首页 > 最新文献

European Journal of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Perspective-taking predicts success in coalition formation 透视预测联盟组建的成功与否
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3091
Anabela Cantiani, Ilja van Beest, Frans Cruijssen, Goos Kant, Thorsten M. Erle

Predominant economic theories of coalition formation assume that the best strategy during coalition negotiations is to approach as few partners as possible with self-serving offers that barely beat all other competitors while maximizing one's profit. Accordingly, to be included in a winning coalition, one needs to be able to predict others’ intentions, which is called perspective-taking. However, economists derived the idea that these strategies lead to success from observed coalition outcomes, rather than testing the role of perspective-taking empirically or modeling coalition negotiation dynamics. The present research revisits these theories from a psychological perspective within a novel coalition formation game that mimics common problems in horizontal supply chain collaboration. In line with theories of coalition formation, perspective-taking during the negotiation predicted higher inclusion in winning coalitions and higher monetary payoffs, but unexpectedly also longer negotiations. Contrary to predominant theories, perspective-taking led individuals to approach more partners and make other-serving offers, suggesting that successful coalition negotiations rely on different mechanisms than previously thought. These results are also practically relevant because companies often fail to orchestrate their transport flows, which leads to economic losses and environmental damage. These insights are valuable as they call for a psychological revision of predominant theories on coalition formation and shed light on how social behaviour influences horizontal collaboration in the transport sector. Furthermore, these findings underscore the potential of coalition-driven approaches in mitigating environmental consequences, offering a pathway toward sustainable practices in the face of climate challenges.

关于联盟形成的主流经济学理论认为,联盟谈判中的最佳策略是尽可能少地与合作伙伴接触,提出既能勉强击败所有其他竞争者,又能使自身利益最大化的自利提议。因此,要想加入获胜联盟,就必须能够预测他人的意图,这就是所谓的 "透视"(perspective-taking)。然而,经济学家们从观察到的联盟结果中得出了这些策略会带来成功的观点,而不是通过实证或联盟谈判动态模型来检验透视法的作用。本研究在模拟横向供应链合作中常见问题的新型联盟形成游戏中,从心理学角度重新审视了这些理论。与联盟形成理论一致的是,谈判过程中的视角选择预示着更高的获胜联盟加入率和更高的货币报酬,但出乎意料的是,谈判时间也更长。与主流理论相反,视角选择导致个体接触更多的合作伙伴并提出为他人服务的提议,这表明成功的联盟谈判所依赖的机制与之前所认为的不同。这些结果也具有现实意义,因为公司经常无法协调其运输流,从而导致经济损失和环境破坏。这些见解很有价值,因为它们要求对联盟形成的主要理论进行心理修正,并阐明社会行为如何影响运输行业的横向合作。此外,这些发现强调了联盟驱动方法在减轻环境后果方面的潜力,为面对气候挑战的可持续做法提供了一条途径。
{"title":"Perspective-taking predicts success in coalition formation","authors":"Anabela Cantiani,&nbsp;Ilja van Beest,&nbsp;Frans Cruijssen,&nbsp;Goos Kant,&nbsp;Thorsten M. Erle","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3091","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3091","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Predominant economic theories of coalition formation assume that the best strategy during coalition negotiations is to approach as few partners as possible with self-serving offers that barely beat all other competitors while maximizing one's profit. Accordingly, to be included in a winning coalition, one needs to be able to predict others’ intentions, which is called perspective-taking. However, economists derived the idea that these strategies lead to success from observed coalition outcomes, rather than testing the role of perspective-taking empirically or modeling coalition negotiation dynamics. The present research revisits these theories from a psychological perspective within a novel coalition formation game that mimics common problems in horizontal supply chain collaboration. In line with theories of coalition formation, perspective-taking during the negotiation predicted higher inclusion in winning coalitions and higher monetary payoffs, but unexpectedly also longer negotiations. Contrary to predominant theories, perspective-taking led individuals to approach more partners and make other-serving offers, suggesting that successful coalition negotiations rely on different mechanisms than previously thought. These results are also practically relevant because companies often fail to orchestrate their transport flows, which leads to economic losses and environmental damage. These insights are valuable as they call for a psychological revision of predominant theories on coalition formation and shed light on how social behaviour influences horizontal collaboration in the transport sector. Furthermore, these findings underscore the potential of coalition-driven approaches in mitigating environmental consequences, offering a pathway toward sustainable practices in the face of climate challenges.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1364-1377"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3091","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141745856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does powerlessness motivate men and women to endorse sexism? 无力感是否会促使男性和女性认可性别歧视?
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-07-14 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3092
Matthew D. Hammond, Nickola C. Overall, Chris G. Sibley

