn, cover the lower half of the face, a common concern is that they inhibit our ability to signal to others through facial expressions like smiles. In this paper, we show through three subsequent studies that smiling faces are easily distinguished from neutral faces even if the person is wearing a face mask (Study 1, N = 1814). We further show that smiling behind a face mask significantly influences ratings regarding attractiveness, trustworthiness, and competence (Study 2, N = 250). We finally show that individuals with about 18 months of experience with face masks are well aware that smiling behind face masks will influence ratings regarding attractiveness and trustworthiness by others (Study 3, N = 94). Together, our studies provide evidence that face masks should not be seen as a threat that inhibits simple non-verbal communication through smiles.

微笑是一种流行的、强大的面部信号,用来影响别人对我们的判断和评价。最近的COVID大流行使口罩在世界范围内普遍使用。由于口罩在佩戴得当的情况下会遮住脸的下半部分,因此人们普遍担心,口罩会抑制我们通过微笑等面部表情向他人发出信号的能力。在本文中,我们通过随后的三个研究表明,即使人戴着口罩,微笑的脸也很容易与中性的脸区分开来(研究1,N = 1814)。我们进一步表明,戴口罩微笑会显著影响人们对吸引力、可信度和能力的评价(研究2,N = 250)。我们最终表明,有18个月口罩使用经验的人很清楚,戴着口罩微笑会影响他人对吸引力和可信度的评价(研究3,N = 94)。总之,我们的研究提供的证据表明,口罩不应该被视为一种威胁,它会阻碍通过微笑进行简单的非语言交流。
{"title":"Smiles behind a mask are detectable and affect judgments of attractiveness, trustworthiness, and competence","authors":"Astrid Hopfensitz ,&nbsp;César Mantilla","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102660","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102660","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Smiling is a popular and powerful facial signal used to influence how we are judged and evaluated by others. The recent COVID pandemic made the use of face masks common around the world. Since face masks, when properly worn, cover the lower half of the face, a common concern is that they inhibit our ability to signal to others through facial expressions like smiles. In this paper, we show through three subsequent studies that smiling faces are easily distinguished from neutral faces even if the person is wearing a face mask (Study 1, N = 1814). We further show that smiling behind a face mask significantly influences ratings regarding attractiveness, trustworthiness, and competence (Study 2, N = 250). We finally show that individuals with about 18 months of experience with face masks are well aware that smiling behind face masks will influence ratings regarding attractiveness and trustworthiness by others (Study 3, N = 94). Together, our studies provide evidence that face masks should not be seen as a threat that inhibits simple non-verbal communication through smiles.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102660"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47045374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are lay expectations of inflation based on recall of specific prices? If so, how and under what conditions? 外行人对通货膨胀的预期是否基于对具体价格的回忆?如果是,如何以及在什么条件下?
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102662
Xiaoxiao Niu, Nigel Harvey

In 2019 when inflation was low and stable, we compared people’s direct estimates of inflation with indirect estimates obtained by averaging their estimates of price changes in all 12 product categories on which the consumer price index is based. Indirect estimates were much higher than direct ones and the two types of estimate were uncorrelated. This is consistent with a price-free model in which direct estimates are not based on recall of prices but are determined by other information such as media reports. In May 2022 when inflation was high, expected to rise in the short-term, but highly unpredictable in the longer term, we found that direct and indirect estimates were very similar and highly correlated. This is consistent with a price-recall model in which a representative set of prices is used to estimate overall inflation. Finally, in September 2022 when inflation was fairly stable except for certain product categories (food) where prices were rising rapidly, we found that results were consistent with a third approach, the price-salience model; overall estimates of inflation are selectively influenced by price rises in those product categories where they are particularly high. People’s strategy for estimating inflation appears adapted to the prevailing inflation environment.

