首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Inquiry最新文献

英文 中文
Beyond Awareness: The Many Forms of Implicit Bias and Its Implications 超越意识:内隐偏见的多种形式及其启示
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106752
T. Schmader, Carmelle Bareket-Shavit, A. Baron
In 1969, in his address to the American Psychological Association, George Miller implored the field of psychology to give its science away. For their part, the general public has shown itself to have a thirst for those ideas that might be useful to solving some of our most intractable social problems. Implicit bias is one of those good ideas that has transcended the laboratory, in part because of the positive efforts of Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu). For example, during her failed election campaign in 2016, Hilary Clinton declared, “We all have implicit biases” (Merica, 2016). The world is looking to our field to better understand if implicit bias is a dangerous and prevalent pathogen or a mildly annoying but mostly benign curiosity. It hasn’t helped that within the ivory tower, there is no clear consensus on how to best define the construct (Corneille & H€ utter, 2020). Into this context, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; GLE) introduce their target article. In the title of their article, GLE make a simple plea to scholars in the field: Whatever it is that implicit measures of stereotypes and attitudes capture, we should not label this implicit bias (IB). We completely agree with this conclusion and would like to happily sign our names to the petition to refrain from this usage moving forward (just as we would similarly agree that a measure of any construct and the construct itself are not one in the same). In fact, we made a similar argument in a recent publication stating, “too often the terms ‘implicit associations’ (the strength of the associations between concepts in the mind, measured indirectly by the IAT) and ‘implicit bias’ (disparate treatment that can result from one’s implicit associations with social groups) are used isomorphically (cf. De Houwer, 2019)” (see Pitfall #3 of Schmader, Dennehy, & Baron, 2022). In that article, two of us argued for having greater conceptual clarity over what IB is and outlined different pathways by which bias unfolds, so that interventionists designing tools to mitigate the harms from bias have an effective playbook to work from. In this commentary, we focus our analysis on GLE’s general definition that IB “captures the idea that people may behave in a biased way without being aware that their behavior is biased” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 139). We agree with GLE’s focus on bias as behavior, not on the individual differences assessed with implicit measures. We suggest, however, that this particular definition of IB must go further to clarify the precise role of awareness in the delineation of IB. We argue that any clear conceptualization of bias needs to recognize the process by which stereotypes and attitudes in mind can lead to biased behavior that does harm to others. Drawing from and extending a recently published bias typology (Schmader et al., 2022), we will see that awareness is only one component needed to distinguish implicit from explicit (or intentional
1969年,乔治·米勒在向美国心理协会发表的演讲中恳求心理学界放弃其科学。就他们而言,公众已经表现出对那些可能有助于解决我们一些最棘手的社会问题的想法的渴望。隐性偏见是那些超越实验室的好想法之一,部分原因是隐性项目的积极努力(https://implicit.harvard.edu)。例如,希拉里·克林顿在2016年竞选失败期间宣称,“我们都有隐性偏见”(Merica,2016)。全世界都在关注我们的领域,以更好地了解隐性偏见是一种危险而普遍的病原体,还是一种稍微令人讨厌但大多是善意的好奇心。在象牙塔里,对于如何最好地定义这个结构没有明确的共识,这并没有起到任何作用(Cornelle&H€utter,2020)。在此背景下,Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick(本期;GLE)介绍了他们的目标文章。在他们文章的标题中,GLE向该领域的学者提出了一个简单的呼吁:无论刻板印象和态度的隐含衡量标准是什么,我们都不应该给这种隐含偏见(IB)贴上标签。我们完全同意这一结论,并很乐意在请愿书上签名,以避免这种用法继续下去(就像我们同样同意任何构造的度量和构造本身都不一样)。事实上,我们在最近的一份出版物中提出了类似的论点,“‘内隐联想’(通过IAT间接测量的头脑中概念之间的联想强度)和‘内隐偏见’(一个人与社会群体的内隐联想可能导致的不同待遇)这两个术语经常被同构地使用(参见De Houwer,2019)”(见Schmader、Dennehy和Baron的Pitfall#3,2022)。在那篇文章中,我们两人主张对什么是IB有更清晰的概念,并概述了偏见产生的不同途径,以便设计减轻偏见危害的工具的干预主义者有一个有效的行动手册。在这篇评论中,我们将分析重点放在GLE的一般定义上,即IB“抓住了人们可能在没有意识到自己的行为有偏见的情况下以有偏见的方式行事的想法”(Gawronski等人,本期,第139页)。我们同意GLE关注的是作为行为的偏见,而不是用内隐测量评估的个体差异。然而,我们建议,IB的这一特定定义必须进一步澄清意识在IB描述中的确切作用。我们认为,任何对偏见的明确概念化都需要认识到,脑海中的刻板印象和态度可能导致对他人造成伤害的偏见行为。根据最近发表的偏见类型学(Schmader et al.,2022),我们将看到,意识只是区分隐性偏见和显性(或故意)偏见所需的一个组成部分,即使在这些类别中,偏见也可以采取不同的形式,人们可以意识到过程的某些部分,但不能意识到其他部分。因此,与GLE声称IB的定义非常明确相反,我们认为还有一些模糊之处需要更多的关注。
{"title":"Beyond Awareness: The Many Forms of Implicit Bias and Its Implications","authors":"T. Schmader, Carmelle Bareket-Shavit, A. Baron","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106752","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106752","url":null,"abstract":"In 1969, in his address to the American Psychological Association, George Miller implored the field of psychology to give its science away. For their part, the general public has shown itself to have a thirst for those ideas that might be useful to solving some of our most intractable social problems. Implicit bias is one of those good ideas that has transcended the laboratory, in part because of the positive efforts of Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu). For example, during her failed election campaign in 2016, Hilary Clinton declared, “We all have implicit biases” (Merica, 2016). The world is looking to our field to better understand if implicit bias is a dangerous and prevalent pathogen or a mildly annoying but mostly benign curiosity. It hasn’t helped that within the ivory tower, there is no clear consensus on how to best define the construct (Corneille & H€ utter, 2020). Into this context, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; GLE) introduce their target article. In the title of their article, GLE make a simple plea to scholars in the field: Whatever it is that implicit measures of stereotypes and attitudes capture, we should not label this implicit bias (IB). We completely agree with this conclusion and would like to happily sign our names to the petition to refrain from this usage moving forward (just as we would similarly agree that a measure of any construct and the construct itself are not one in the same). In fact, we made a similar argument in a recent publication stating, “too often the terms ‘implicit associations’ (the strength of the associations between concepts in the mind, measured indirectly by the IAT) and ‘implicit bias’ (disparate treatment that can result from one’s implicit associations with social groups) are used isomorphically (cf. De Houwer, 2019)” (see Pitfall #3 of Schmader, Dennehy, & Baron, 2022). In that article, two of us argued for having greater conceptual clarity over what IB is and outlined different pathways by which bias unfolds, so that interventionists designing tools to mitigate the harms from bias have an effective playbook to work from. In this commentary, we focus our analysis on GLE’s general definition that IB “captures the idea that people may behave in a biased way without being aware that their behavior is biased” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 139). We agree with GLE’s focus on bias as behavior, not on the individual differences assessed with implicit measures. We suggest, however, that this particular definition of IB must go further to clarify the precise role of awareness in the delineation of IB. We argue that any clear conceptualization of bias needs to recognize the process by which stereotypes and attitudes in mind can lead to biased behavior that does harm to others. Drawing from and extending a recently published bias typology (Schmader et al., 2022), we will see that awareness is only one component needed to distinguish implicit from explicit (or intentional","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42675046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Reflections on the Difference Between Implicit Bias and Bias on Implicit Measures 关于内隐偏差与内隐测量偏差差异的思考
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2115729
Bertram Gawronski, A. Ledgerwood, Paul W. Eastwick