People who feel powerless are motivated to gain power, which may include men endorsing hostile sexism to affirm societal power or women endorsing benevolent sexism to affirm power in relationships. We used four waves of an annual longitudinal panel sample (= 58,405) to test whether within-person changes in powerlessness predicted subsequent changes in men's hostile sexism and women's benevolent sexism. Results from a random intercept cross-lagged panel model indicated that men who generally felt more powerless tended to endorse hostile sexism, but within-person tests did not provide directional evidence linking men's powerlessness with hostile sexism. By contrast, women who generally felt more powerless endorsed benevolent sexism more strongly, and small within-person lagged effects indicated that upward deviations in women's powerlessness in 1 year predicted stronger endorsement of benevolent sexism the following year. These results provide novel evidence that powerlessness motivates women's benevolent sexism and their toleration of gender inequalities.

感到无能为力的人有获得权力的动机,这可能包括男性赞同敌意性别主义以肯定社会权力,或女性赞同仁慈性别主义以肯定人际关系中的权力。我们使用了四波年度纵向面板样本(N = 58,405)来检验个人内部的无力感变化是否会预测男性敌意性别歧视和女性善意性别歧视的后续变化。随机截距交叉滞后面板模型的结果表明,普遍感到更无力的男性倾向于赞同敌意性别歧视,但个人内部测试并未提供将男性的无力感与敌意性别歧视联系起来的方向性证据。与此相反,普遍感到更无力的女性则更强烈地赞同仁慈的性别歧视,而微小的人内滞后效应表明,一年内女性无力感的上升偏差预示着第二年女性会更强烈地赞同仁慈的性别歧视。这些结果提供了新的证据,证明无权感会促使女性产生善意的性别歧视并容忍性别不平等。
{"title":"Does powerlessness motivate men and women to endorse sexism?","authors":"Matthew D. Hammond,&nbsp;Nickola C. Overall,&nbsp;Chris G. Sibley","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3092","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3092","url":null,"abstract":"<p>People who feel powerless are motivated to gain power, which may include men endorsing hostile sexism to affirm societal power or women endorsing benevolent sexism to affirm power in relationships. We used four waves of an annual longitudinal panel sample (<i>N </i>= 58,405) to test whether within-person changes in powerlessness predicted subsequent changes in men's hostile sexism and women's benevolent sexism. Results from a random intercept cross-lagged panel model indicated that men who generally felt more powerless tended to endorse hostile sexism, but within-person tests did not provide directional evidence linking men's powerlessness with hostile sexism. By contrast, women who generally felt more powerless endorsed benevolent sexism more strongly, and small within-person lagged effects indicated that upward deviations in women's powerlessness in 1 year predicted stronger endorsement of benevolent sexism the following year. These results provide novel evidence that powerlessness motivates women's benevolent sexism and their toleration of gender inequalities.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1354-1363"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3092","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141650083","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Majority residents’ perceptions of ‘economic’ and ‘political’ refugees: Psychological processes underlying the flight-reason bias 多数居民对 "经济 "和 "政治 "难民的看法:逃离原因偏见背后的心理过程
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-07-07 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3088
Judith Knausenberger, Jens H. Hellmann, Franziska A. Stanke, Inga Bechler, Lea Sophie C. Haarmann, Marvin Jehn, Gerald Echterhoff

Migrants who flee their home country for economic (vs. political) reasons often elicit more negative responses in receiving countries. We examined psychological processes underlying this flight-reason bias, focusing on majority residents’ perceptions of migration circumstances and their emotional responses. In three preregistered studies (total N = 1394), participants read texts about an individual refugee (Studies 1 and 2) or groups of refugees (Study 3) who fled because of economic versus political reasons. Overall, participants perceived migration as less forced in the economic-flight reason condition. Economic (vs. political) flight reasons also elicited less willingness to accept refugees. Additional analyses suggest that the flight-reason bias was driven by greater perceived control of refugees over emigration (for perceived forcedness), by lower empathy (for willingness to accept refugees) and by lower perceived pre-migration perils (for both outcomes) in the economic-flight reason conditions. We discuss implications for political and humanitarian issues surrounding refugee immigration and integration.