在2019年通货膨胀率较低且稳定的情况下,我们将人们对通货膨胀的直接估计与间接估计进行了比较,间接估计是通过平均他们对消费者价格指数所依据的所有12种产品类别的价格变化的估计得到的。间接估计值远高于直接估计值,且两种估计值不相关。这与无价格模型是一致的,在无价格模型中,直接估计不是基于对价格的回忆,而是由媒体报道等其他信息决定的。在2022年5月,当通货膨胀处于高位时,预计短期内会上升,但从长期来看是高度不可预测的,我们发现直接和间接的估计非常相似,并且高度相关。这与价格召回模型是一致的,在价格召回模型中,使用一组有代表性的价格来估计总体通胀。最后,在2022年9月,通货膨胀相对稳定,除了某些产品类别(食品)的价格迅速上涨,我们发现结果与第三种方法一致,即价格显著性模型;通货膨胀的总体估计有选择地受到那些价格特别高的产品类别价格上涨的影响。人们估计通货膨胀的策略似乎适应了当前的通货膨胀环境。
{"title":"Are lay expectations of inflation based on recall of specific prices? If so, how and under what conditions?","authors":"Xiaoxiao Niu,&nbsp;Nigel Harvey","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102662","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2023.102662","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In 2019 when inflation was low and stable, we compared people’s direct estimates of inflation with indirect estimates obtained by averaging their estimates of price changes in all 12 product categories on which the consumer price index is based. Indirect estimates were much higher than direct ones and the two types of estimate were uncorrelated. This is consistent with a price-free model in which direct estimates are not based on recall of prices but are determined by other information such as media reports. In May 2022 when inflation was high, expected to rise in the short-term, but highly unpredictable in the longer term, we found that direct and indirect estimates were very similar and highly correlated. This is consistent with a price-recall model in which a representative set of prices is used to estimate overall inflation. Finally, in September 2022 when inflation was fairly stable except for certain product categories (food) where prices were rising rapidly, we found that results were consistent with a third approach, the price-salience model; overall estimates of inflation are selectively influenced by price rises in those product categories where they are particularly high. People’s strategy for estimating inflation appears adapted to the prevailing inflation environment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102662"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49858886","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ovulatory shift, hormonal changes, and no effects on incentivized decision-making 排卵改变,荷尔蒙变化,对激励决策没有影响
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102656
Miloš Fišar , Lubomír Cingl , Tommaso Reggiani , Eva Kundtová Klocová , Radek Kundt , Jan Krátký , Katarína Kostolanská , Petra Bencúrová , Marie Kudličková Pešková , Klára Marečková

Employing an incentivized controlled lab experiment, we investigate the effects of ovulatory shift on salient behavioral outcomes related to (i) risk preferences, (ii) rule violation, and (iii) exploratory attitude. As evolutionary psychology suggests, these outcomes may play an important role in economic decision-making and represent behavioral aspects that may systematically vary over the menstrual cycle to increase the reproductive success. Exploiting a within-subjects design, 124 naturally cycling females participated in experimental sessions during their ovulation and menstruation, the phases between which the difference in the investigated behavior should be the largest. In each session, hormonal samples for cortisol, estradiol, and testosterone were collected. The group of women was also contrasted against an auxiliary reference group composed of 47 males, who are not subject to hormonal variations of this nature. Our results reveal no systematic behavioral differences between the ovulation and menstruation phases.

采用激励控制的实验室实验,我们研究了排卵变化对与(i)风险偏好、(ii)违反规则和(iii)探索态度相关的显著行为结果的影响。正如进化心理学所表明的,这些结果可能在经济决策中发挥重要作用,并代表了在月经周期中系统变化以提高生殖成功率的行为方面。利用受试者内设计,124名自然循环的女性在排卵期和月经期参加了实验,这两个阶段之间的调查行为差异应该是最大的。在每个疗程中,收集皮质醇、雌二醇和睾酮的激素样本。这组女性还与由47名男性组成的辅助参照组进行了对比,这些男性不受这种性质的荷尔蒙变化的影响。我们的研究结果显示排卵期和月经期之间没有系统的行为差异。
{"title":"Ovulatory shift, hormonal changes, and no effects on incentivized decision-making","authors":"Miloš Fišar ,&nbsp;Lubomír Cingl ,&nbsp;Tommaso Reggiani ,&nbsp;Eva Kundtová Klocová ,&nbsp;Radek Kundt ,&nbsp;Jan Krátký ,&nbsp;Katarína Kostolanská ,&nbsp;Petra Bencúrová ,&nbsp;Marie Kudličková Pešková ,&nbsp;Klára Marečková","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102656","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102656","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Employing an incentivized controlled lab experiment, we investigate the effects of ovulatory shift on salient behavioral outcomes related to (i) risk preferences, (ii) rule violation, and (iii) exploratory attitude. As evolutionary psychology suggests, these outcomes may play an important role in economic decision-making and represent behavioral aspects that may systematically vary over the menstrual cycle to increase the reproductive success. Exploiting a within-subjects design, 124 naturally cycling females participated in experimental sessions during their ovulation and menstruation, the phases between which the difference in the investigated behavior should be the largest. In each session, hormonal samples for cortisol, estradiol, and testosterone were collected. The group of women was also contrasted against an auxiliary reference group composed of 47 males, who are not subject to hormonal variations of this nature. Our results reveal no systematic behavioral differences between the ovulation and menstruation phases.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102656"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49170104","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A “More-is-Better” heuristic in anticommons dilemmas: Psychological insights from a new anticommons bargaining game 反公地困境中的“越多越好”启发式:来自一种新的反公地议价博弈的心理学洞察
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102653
Erik W. de Kwaadsteniet , Jörg Gross , Eric van Dijk