We are pleased about the considerable interest in our target article and that there is overwhelming agreement with our central thesis that, if the term implicit is understood as unconscious in reference to bias, implicit bias (IB) should not be equated with bias on implicit measures (BIM) (Cesario, this issue; Corneille & B ena, this issue; Cyrus-Lai et al., this issue; De Houwer & Boddez, this issue; Dovidio & Kunst, this issue; Melnikoff & Kurdi, this issue; Norman & Chen, this issue; Olson & Gill, this issue; Schmader et al., this issue; but see Krajbich, this issue; Ratliff & Smith, this issue). We are also grateful for the insightful commentaries, which continue to advance the field’s thinking on this topic. The comments inspired us to think further about the relation between IB and BIM as well as the implications of a clear distinction between the two. In the current reply, we build on these comments, respond to some critical questions, and clarify some arguments that were insufficiently clear in our target article. Before doing so, we would like to express our appreciation for the extreme thoughtfulness of the commentaries, every single one of which deserves their own detailed response. For the purpose of this reply, we will focus on recurring themes and individual points that we deem most important for moving forward. We start our reply with basic questions about the concept of bias, including the difference between behavioral effects and explanatory mental constructs, the role of social context, goals, and values in evaluating instances of bias, and issues pertaining to the role of social category cues in biased behavior. Expanding on the analysis of the bias construct, the next sections address questions related to the implicitness of bias, including the presumed unconsciousness of BIM, methodological difficulties of studying unconscious effects, and the implications of a broader interpretation of implicit as automatic. The next sections again build on the discussions in the preceding sections, addressing questions about the presumed significance of IB research for understanding societal disparities and the value of BIM research if IB is treated as distinct from BIM. The final section presents our general conclusions from the conversation about our target article and several suggestions on how to move forward. Reflections on Bias
我们很高兴对我们的目标文章有相当大的兴趣,并且与我们的中心论点达成了压倒性的一致意见,即如果隐式一词在提及偏见时被理解为无意识的,隐性偏见(IB)不应等同于对隐性测量的偏见(BIM)(Cesario,本期;Cornelle和B ena,本期,Cyrus Lai等人,本期、De Houwer和Boddez,本期。我们也感谢富有见地的评论,这些评论继续推动该领域对这一主题的思考。这些评论启发我们进一步思考IB和BIM之间的关系,以及两者之间明确区别的含义。在目前的答复中,我们以这些评论为基础,回答了一些关键问题,并澄清了目标文章中不够明确的一些论点。在这样做之前,我们要对评论的极端周到表示赞赏,每一个评论都应该得到详细的回应。为了本答复的目的,我们将集中讨论我们认为对向前发展最重要的反复出现的主题和个别要点。我们从关于偏见概念的基本问题开始回答,包括行为效应和解释性心理结构之间的差异,社会背景、目标和价值观在评估偏见实例中的作用,以及与社会类别线索在偏见行为中的作用有关的问题。在对偏见结构分析的基础上,下一节将讨论与偏见隐含性相关的问题,包括BIM的假定无意识、研究无意识效应的方法学困难,以及将内隐解释为自动的含义。下一节再次以前几节的讨论为基础,讨论IB研究对理解社会差异的假定意义,以及如果IB与BIM不同,则BIM研究的价值等问题。最后一节介绍了我们从关于目标文章的对话中得出的一般结论,以及关于如何前进的几点建议。关于偏见的思考
{"title":"Reflections on the Difference Between Implicit Bias and Bias on Implicit Measures","authors":"Bertram Gawronski, A. Ledgerwood, Paul W. Eastwick","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2115729","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2115729","url":null,"abstract":"We are pleased about the considerable interest in our target article and that there is overwhelming agreement with our central thesis that, if the term implicit is understood as unconscious in reference to bias, implicit bias (IB) should not be equated with bias on implicit measures (BIM) (Cesario, this issue; Corneille & B ena, this issue; Cyrus-Lai et al., this issue; De Houwer & Boddez, this issue; Dovidio & Kunst, this issue; Melnikoff & Kurdi, this issue; Norman & Chen, this issue; Olson & Gill, this issue; Schmader et al., this issue; but see Krajbich, this issue; Ratliff & Smith, this issue). We are also grateful for the insightful commentaries, which continue to advance the field’s thinking on this topic. The comments inspired us to think further about the relation between IB and BIM as well as the implications of a clear distinction between the two. In the current reply, we build on these comments, respond to some critical questions, and clarify some arguments that were insufficiently clear in our target article. Before doing so, we would like to express our appreciation for the extreme thoughtfulness of the commentaries, every single one of which deserves their own detailed response. For the purpose of this reply, we will focus on recurring themes and individual points that we deem most important for moving forward. We start our reply with basic questions about the concept of bias, including the difference between behavioral effects and explanatory mental constructs, the role of social context, goals, and values in evaluating instances of bias, and issues pertaining to the role of social category cues in biased behavior. Expanding on the analysis of the bias construct, the next sections address questions related to the implicitness of bias, including the presumed unconsciousness of BIM, methodological difficulties of studying unconscious effects, and the implications of a broader interpretation of implicit as automatic. The next sections again build on the discussions in the preceding sections, addressing questions about the presumed significance of IB research for understanding societal disparities and the value of BIM research if IB is treated as distinct from BIM. The final section presents our general conclusions from the conversation about our target article and several suggestions on how to move forward. Reflections on Bias","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46426469","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The “Implicit Bias” Wording Is a Relic. Let’s Move On and Study Unconscious Social Categorization Effects “隐性偏见”是一种遗迹。让我们继续研究无意识的社会分类效应
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106754
O. Corneille, J. Béna
In their article, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; hereafter, GLE) explain why “implicit bias” (defined as the unconscious effect of social category cues on behavioral responses) should not be confused with “bias on implicit measures.” We see much value in their clarification and agree with their bleak assessment of research on implicit tasks when they are said to measure “implicit bias” (hereafter “implicit measures of bias”), the most prominent of which is the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). The article opens with a puzzling statement, though. GLE celebrate the educational value of “implicit measures of bias”: “implicit measures of bias deserve enormous credit for providing a tool for the widespread dissemination of the idea that people can be biased without being aware of it” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 139). However, while reading their article, it becomes quickly clear (1) that “implicit measures of bias” have little conceptual consistency, and (2) that critical assumptions underlying their use and interpretation are unsubstantiated (e.g., the assumption that these tasks tap into unconscious mental contents or hold a special relation to associative learning). GLE also note that social cognition research has barely started to study the unconsciousness of category-driven biases beyond responses entered on computer keyboards. It is an open secret that we do not clearly know how to interpret outcomes from “implicit measures of bias” (see, e.g., Fiedler, Messner & Bluemke, 2006). The managers of Project Implicit, the largest educational and researchoriented platform conventionally said to study “implicit biases” feature an honest disclaimer on the website of the platform: the designers of the task, their promoters, and their associated institutions “make no claim for the validity” of their suggested interpretations of IAT scores (https:// implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/takeatest.html). If we want to be honest about it, we do not know much which and when social behaviors are driven by an unconscious influence of social categories either. If social cognition research relied on tasks and study settings that are detached from “implicit biases” (as GLE define them), then this begs the question of how accurate and profitable the education around this notion has been. As a case in point, introductory psychology textbooks generally fail to accurately portray the most prominent “implicit measure of bias” (Bartels & Schoenrade, 2022). We suspect that extraacademic education does not fare better. In the present commentary, we speculate on how we got here, we discuss how bad it can get when scientists conflate science with mere opinions, and we propose ways forward. We argue that strong research on “implicit bias” can finally see the light if drastic changes are implemented in social cognition research, starting with radical terminological changes.
Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick(本期;以下简称GLE)在他们的文章中解释了为什么“内隐偏见”(定义为社会类别线索对行为反应的无意识影响)不应与“内隐测量的偏见”混淆。“我们从他们的澄清中看到了很大的价值,并同意他们对内隐任务研究的悲观评估,因为据说他们测量“内隐偏见”(以下简称“偏见的内隐测量”),其中最突出的是内隐联想测试(IAT;Greenwald、McGhee和Schwartz,1998)。不过,这篇文章的开头有一句令人费解的话。GLE赞扬“隐性偏见衡量标准”的教育价值:“隐性偏见测量标准为广泛传播人们可能在没有意识到的情况下存在偏见的观点提供了工具,值得高度赞扬”(Gawronski等人,本期,第139页)。然而,在阅读他们的文章时,很快就清楚了(1)“隐性偏见测量”在概念上几乎没有一致性,(2)其使用和解释背后的关键假设是未经证实的(例如,假设这些任务利用了无意识的心理内容或与联想学习有特殊关系)。GLE还指出,社会认知研究几乎没有开始研究在电脑键盘上输入的反应之外的类别驱动偏见的无意识。我们不清楚如何解释“隐性偏见测量”的结果,这是一个公开的秘密(例如,见Fiedler、Messner和Bluemke,2006)。Project Implicit是一个最大的教育和研究平台,通常被称为研究“隐性偏见”,其管理者在平台网站上有一个诚实的免责声明:任务的设计者、他们的推动者、,以及他们的相关机构“不声称”他们对IAT分数的建议解释的有效性(https://implict.harvard.edu/implict/takatest.html)。如果我们想诚实地说,我们也不知道哪些社会行为以及何时是由社会类别的无意识影响驱动的。如果社会认知研究依赖于脱离“隐性偏见”(GLE对其的定义)的任务和学习环境,那么这就引出了围绕这一概念的教育有多准确和有利可图的问题。作为一个恰当的例子,心理学入门教材通常无法准确描述最突出的“偏见的隐性衡量标准”(Bartels&Schoenrade,2022)。我们怀疑,校外教育并没有好到哪里去。在本评论中,我们推测了我们是如何走到这一步的,我们讨论了当科学家将科学与纯粹的观点混为一谈时,情况会有多糟糕,并提出了前进的道路。我们认为,如果从彻底的术语变化开始,在社会认知研究中进行剧烈的改变,那么对“内隐偏见”的有力研究最终可以看到曙光。
{"title":"The “Implicit Bias” Wording Is a Relic. Let’s Move On and Study Unconscious Social Categorization Effects","authors":"O. Corneille, J. Béna","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106754","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106754","url":null,"abstract":"In their article, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; hereafter, GLE) explain why “implicit bias” (defined as the unconscious effect of social category cues on behavioral responses) should not be confused with “bias on implicit measures.” We see much value in their clarification and agree with their bleak assessment of research on implicit tasks when they are said to measure “implicit bias” (hereafter “implicit measures of bias”), the most prominent of which is the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). The article opens with a puzzling statement, though. GLE celebrate the educational value of “implicit measures of bias”: “implicit measures of bias deserve enormous credit for providing a tool for the widespread dissemination of the idea that people can be biased without being aware of it” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 139). However, while reading their article, it becomes quickly clear (1) that “implicit measures of bias” have little conceptual consistency, and (2) that critical assumptions underlying their use and interpretation are unsubstantiated (e.g., the assumption that these tasks tap into unconscious mental contents or hold a special relation to associative learning). GLE also note that social cognition research has barely started to study the unconsciousness of category-driven biases beyond responses entered on computer keyboards. It is an open secret that we do not clearly know how to interpret outcomes from “implicit measures of bias” (see, e.g., Fiedler, Messner & Bluemke, 2006). The managers of Project Implicit, the largest educational and researchoriented platform conventionally said to study “implicit biases” feature an honest disclaimer on the website of the platform: the designers of the task, their promoters, and their associated institutions “make no claim for the validity” of their suggested interpretations of IAT scores (https:// implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/takeatest.html). If we want to be honest about it, we do not know much which and when social behaviors are driven by an unconscious influence of social categories either. If social cognition research relied on tasks and study settings that are detached from “implicit biases” (as GLE define them), then this begs the question of how accurate and profitable the education around this notion has been. As a case in point, introductory psychology textbooks generally fail to accurately portray the most prominent “implicit measure of bias” (Bartels & Schoenrade, 2022). We suspect that extraacademic education does not fare better. In the present commentary, we speculate on how we got here, we discuss how bad it can get when scientists conflate science with mere opinions, and we propose ways forward. We argue that strong research on “implicit bias” can finally see the light if drastic changes are implemented in social cognition research, starting with radical terminological changes.","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42887446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What Implicit Measures of Bias Can Do 偏见的隐性测量可以做什么
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106759
David E. Melnikoff, Benedek Kurdi
Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; GLE) bring much needed attention to the limitations of currently available implicit measures as tools for studying unconscious bias. We agree with the authors of the target article that the current state of the literature offers little reason to believe that commonly used implicit measures, such as sequential priming (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986), the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), and the Affect Misattribution Procedure (Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005), capture unconscious influences of social category cues on behavioral responses. If anything, the evidence suggests the opposite: Participants may well be aware of how their responses are influenced by social cues on implicit measures (Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014; Hahn & Gawronski, 2019), although to what degree and as a result of what type of process or processes remains to be investigated (Morris & Kurdi, 2022). Nonetheless, even if the extent of awareness differs depending on the specific conditions of the task, the lack of compelling evidence for the ability of currently available implicit measures to index unconscious bias is surprising. As GLE observe, the concepts of unconscious bias and bias on implicit measures have been, and continue to be, conflated, both in the empirical literature and popular discourse. This conundrum will prompt many readers to wonder: If implicit measures of bias are not useful for measuring unconscious bias, are they useful at all? They are. Whether or not they shed light on unconscious bias, implicit measures have been, and we believe will remain, essential to the study of social cognition. We suspect that the lead author of the target article, who has used implicit measures of bias to make numerous contributions to the understanding of social information processing, would agree. But what is it, exactly, that implicit measures of bias are good for, if not probing the human unconscious? This is the question we address in the current commentary. Broadly speaking, implicit measures of bias have been and continue to be critical for addressing two related questions: (i) what is the nature of unintentional bias? and (ii) what is the cognitive architecture of bias? In what follows, we show how implicit measures fuel progress on both fronts while, crucially, also advancing the translational goal of revealing the nature of, and reducing, groupbased inequality. Using Implicit Measures of Bias to Reveal the Nature of Unintentional Bias
Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick(本期;GLE)引起了人们对当前可用的内隐测量作为研究无意识偏见工具的局限性的迫切关注。我们同意目标文章的作者的观点,即文献的现状几乎没有理由相信常用的内隐测量,如顺序启动(Fazio,Sanbonmatsu,Powell,&Kardes,1986)、内隐联想测试(Greenwald,McGhee,&Schwartz,1998)和情感归因错误程序(Payne,Cheng,Govorun,&Stewart,2005),捕捉社会类别线索对行为反应的无意识影响。如果有什么不同的话,证据表明情况恰恰相反:参与者可能很清楚他们的反应是如何受到社会暗示对隐性测量的影响的(Hahn,Judd,Hirsch,&Blair,2014;Hahn和Gawronski,2019),尽管在多大程度上以及由于何种类型的过程仍有待调查(Morris和Kurdi,2022)。尽管如此,即使意识的程度因任务的具体条件而异,但缺乏令人信服的证据来证明目前可用的隐性指标是否有能力索引无意识偏见,这是令人惊讶的。正如GLE所观察到的,在实证文献和流行话语中,无意识偏见和对内隐测量的偏见的概念一直并将继续被混为一谈。这个难题会让许多读者产生疑问:如果隐性偏见测量对测量无意识偏见没有用处,那么它们有用吗?确实如此。无论它们是否揭示了无意识偏见,内隐测量一直是,我们相信将继续是社会认知研究的关键。我们怀疑,目标文章的主要作者会同意这一观点,他使用了隐含的偏见衡量标准,为理解社会信息处理做出了许多贡献。但是,如果不探究人类的无意识,那么隐性的偏见测量究竟有什么好处呢?这就是我们在当前评注中谈到的问题。从广义上讲,隐性偏见测量一直是并将继续是解决两个相关问题的关键:(i)无意偏见的性质是什么?以及(ii)偏见的认知结构是什么?在接下来的内容中,我们展示了隐性措施如何在两个方面推动进步,同时至关重要的是,还推进了揭示和减少基于群体的不平等的本质的转化目标。利用隐性偏见测量揭示无意偏见的本质
{"title":"What Implicit Measures of Bias Can Do","authors":"David E. Melnikoff, Benedek Kurdi","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106759","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106759","url":null,"abstract":"Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue; GLE) bring much needed attention to the limitations of currently available implicit measures as tools for studying unconscious bias. We agree with the authors of the target article that the current state of the literature offers little reason to believe that commonly used implicit measures, such as sequential priming (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986), the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), and the Affect Misattribution Procedure (Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005), capture unconscious influences of social category cues on behavioral responses. If anything, the evidence suggests the opposite: Participants may well be aware of how their responses are influenced by social cues on implicit measures (Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014; Hahn & Gawronski, 2019), although to what degree and as a result of what type of process or processes remains to be investigated (Morris & Kurdi, 2022). Nonetheless, even if the extent of awareness differs depending on the specific conditions of the task, the lack of compelling evidence for the ability of currently available implicit measures to index unconscious bias is surprising. As GLE observe, the concepts of unconscious bias and bias on implicit measures have been, and continue to be, conflated, both in the empirical literature and popular discourse. This conundrum will prompt many readers to wonder: If implicit measures of bias are not useful for measuring unconscious bias, are they useful at all? They are. Whether or not they shed light on unconscious bias, implicit measures have been, and we believe will remain, essential to the study of social cognition. We suspect that the lead author of the target article, who has used implicit measures of bias to make numerous contributions to the understanding of social information processing, would agree. But what is it, exactly, that implicit measures of bias are good for, if not probing the human unconscious? This is the question we address in the current commentary. Broadly speaking, implicit measures of bias have been and continue to be critical for addressing two related questions: (i) what is the nature of unintentional bias? and (ii) what is the cognitive architecture of bias? In what follows, we show how implicit measures fuel progress on both fronts while, crucially, also advancing the translational goal of revealing the nature of, and reducing, groupbased inequality. Using Implicit Measures of Bias to Reveal the Nature of Unintentional Bias","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49425156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Commentary on Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick, Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick的评论,隐性偏见 ≠ 隐性测量的偏差
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761
M. Olson, L. Gill
The authors of the target article offer a definition of implicit bias as “unconscious effects of social category cues” (Gawronski, Ledgerwood, & Eastwick, this issue, p. 140), and, so defined, make a case for examining its causes, effects, and possible amelioration. We support this pursuit and offer some suggestions on how that might be accomplished. For years we have argued that however implicit bias might be defined, to equate it to the output of a measure that one happens to also call “implicit” or is a bad idea (e.g., Fazio & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2009; Olson & Gill, 2022; Olson & Zabel, 2016). Indeed, we wrote nearly twenty years ago, “We would encourage researchers not to equate an implicitly measured construct with an unconscious one” (Fazio & Olson, 2003, p. 303). Since then, evidence has accumulated that bias on implicit measures is not implicit in the sense of being inaccessible to consciousness. For example, in 2007 we showed that implicitly measured self-esteem was consciously accessible and hence reportable on explicit measures when respondents were implored to be honest (Olson, Fazio, & Hermann, 2007). Similarly, implicitly-assessed antiBlack bias correlates with explicitly-assessed anti-Black bias under conditions of honesty and anonymity (Phillips & Olson, 2014; see also Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014). Nevertheless, we see the need for the authors’ treatise on the problems of conflating implicit bias with bias on an implicit measure, as prominent researchers in these domains persist in equating the two (Greenwald et al., 2022). However, and despite social scientists’ proliferation of near-synonyms, we also want to make a case that a focus on automaticity over implicitness with regard to implicit measures (and, as we will see, implicit bias) has a strong theoretical foundation and empirical support. Before the term “implicit” was popularized and applied to prejudice or attitude measurement, Fazio and colleagues (e.g., Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986) were investigating the automatic activation of attitudes. The evaluative priming measure they developed is probably the second most-used implicit measure, after the IAT. On a given trial in a priming task, a prime (the attitude object, usually in image form) is presented briefly, followed by a clearly valenced target adjective (e.g., wonderful) participants are tasked with identifying as either good or bad by pressing one of two corresponding keys as quickly as possible. This seminal work found that for particularly strong attitudes, primes facilitate the identification of valence-congruent target adjectives: cake primes facilitated identification of positive targets, and death primes facilitated identification of negative targets (inhibition of valence incongruent prime-target pairs was also observed). This facilitation effect is automatic because the activation of participants’ attitudes occurred despite their goal to identify the valence of the targets, not the p