出于经济(与政治)原因逃离母国的移民往往会在接收国引起更多负面反应。我们研究了这种逃离原因偏差的心理过程,重点关注多数居民对移民情况的看法及其情绪反应。在三项预先登记的研究中(总人数 = 1394),参与者阅读了有关难民个人(研究 1 和 2)或难民群体(研究 3)的文章,他们分别是因为经济原因和政治原因而逃离。总体而言,在经济原因条件下,参与者认为移民的被迫性较小。经济(相对于政治)原因也会降低接受难民的意愿。其他分析表明,在经济--逃离原因条件下,逃离原因偏差的驱动因素包括:难民对移民的控制感更强(就被迫感而言)、移情能力较低(就接受难民的意愿而言)以及移民前的危险感较低(就这两种结果而言)。我们讨论了围绕难民移民和融入社会的政治和人道主义问题的影响。
{"title":"Majority residents’ perceptions of ‘economic’ and ‘political’ refugees: Psychological processes underlying the flight-reason bias","authors":"Judith Knausenberger,&nbsp;Jens H. Hellmann,&nbsp;Franziska A. Stanke,&nbsp;Inga Bechler,&nbsp;Lea Sophie C. Haarmann,&nbsp;Marvin Jehn,&nbsp;Gerald Echterhoff","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3088","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3088","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Migrants who flee their home country for economic (vs. political) reasons often elicit more negative responses in receiving countries. We examined psychological processes underlying this flight-reason bias, focusing on majority residents’ perceptions of migration circumstances and their emotional responses. In three preregistered studies (total <i>N =</i> 1394), participants read texts about an individual refugee (Studies 1 and 2) or groups of refugees (Study 3) who fled because of economic versus political reasons. Overall, participants perceived migration as less forced in the economic-flight reason condition. Economic (vs. political) flight reasons also elicited less willingness to accept refugees. Additional analyses suggest that the flight-reason bias was driven by greater perceived control of refugees over emigration (for perceived forcedness), by lower empathy (for willingness to accept refugees) and by lower perceived pre-migration perils (for both outcomes) in the economic-flight reason conditions. We discuss implications for political and humanitarian issues surrounding refugee immigration and integration.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1335-1353"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3088","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141576112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The true self in reflections? Visual self-recognition and the Big Five 反思中的真实自我?视觉自我识别与五大要素
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-25 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3089
Elena Stephan, Mirella Walker

Typically, individuals are motivated to see themselves in a flattering light regarding their physical and personality characteristics. We suggest that visual self-perception is anchored in the true self which is largely the self-enhancing self, and may also reflect the privileged access to one's traits and internal states. Here, we examine the impact and limits of self-enhancement motivation and egocentric bias on individuals’ recognition of variations of their portraits in terms of the Big Five personality dimensions. We predict that individuals recognise themselves better in positively altered portraits, namely, portraits looking more agreeable, conscientious and open. We also predict that self-enhancement is anchored in reality: Individuals recognise themselves less in strongly altered than in slightly or non-altered portraits. In Study 1, we measure the directions of self-recognition distortions by investigating in which random variations of their portraits individuals recognise themselves. In Study 2, relying on a mouse tracking paradigm, we additionally measure the degree of distortion, and the conflict individuals experience during the self-recognition task. Results of both studies mainly demonstrate self-recognition distortions towards agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness and neuroticism. Moreover, results from Study 2 reveal that self-recognition is anchored in reality. Findings of two studies applying different methods convergently show systematic biases in self-recognition regarding the Big Five dimensions, both enriching understanding of the self-enhancement processes and demonstrating that the methods applied might be useful for future research focusing on self-recognition.