In the present paper, we investigate how people make decisions when bargaining about complementary resources. When the ownership of such resources is fragmented, actors often fail to coordinate on efficient access, leading to an overall loss in social welfare; the tragedy of the anticommons. In a series of three experiments, in which we introduce a newly developed Anticommons Bargaining Game, we show that people tend to treat perfectly complementary resources as if they are non-complementary. Specifically, we demonstrate that both sellers and buyers of such resources used a more-is-better heuristic when determining their prices. That is, sellers who initially owned a larger part of the resource asked a higher price for their resource than sellers with a smaller part, even though only the combination of parts generated value for the buyer. Likewise, buyers offered more money to sellers with a larger part than to sellers with a smaller part. While this heuristic does not necessarily impede coordination, inequality in resources led to unequal monetary outcomes between the two sellers.

在本文中,我们研究了人们在对互补资源讨价还价时是如何做出决策的。当这类资源的所有权分散时,行动者往往不能就有效获取进行协调,从而导致社会福利的全面损失;反公地的悲剧。在一系列的三个实验中,我们引入了一个新开发的反公地议价博弈,我们表明人们倾向于将完全互补的资源视为非互补资源。具体来说,我们证明了这些资源的卖方和买方在确定价格时都使用了“越多越好”的启发式。也就是说,最初拥有较大部分资源的卖家比拥有较小部分资源的卖家要求更高的价格,即使只有部分的组合为买家创造了价值。同样地,买方向资金较多的卖方提供的钱也多于资金较少的卖方。虽然这种启发式不一定会阻碍协调,但资源的不平等导致两个卖家之间的货币结果不平等。
{"title":"A “More-is-Better” heuristic in anticommons dilemmas: Psychological insights from a new anticommons bargaining game","authors":"Erik W. de Kwaadsteniet ,&nbsp;Jörg Gross ,&nbsp;Eric van Dijk","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102653","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102653","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the present paper, we investigate how people make decisions when bargaining about complementary resources. When the ownership of such resources is fragmented, actors often fail to coordinate on efficient access, leading to an overall loss in social welfare; the tragedy of the anticommons. In a series of three experiments, in which we introduce a newly developed Anticommons Bargaining Game, we show that people tend to treat perfectly complementary resources as if they are non-complementary. Specifically, we demonstrate that both sellers and buyers of such resources used a more-is-better heuristic when determining their prices. That is, sellers who initially owned a larger part of the resource asked a higher price for their resource than sellers with a smaller part, even though only the combination of parts generated value for the buyer. Likewise, buyers offered more money to sellers with a larger part than to sellers with a smaller part. While this heuristic does not necessarily impede coordination, inequality in resources led to unequal monetary outcomes between the two sellers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102653"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49269620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do international students learn foreign preferences? The interplay of language, identity and assimilation 国际学生了解外国的喜好吗?语言、身份和同化的相互作用
IF 3.5 2区 经济学 Q2 ECONOMICS Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2023.102658
Paul Clist , Ying-yi Hong