这种态度一旦被激活,就会引发一连串的过程,最终形成一种自发的态度-行为关系:一个怀有反黑人偏见的人
{"title":"Commentary on Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick, Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures","authors":"M. Olson, L. Gill","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761","url":null,"abstract":"The authors of the target article offer a definition of implicit bias as “unconscious effects of social category cues” (Gawronski, Ledgerwood, & Eastwick, this issue, p. 140), and, so defined, make a case for examining its causes, effects, and possible amelioration. We support this pursuit and offer some suggestions on how that might be accomplished. For years we have argued that however implicit bias might be defined, to equate it to the output of a measure that one happens to also call “implicit” or is a bad idea (e.g., Fazio & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2009; Olson & Gill, 2022; Olson & Zabel, 2016). Indeed, we wrote nearly twenty years ago, “We would encourage researchers not to equate an implicitly measured construct with an unconscious one” (Fazio & Olson, 2003, p. 303). Since then, evidence has accumulated that bias on implicit measures is not implicit in the sense of being inaccessible to consciousness. For example, in 2007 we showed that implicitly measured self-esteem was consciously accessible and hence reportable on explicit measures when respondents were implored to be honest (Olson, Fazio, & Hermann, 2007). Similarly, implicitly-assessed antiBlack bias correlates with explicitly-assessed anti-Black bias under conditions of honesty and anonymity (Phillips & Olson, 2014; see also Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014). Nevertheless, we see the need for the authors’ treatise on the problems of conflating implicit bias with bias on an implicit measure, as prominent researchers in these domains persist in equating the two (Greenwald et al., 2022). However, and despite social scientists’ proliferation of near-synonyms, we also want to make a case that a focus on automaticity over implicitness with regard to implicit measures (and, as we will see, implicit bias) has a strong theoretical foundation and empirical support. Before the term “implicit” was popularized and applied to prejudice or attitude measurement, Fazio and colleagues (e.g., Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986) were investigating the automatic activation of attitudes. The evaluative priming measure they developed is probably the second most-used implicit measure, after the IAT. On a given trial in a priming task, a prime (the attitude object, usually in image form) is presented briefly, followed by a clearly valenced target adjective (e.g., wonderful) participants are tasked with identifying as either good or bad by pressing one of two corresponding keys as quickly as possible. This seminal work found that for particularly strong attitudes, primes facilitate the identification of valence-congruent target adjectives: cake primes facilitated identification of positive targets, and death primes facilitated identification of negative targets (inhibition of valence incongruent prime-target pairs was also observed). This facilitation effect is automatic because the activation of participants’ attitudes occurred despite their goal to identify the valence of the targets, not the p","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45581056","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Avoiding Bias in the Search for Implicit Bias 在寻找隐性偏见中避免偏见
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106762
Wilson Cyrus-Lai, W. Tierney, Christilene du Plessis, M. Nguyen, M. Schaerer, Elena Giulia Clemente, E. Uhlmann
To revitalize the study of unconscious bias, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) propose a paradigm shift away from implicit measures of intergroup attitudes and beliefs. Specifically, researchers should capture discriminatory biases and demonstrate that participants are unaware of the influence of social category cues on their judgments and actions. Individual differences in scores on implicit measures will be useful to predict and better understand implicitly prejudiced behaviors, but the latter should be the collective focus of researchers interested in unconscious biases against social groups. We welcome Gawronski et al.’s (this issue) proposal and seek to build on their insights. We begin by summarizing recent empirical challenges to the implicit measurement approach, which has for the last quarter century focused heavily on capturing individual differences and examining their potential antecedents and consequences. In our view, Gawronski et al. (this issue) underestimate the problems the subfield of implicit bias research is currently facing; the need for a paradigm shift in focus and approach is truly urgent. Although we strongly agree with their basic thesis, we also stress the importance of avoiding various forms of potential bias in the search for implicit bias. First, research in this area should leverage open science innovations such as pre-registration of competing predictions to allow for intellectually and ideologically dissonant conclusions of equal treatment and “reverse” discrimination against members of historically privileged groups. Second, in assessing awareness of bias, researchers should avoid equating unconsciousness with the null hypothesis that evidence of awareness will not emerge, and instead seek positive evidence that the behavioral bias is implicit in nature. Finally, to avoid underestimating the pervasiveness of intergroup bias, scientists should continue to develop and attempt to validate implicit measures of attitudes and beliefs, which may tap latent prejudices expressed in only a small subset of overt actions.