通常情况下,个人都希望自己的身体和个性特征能够得到他人的认可。我们认为,视觉上的自我感知是以真实自我为基础的,而真实自我在很大程度上是自我提升的自我,同时也可能反映出一个人对自己的特质和内在状态的优先访问权。在此,我们从五大人格维度出发,研究了自我提升动机和自我中心偏差对个体识别其肖像变化的影响和限制。我们预测,在积极改变的肖像中,即看起来更合群、更认真和更开放的肖像中,个体能更好地识别自己。我们还预测,自我提升是以现实为基础的:与轻微改变或未改变的肖像相比,个体在强烈改变的肖像中对自己的认可度较低。在研究 1 中,我们通过调查个体在哪种随机变化的肖像中认识到自己,来测量自我认知扭曲的方向。在研究 2 中,我们依靠鼠标跟踪范式,额外测量了失真的程度,以及个体在自我识别任务中经历的冲突。两项研究的结果都表明,自我认知的失真主要集中在合意性、自觉性、开放性和神经质方面。此外,研究 2 的结果表明,自我认知是以现实为基础的。两项采用不同方法的研究结果共同显示了自我认知在大五维度上的系统性偏差,这既丰富了对自我提升过程的理解,也表明所采用的方法可能对未来以自我认知为重点的研究有用。
{"title":"The true self in reflections? Visual self-recognition and the Big Five","authors":"Elena Stephan,&nbsp;Mirella Walker","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3089","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3089","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Typically, individuals are motivated to see themselves in a flattering light regarding their physical and personality characteristics. We suggest that visual self-perception is anchored in the true self which is largely the self-enhancing self, and may also reflect the privileged access to one's traits and internal states. Here, we examine the impact and limits of self-enhancement motivation and egocentric bias on individuals’ recognition of variations of their portraits in terms of the Big Five personality dimensions. We predict that individuals recognise themselves better in positively altered portraits, namely, portraits looking more agreeable, conscientious and open. We also predict that self-enhancement is anchored in reality: Individuals recognise themselves less in strongly altered than in slightly or non-altered portraits. In Study 1, we measure the <i>directions of self-recognition distortions</i> by investigating in which random variations of their portraits individuals recognise themselves. In Study 2, relying on a mouse tracking paradigm, we additionally measure the <i>degree of distortion</i>, and the <i>conflict</i> individuals experience during the self-recognition task. Results of both studies mainly demonstrate self-recognition distortions towards agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness and neuroticism. Moreover, results from Study 2 reveal that self-recognition is anchored in reality. Findings of two studies applying different methods convergently show systematic biases in self-recognition regarding the Big Five dimensions, both enriching understanding of the self-enhancement processes and demonstrating that the methods applied might be useful for future research focusing on self-recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1311-1334"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3089","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141529046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The minority extremity bias 少数极端偏见
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3084
Yvonne Emig, Hans-Peter Erb

Entities of low prevalence are evaluated as more extreme in their characteristics than those of high prevalence. We extended this idea to minorities, which by definition have fewer members than majorities, and found converging evidence for a minority extremity bias (MEB; N = 957). Participants associated the term ‘minority’ (rather than ‘majority’) with higher extremity (Study 1). Minorities were associated with higher extremity than majorities (Study 2). We added a manipulation of the minority size (Study 3, Part 1). Results indicated that the MEB is stronger for very small minorities than for small minorities. Participants assigned extreme behaviour more often to a minority than to a majority (Study 3, Part 2). We extended the MEB to more extreme ratings of minorities on evaluation scales (Study 4). Prevalence of group members correlated negatively with the extremity of group stereotyping (n = 118 groups; Study 5). Participants rated minorities on induced stereotypes more extreme than majorities (Study 6). We discuss the MEB in the context of alternative explanations such as the outgroup extremity effect. Taken together, this research demonstrates the MEB, which is a novel exploration in the realm of group evaluation that contributes to existing literature.