Every year millions of students study at foreign universities, swapping one set of cultural surroundings for another. This may reveal whether measured preferences are fixed or flexible, whether they can be altered in the short run by moving country, or learning a new language. We disentangle these influences by measuring international students’ preferences. For Chinese students in the UK (who arrived up to five years previously) we randomise a survey’s language. We add reference groups in each country, doing the survey in the relevant language. Simple comparisons provide a causal estimate of language’s effect and observational estimates of differences by country, location and nationality. We find language has a large causal effect on a range of survey responses. The effect size is similar to differences by country or nationality (at 0.4 standard deviations), and larger than differences by location (at 0.1 standard deviations). Assimilation theories predict any movement in measured preferences for Chinese students in the UK would be towards those of UK students, even if they may be small. We do not find this. In Mandarin, Chinese students hardly differ from those in Beijing. Yet in English, they are not close to either Chinese students in Beijing or British students in the UK. This can be explained by a model of identity priming with monocultural subjects. For Chinese students in the UK, speaking English reduces the pull of a Chinese frame without increasing the pull of a British one. International students do not so much learn foreign preferences as learn to ignore old ones. Our reliance on mostly stated preferences enables a rich dataset covering many domains; future work is needed to see if such large effects are also found for a wide range of revealed preferences.

每年都有数以百万计的学生在国外的大学学习,换一种文化环境。这可能会揭示衡量偏好是固定的还是灵活的,是否可以通过迁移国家或学习一门新语言在短期内改变。我们通过测量国际学生的偏好来理清这些影响。对于在英国的中国学生(他们来英国的时间不超过5年),我们随机选择一种调查语言。我们在每个国家增加参考小组,用相关语言进行调查。简单的比较提供了对语言影响的因果估计和对国家、地区和国籍差异的观察估计。我们发现语言对一系列调查结果有很大的因果影响。效应大小与不同国家或民族的差异相似(0.4个标准差),大于不同地区的差异(0.1个标准差)。同化理论预测,在英国的中国学生在测量偏好上的任何变化都将倾向于英国学生,即使这种变化可能很小。我们找不到这个。在普通话上,中国学生和北京学生几乎没有什么不同。但在英语中,他们与在北京的中国学生或在英国的英国学生都不亲近。这可以用单一文化主体的身份启动模型来解释。对于在英国的中国学生来说,说英语会减少对中国框架的吸引力,而不会增加对英国框架的吸引力。国际学生与其说是学会了外国的偏好,不如说是学会了忽略旧的偏好。我们对大多数陈述偏好的依赖使丰富的数据集涵盖许多领域;未来还需要进一步研究,以确定这种巨大的影响是否也适用于广泛的偏好。
{"title":"Do international students learn foreign preferences? The interplay of language, identity and assimilation","authors":"Paul Clist ,&nbsp;Ying-yi Hong","doi":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102658","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.joep.2023.102658","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Every year millions of students study at foreign universities, swapping one set of cultural surroundings for another. This may reveal whether measured preferences are fixed or flexible, whether they can be altered in the short run by moving country, or learning a new language. We disentangle these influences by measuring international students’ preferences. For Chinese students in the UK (who arrived up to five years previously) we randomise a survey’s language. We add reference groups in each country, doing the survey in the relevant language. Simple comparisons provide a causal estimate of language’s effect and observational estimates of differences by country, location and nationality. We find language has a large causal effect on a range of survey responses. The effect size is similar to differences by country or nationality (at 0.4 standard deviations), and larger than differences by location (at 0.1 standard deviations). Assimilation theories predict any movement in measured preferences for Chinese students in the UK would be towards those of UK students, even if they may be small. We do not find this. In Mandarin, Chinese students hardly differ from those in Beijing. Yet in English, they are not close to either Chinese students in Beijing or British students in the UK. This can be explained by a model of identity priming with monocultural subjects. For Chinese students in the UK, speaking English reduces the pull of a Chinese frame without increasing the pull of a British one. International students do not so much learn foreign preferences as learn to ignore old ones. Our reliance on mostly stated preferences enables a rich dataset covering many domains; future work is needed to see if such large effects are also found for a wide range of revealed preferences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Economic Psychology","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 102658"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48424496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"经济学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Economic Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1