为了振兴对无意识偏见的研究,Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick(本期)提出了一种范式转变,从群体间态度和信仰的内隐测量中转移出来。具体来说,研究人员应该捕捉歧视性偏见,并证明参与者没有意识到社会类别线索对他们的判断和行动的影响。内隐测量得分的个体差异将有助于预测和更好地理解隐性偏见行为,但后者应该是对针对社会群体的无意识偏见感兴趣的研究人员的集体关注点。我们欢迎Gawronski等人(本期)的建议,并寻求在他们的见解基础上再接再厉。我们首先总结了最近对内隐测量方法的实证挑战,在过去的四分之一个世纪里,内隐测量法一直专注于捕捉个体差异并研究其潜在的前因和后果。在我们看来,Gawronski等人(本期)低估了内隐偏见研究子领域目前面临的问题;确实迫切需要在重点和方法上进行范式转变。尽管我们强烈同意他们的基本论点,但我们也强调在寻找隐性偏见时避免各种形式的潜在偏见的重要性。首先,这一领域的研究应该利用开放科学创新,例如预先登记相互竞争的预测,以得出在智力和意识形态上不和谐的平等待遇结论,并“扭转”对历史特权群体成员的歧视。其次,在评估偏见意识时,研究人员应避免将无意识等同于意识证据不会出现的无效假设,而是寻求行为偏见本质上是隐含的积极证据。最后,为了避免低估群体间偏见的普遍性,科学家们应该继续开发并尝试验证态度和信仰的隐性测量,这可能会利用仅在一小部分公开行动中表达的潜在偏见。
{"title":"Avoiding Bias in the Search for Implicit Bias","authors":"Wilson Cyrus-Lai, W. Tierney, Christilene du Plessis, M. Nguyen, M. Schaerer, Elena Giulia Clemente, E. Uhlmann","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106762","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106762","url":null,"abstract":"To revitalize the study of unconscious bias, Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) propose a paradigm shift away from implicit measures of intergroup attitudes and beliefs. Specifically, researchers should capture discriminatory biases and demonstrate that participants are unaware of the influence of social category cues on their judgments and actions. Individual differences in scores on implicit measures will be useful to predict and better understand implicitly prejudiced behaviors, but the latter should be the collective focus of researchers interested in unconscious biases against social groups. We welcome Gawronski et al.’s (this issue) proposal and seek to build on their insights. We begin by summarizing recent empirical challenges to the implicit measurement approach, which has for the last quarter century focused heavily on capturing individual differences and examining their potential antecedents and consequences. In our view, Gawronski et al. (this issue) underestimate the problems the subfield of implicit bias research is currently facing; the need for a paradigm shift in focus and approach is truly urgent. Although we strongly agree with their basic thesis, we also stress the importance of avoiding various forms of potential bias in the search for implicit bias. First, research in this area should leverage open science innovations such as pre-registration of competing predictions to allow for intellectually and ideologically dissonant conclusions of equal treatment and “reverse” discrimination against members of historically privileged groups. Second, in assessing awareness of bias, researchers should avoid equating unconsciousness with the null hypothesis that evidence of awareness will not emerge, and instead seek positive evidence that the behavioral bias is implicit in nature. Finally, to avoid underestimating the pervasiveness of intergroup bias, scientists should continue to develop and attempt to validate implicit measures of attitudes and beliefs, which may tap latent prejudices expressed in only a small subset of overt actions.","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48002446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Delight in Disorder: Inclusively Defining and Operationalizing Implicit Bias 混乱中的快乐:内隐偏见的包容性定义和操作
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106756
J. Dovidio, J. Kunst
Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) address a timely issue of both theoretical and practical importance in the burgeoning study of implicit bias. The authors “highlight conceptual and empirical problems with the widespread equation of implicit bias and bias on implicit measures” (p. 139). They are not the first to raise and grapple with a question closely related to deciphering the conceptual meaning of implicit bias and its relationship to measures of implicit bias, but they distinguish themselves with their mastery of diverse literatures, sophisticated analyses of core theoretical issues, and original insights. While maintaining a steady focus on their core question, the authors’ review and synthesis of the work that they cover makes this a valuable resource for various audiences. It provides a detailed, yet accessible introduction for those who are interested in but relatively unfamiliar with the topic, as well as a thought-provoking and well-argued contribution for those who have considerable expertise in the area and may already have well-formed perspectives on the questions posed and answers provided. Importantly, in an area in which heated debate has been common, Gawronski et al. navigate through complex issues with logic and data in an even-handed way. This is an impressive piece of scholarship. A common colloquial expression is, “If the shoe fits, wear it.” The article is particularly impressive in the way the authors examine the many ways that scholars have attempted to define implicit bias. They try on many shoes for the term “implicit,” as compared to “explicit.” Gawronski et al. (this issue) consider distinctions in process, such as in differences between “mental levels.” For instance, they discuss how implicit has been treated as reflecting associative processes “involving unqualified mental links between concepts”, whereas explicit processes are propositional “involving the perceived validity of specific relations” (p. 141). Alternative, procedural distinctions are also reviewed. These tend to be instrument-focused. For example, a measure would qualify as implicit to the extent to which the response is automatic—that is, unintentional and difficult to control. By contrast, an explicit measure would be one in which people respond in a deliberative, intentional, and selfreflective way. Indeed, the first author of this commentary falls into this procedural camp, describing implicit as activation that occurs unintentionally (Dovidio, Kawakami, & Beach, 2001), automatically (Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick, & Esses, 2010), and which can operate without people being aware of the “biased associations or of the role those associations play in guiding their judgment and action” (Greenwald, Dsagupta, et al., 2022, p. 8). However, Gawronski et al. (this issue) skillfully argue how and why none of these shoes fit. In the end, we resonate with Gawronski et al.’s critical conclusion that “despite 25 years of extensive research, the current
Gawronski, Ledgerwood和Eastwick(本期)在新兴的内隐偏见研究中提出了一个具有理论和实践重要性的及时问题。作者“强调了关于内隐偏差和内隐测量偏差的广泛等式的概念和经验问题”(第139页)。他们并不是第一个提出并解决与解读内隐偏见的概念意义及其与内隐偏见测量的关系密切相关的问题的人,但他们以对各种文献的掌握、对核心理论问题的复杂分析和原创性见解而脱颖而出。在保持对其核心问题的稳定关注的同时,作者对他们所涵盖的工作的回顾和综合使其成为各种受众的宝贵资源。它为那些对该主题感兴趣但相对不熟悉的人提供了一个详细的,但易于理解的介绍,也为那些在该领域有相当专业知识并且可能已经对所提出的问题和所提供的答案有良好形成的观点的人提供了一个发人深省和充分论证的贡献。重要的是,在一个激烈争论已经司空见惯的领域,Gawronski等人以一种不偏不倚的方式用逻辑和数据来解决复杂的问题。这是一项令人印象深刻的学术研究。一个常见的口语表达是,“如果鞋子合脚,就穿它。”这篇文章特别令人印象深刻的是,作者对学者们试图定义隐性偏见的许多方式进行了研究。他们试了很多鞋子,是为了“隐性”,而不是“显性”。Gawronski等人(本期)考虑了过程中的差异,例如“心理水平”之间的差异。例如,他们讨论了内隐过程如何被视为反映“涉及概念之间不确定的心理联系”的联想过程,而外显过程是“涉及特定关系的感知有效性”的命题过程(第141页)。还审查了其他程序上的区别。这些倾向于以工具为中心。例如,一种测量方法被认为是隐含的,因为它的反应是自动的,也就是说,是无意的,难以控制的。相比之下,明确的衡量标准是人们以深思熟虑、有意识和自我反思的方式做出反应。事实上,这篇评论的第一作者属于程序性阵营,将隐性激活描述为无意识地(Dovidio, Kawakami, & Beach, 2001)、自动地(Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick, & ess, 2010)发生的激活,并且可以在人们没有意识到“有偏见的联想或这些联想在指导他们的判断和行动中所起的作用”的情况下运行(Greenwald, Dsagupta, et al., 2022, p. 8)。Gawronski等人(本期)巧妙地论证了这些鞋子不合脚的原因和原因。最后,我们与Gawronski等人的关键结论产生共鸣,即“尽管经过了25年的广泛研究,目前的标签惯例仍然基于概念模糊的列表,根据该列表,如果研究人员过去将其描述为隐含的,则该测量具有隐含的资格”(第142页;另见Gawronski, De Houwer, & Sherman, 2020)。虽然我们同意Gawronski, Ledgerwood和Eastwick对当前问题的分析,即内隐偏见的设想和研究方式,但我们的分歧在于提出的解决方案。我们故意使用“分歧”这个词,而不是“不同意”,因为把我们带到这里的观点是完全不同的。不同的观点有不同的假设,决定了不同的优先顺序。Gawronski等人(本期)将内隐偏见视为一种行为现象,可以与内隐测量评估的偏见区分开来。他们写道,“偏见可以定义为社会类别线索(例如,与种族、性别等有关的线索)对行为反应的影响”,“将一个人对目标的行为反应归类为IB[内隐偏见]的实例,一个人必须证明(1)行为反应受到社会类别线索的影响,(2)这个人没有意识到相关的社会类别线索对他们的行为反应的影响”(第5页)。尽管这组定义清晰而直接,但我们也不相信这是正确的。乍一看,Gawronski等人(本期)的定义似乎与Greenwald, Dsagupta等人(2022,p. 8)最近使用的定义非常相似,这是一种偏见
{"title":"Delight in Disorder: Inclusively Defining and Operationalizing Implicit Bias","authors":"J. Dovidio, J. Kunst","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106756","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106756","url":null,"abstract":"Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) address a timely issue of both theoretical and practical importance in the burgeoning study of implicit bias. The authors “highlight conceptual and empirical problems with the widespread equation of implicit bias and bias on implicit measures” (p. 139). They are not the first to raise and grapple with a question closely related to deciphering the conceptual meaning of implicit bias and its relationship to measures of implicit bias, but they distinguish themselves with their mastery of diverse literatures, sophisticated analyses of core theoretical issues, and original insights. While maintaining a steady focus on their core question, the authors’ review and synthesis of the work that they cover makes this a valuable resource for various audiences. It provides a detailed, yet accessible introduction for those who are interested in but relatively unfamiliar with the topic, as well as a thought-provoking and well-argued contribution for those who have considerable expertise in the area and may already have well-formed perspectives on the questions posed and answers provided. Importantly, in an area in which heated debate has been common, Gawronski et al. navigate through complex issues with logic and data in an even-handed way. This is an impressive piece of scholarship. A common colloquial expression is, “If the shoe fits, wear it.” The article is particularly impressive in the way the authors examine the many ways that scholars have attempted to define implicit bias. They try on many shoes for the term “implicit,” as compared to “explicit.” Gawronski et al. (this issue) consider distinctions in process, such as in differences between “mental levels.” For instance, they discuss how implicit has been treated as reflecting associative processes “involving unqualified mental links between concepts”, whereas explicit processes are propositional “involving the perceived validity of specific relations” (p. 141). Alternative, procedural distinctions are also reviewed. These tend to be instrument-focused. For example, a measure would qualify as implicit to the extent to which the response is automatic—that is, unintentional and difficult to control. By contrast, an explicit measure would be one in which people respond in a deliberative, intentional, and selfreflective way. Indeed, the first author of this commentary falls into this procedural camp, describing implicit as activation that occurs unintentionally (Dovidio, Kawakami, & Beach, 2001), automatically (Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick, & Esses, 2010), and which can operate without people being aware of the “biased associations or of the role those associations play in guiding their judgment and action” (Greenwald, Dsagupta, et al., 2022, p. 8). However, Gawronski et al. (this issue) skillfully argue how and why none of these shoes fit. In the end, we resonate with Gawronski et al.’s critical conclusion that “despite 25 years of extensive research, the current","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44465910","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures 隐式偏差 ≠ 隐性测量的偏差
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106750
Bertram Gawronski, A. Ledgerwood, Paul W. Eastwick
Abstract People can behave in a biased manner without being aware that their behavior is biased, an idea commonly referred to as implicit bias. Research on implicit bias has been heavily influenced by implicit measures, in that implicit bias is often equated with bias on implicit measures. Drawing on a definition of implicit bias as an unconscious effect of social category cues on behavioral responses, the current article argues that the widespread equation of implicit bias and bias on implicit measures is problematic on conceptual and empirical grounds. A clear separation of the two constructs will: (1) resolve ambiguities arising from the multiple meanings implied by current terminological conventions; (2) stimulate new research by uncovering important questions that have been largely ignored; (3) provide a better foundation for theories of implicit bias through greater conceptual precision; and (4) highlight the broader significance of implicit bias in a manner that is not directly evident from bias on implicit measures.
摘要人们可以在没有意识到自己的行为有偏见的情况下以有偏见的方式行事,这种想法通常被称为隐性偏见。对内隐偏见的研究在很大程度上受到了内隐测量的影响,因为内隐偏见往往等同于对内隐度量的偏见。本文将内隐偏见定义为社会类别线索对行为反应的无意识影响,认为内隐偏见和对内隐测量的偏见的普遍等式在概念和经验上都存在问题。这两个结构的明确分离将:(1)解决当前术语惯例所隐含的多重含义所产生的歧义;(2) 通过发现基本上被忽视的重要问题来激发新的研究;(3) 通过更高的概念精度为隐性偏见理论提供更好的基础;以及(4)强调隐性偏见的更广泛意义,这种方式从隐性测量的偏见中并不直接明显。
{"title":"Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures","authors":"Bertram Gawronski, A. Ledgerwood, Paul W. Eastwick","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106750","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106750","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract People can behave in a biased manner without being aware that their behavior is biased, an idea commonly referred to as implicit bias. Research on implicit bias has been heavily influenced by implicit measures, in that implicit bias is often equated with bias on implicit measures. Drawing on a definition of implicit bias as an unconscious effect of social category cues on behavioral responses, the current article argues that the widespread equation of implicit bias and bias on implicit measures is problematic on conceptual and empirical grounds. A clear separation of the two constructs will: (1) resolve ambiguities arising from the multiple meanings implied by current terminological conventions; (2) stimulate new research by uncovering important questions that have been largely ignored; (3) provide a better foundation for theories of implicit bias through greater conceptual precision; and (4) highlight the broader significance of implicit bias in a manner that is not directly evident from bias on implicit measures.","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42907651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Bias in Implicit Measures as Instances of Biased Behavior under Suboptimal Conditions in the Laboratory 在实验室的次优条件下,隐式测量中的偏差作为偏差行为的实例
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106755
J. de Houwer, Y. Boddez
The target paper of Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) provides a valuable contribution to the literature on implicit bias (IB). We find ourselves in agreement with many of the points that the authors put forward. Most importantly, we agree that it is important to realize that scores on implicit measurement tasks such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) cannot by default be interpreted as instances of unconscious bias. We also agree that the focus on bias in implicit measures (BIM) may have slowed progress in research on IB, that the focus of bias research should be on reducing real-world instances of bias, and that societal dispar-ities can result in social discrimination in a way that is not captured by the psychological concept of bias. We are happy to see that Gawronski et al. share many aspects of our perspective on IB and implicit measures (see De Houwer, 2006, 2014, 2019; De Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba, Spruyt, & Moors, 2009, De Houwer, Gawronski, & Barnes-Holmes, 2013; De Houwer, Van Dessel, & Moran, 2021). Most importantly, (a) IB can indeed be conceived of as a behavioral phenomenon that refers to the impact of social cues on behavior, and (b) implicit measures are not the same as indirect measures, nor do they necessarily reflect associative processes. In sum, we support much of what Gawronski et al. put forward in their target paper. Nevertheless, we also disagree with Gawronski et al. (this issue) on some points. First, we continue to believe IB should not be limited to unconscious bias but should include also instances of bias that are automatic in other ways (e.g., unintentional). Second, we continue to see a potential role for BIM in research on IB, more specifically as an educational tool and as a lab model of IB in the real world.
Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick的目标论文(本期)为内隐偏见(IB)文献提供了宝贵的贡献。我们发现自己同意作者提出的许多观点。最重要的是,我们一致认为,重要的是要认识到,内隐测量任务(如内隐关联测试(IAT))的分数在默认情况下不能被解释为无意识偏见的例子。我们也同意,对内隐测量中的偏见的关注可能减缓了IB研究的进展,偏见研究的重点应该是减少现实世界中的偏见,社会差异可能会导致社会歧视,而偏见的心理概念无法捕捉到这种歧视。我们很高兴看到Gawronski等人分享了我们对IB和隐性测量的许多观点(见De Houwer,2006、2014、2019;De Houwer、Teige Mocigenba、Spruyt和Moors,2009年;De Houower、Gawronsky和Barnes Holmes,2013年;De Hoower、Van Dessel和Moran,2021)。最重要的是,(a)IB确实可以被认为是一种行为现象,指的是社会线索对行为的影响,(b)内隐测量与间接测量不同,也不一定反映联想过程。总之,我们支持Gawronski等人在他们的目标论文中提出的大部分内容。尽管如此,我们在某些方面也不同意Gawronski等人(这个问题)的观点。首先,我们仍然认为IB不应局限于无意识的偏见,还应包括以其他方式自动产生的偏见(例如,无意的)。其次,我们继续看到BIM在IB研究中的潜在作用,更具体地说,它是一种教育工具,也是IB在现实世界中的实验室模型。
{"title":"Bias in Implicit Measures as Instances of Biased Behavior under Suboptimal Conditions in the Laboratory","authors":"J. de Houwer, Y. Boddez","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106755","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106755","url":null,"abstract":"The target paper of Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) provides a valuable contribution to the literature on implicit bias (IB). We find ourselves in agreement with many of the points that the authors put forward. Most importantly, we agree that it is important to realize that scores on implicit measurement tasks such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) cannot by default be interpreted as instances of unconscious bias. We also agree that the focus on bias in implicit measures (BIM) may have slowed progress in research on IB, that the focus of bias research should be on reducing real-world instances of bias, and that societal dispar-ities can result in social discrimination in a way that is not captured by the psychological concept of bias. We are happy to see that Gawronski et al. share many aspects of our perspective on IB and implicit measures (see De Houwer, 2006, 2014, 2019; De Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba, Spruyt, & Moors, 2009, De Houwer, Gawronski, & Barnes-Holmes, 2013; De Houwer, Van Dessel, & Moran, 2021). Most importantly, (a) IB can indeed be conceived of as a behavioral phenomenon that refers to the impact of social cues on behavior, and (b) implicit measures are not the same as indirect measures, nor do they necessarily reflect associative processes. In sum, we support much of what Gawronski et al. put forward in their target paper. Nevertheless, we also disagree with Gawronski et al. (this issue) on some points. First, we continue to believe IB should not be limited to unconscious bias but should include also instances of bias that are automatic in other ways (e.g., unintentional). Second, we continue to see a potential role for BIM in research on IB, more specifically as an educational tool and as a lab model of IB in the real world.","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47285513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Grappling with Social Complexity When Defining and Assessing Implicit Bias 在定义和评估内隐偏见时应对社会复杂性
IF 9.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106760
Jasmine B. Norman, Jacqueline M. Chen
The target article by Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) presents a thorough overview of the intergroup bias literature, honing in on issues that are both conceptual and methodological. In order to address these issues, Gawronski et al. present some new conceptual definitions and distinctions. One central definition provided is of implicit bias, defined as “unconscious effects of social category cues (e.g., cues related to race, gender, etc.) on behavioral responses” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 140). The target article subsequently discusses the implications of this definition for methodology in detail. Our commentary highlights important considerations for different aspects of the target article’s definition of implicit bias. First, we outline the complexity of a seemingly straightforward part of this definition: social category cues. We consider the implications of categorical ambiguity in relation to the current definition of bias. Further, we propose that disparate impact and the importance of social context must be definitional to implicit bias. We provide an argument for how social and structural context are inseparable from social category cues and behavior. Second, turning our attention to the criterion of unconsciousness, we discuss and illustrate the challenges of measuring constructs that are under awareness and, informed by other fields, attempt to provide some solutions.
Gawronski, Ledgerwood和Eastwick的目标文章(本期)对群体间偏见文献进行了全面概述,重点关注概念和方法上的问题。为了解决这些问题,Gawronski等人提出了一些新的概念定义和区别。提供的一个中心定义是内隐偏见,定义为“社会类别线索(例如,与种族,性别等有关的线索)对行为反应的无意识影响”(Gawronski等人,本期,第140页)。目标文章随后将详细讨论该定义对方法论的含义。我们的评论强调了目标文章对内隐偏见定义的不同方面的重要考虑。首先,我们概述了这个定义中一个看似简单的部分的复杂性:社会类别线索。我们考虑与当前偏见定义相关的分类歧义的含义。此外,我们提出差异性影响和社会背景的重要性必须定义内隐偏见。我们为社会和结构背景如何与社会类别线索和行为不可分割提供了一个论据。其次,将我们的注意力转向无意识的标准,我们讨论并说明了测量在意识下的构念的挑战,并通过其他领域的信息,试图提供一些解决方案。
{"title":"Grappling with Social Complexity When Defining and Assessing Implicit Bias","authors":"Jasmine B. Norman, Jacqueline M. Chen","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106760","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106760","url":null,"abstract":"The target article by Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick (this issue) presents a thorough overview of the intergroup bias literature, honing in on issues that are both conceptual and methodological. In order to address these issues, Gawronski et al. present some new conceptual definitions and distinctions. One central definition provided is of implicit bias, defined as “unconscious effects of social category cues (e.g., cues related to race, gender, etc.) on behavioral responses” (Gawronski et al., this issue, p. 140). The target article subsequently discusses the implications of this definition for methodology in detail. Our commentary highlights important considerations for different aspects of the target article’s definition of implicit bias. First, we outline the complexity of a seemingly straightforward part of this definition: social category cues. We consider the implications of categorical ambiguity in relation to the current definition of bias. Further, we propose that disparate impact and the importance of social context must be definitional to implicit bias. We provide an argument for how social and structural context are inseparable from social category cues and behavior. Second, turning our attention to the criterion of unconsciousness, we discuss and illustrate the challenges of measuring constructs that are under awareness and, informed by other fields, attempt to provide some solutions.","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":9.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41853818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Psychological Inquiry
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1