与流行率高的实体相比,流行率低的实体在特征上更极端。我们将这一观点延伸到了少数群体(顾名思义,少数群体的成员数量少于多数群体),并发现了少数群体极端性偏差(MEB;N = 957)的一致证据。参与者将 "少数群体"(而非 "多数群体")与较高的极端性联系起来(研究 1)。与多数人相比,少数人与更高的极值相关(研究 2)。我们增加了对少数群体规模的操纵(研究 3,第 1 部分)。结果表明,极少数人的 MEB 比少数人的 MEB 更强。参与者将极端行为分配给少数群体的频率高于分配给多数群体的频率(研究 3,第 2 部分)。我们将 MEB 扩展到评价量表中对少数群体的更极端评价(研究 4)。群体成员的普遍性与群体刻板印象的极端性呈负相关(n = 118 个群体;研究 5)。参与者在诱导刻板印象中对少数群体的评价比多数群体更极端(研究 6)。我们结合其他解释(如外群体极端效应)讨论了 MEB。总之,这项研究证明了 MEB,它是群体评价领域的一项新探索,为现有文献做出了贡献。
{"title":"The minority extremity bias","authors":"Yvonne Emig,&nbsp;Hans-Peter Erb","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3084","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3084","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Entities of low prevalence are evaluated as more extreme in their characteristics than those of high prevalence. We extended this idea to minorities, which by definition have fewer members than majorities, and found converging evidence for a <i>minority extremity bias</i> (MEB; <i>N</i> = 957). Participants associated the term ‘minority’ (rather than ‘majority’) with higher extremity (Study 1). Minorities were associated with higher extremity than majorities (Study 2). We added a manipulation of the minority size (Study 3, Part 1). Results indicated that the MEB is stronger for very small minorities than for small minorities. Participants assigned extreme behaviour more often to a minority than to a majority (Study 3, Part 2). We extended the MEB to more extreme ratings of minorities on evaluation scales (Study 4). Prevalence of group members correlated negatively with the extremity of group stereotyping (<i>n</i> = 118 groups; Study 5). Participants rated minorities on induced stereotypes more extreme than majorities (Study 6). We discuss the MEB in the context of alternative explanations such as the outgroup extremity effect. Taken together, this research demonstrates the MEB, which is a novel exploration in the realm of group evaluation that contributes to existing literature.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1296-1310"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3084","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141504626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
‘Hunger happens elsewhere, here malnutrition results from lack of proper care’: Social representations of malnutrition and processes of Othering in the Nepalese press 饥饿发生在别处,这里的营养不良是因为缺乏适当的照顾":尼泊尔新闻界对营养不良的社会表述和他者化过程
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3087
Sabine Caillaud, Sofia Payotte

Malnutrition gives rise to stigmatisation worldwide and is a pressing societal issue. Drawing on social representation theory and on the process of othering, a lexicometric analysis of Nepalese press articles (N = 440) was conducted to explore if, and how, Othering of the malnourished is at play and how articles (re)construct the threat of malnutrition. The results indicate that Othering takes different forms depending on the threat the malnourished represent: (a) the malnourished in foreign countries are perceived as suffering from hunger due to a lack of social development and democracy whereas (b) the malnourished in Nepal are perceived as being afflicted because they hold traditional beliefs. However, (c) when the threat can be attributed to natural disasters, no negative attributes were associated with the malnourished in Nepal. This suggests that Othering is neither systematic nor monolithic. These findings illustrate how social psychology can address urgent societal questions while equally offering an opportunity for new theoretical developments.

营养不良在世界范围内引起了鄙视,是一个紧迫的社会问题。根据社会表征理论和 "他者化 "过程,我们对尼泊尔的报刊文章(440 篇)进行了词汇分析,以探讨是否以及如何对营养不良者进行 "他者化",以及文章如何(重新)构建营养不良的威胁。结果表明,"他者化 "因营养不良者所代表的威胁不同而表现出不同的形式:(a) 外国的营养不良者被视为因缺乏社会发展和民主而遭受饥饿,而 (b) 尼泊尔的营养不良者则被视为因持有传统信仰而遭受痛苦。然而,(c) 当威胁可归因于自然灾害时,尼泊尔的营养不良者就没有负面属性了。这表明,他者化既不是系统的,也不是单一的。这些发现说明了社会心理学如何能够解决紧迫的社会问题,同时也为新的理论发展提供了机会。
{"title":"‘Hunger happens elsewhere, here malnutrition results from lack of proper care’: Social representations of malnutrition and processes of Othering in the Nepalese press","authors":"Sabine Caillaud,&nbsp;Sofia Payotte","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3087","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3087","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Malnutrition gives rise to stigmatisation worldwide and is a pressing societal issue. Drawing on social representation theory and on the process of othering, a lexicometric analysis of Nepalese press articles (<i>N</i> = 440) was conducted to explore if, and how, Othering of the malnourished is at play and how articles (re)construct the threat of malnutrition. The results indicate that Othering takes different forms depending on the threat the malnourished represent: (a) the malnourished in foreign countries are perceived as suffering from hunger due to a lack of social development and democracy whereas (b) the malnourished in Nepal are perceived as being afflicted because they hold traditional beliefs. However, (c) when the threat can be attributed to natural disasters, no negative attributes were associated with the malnourished in Nepal. This suggests that Othering is neither systematic nor monolithic. These findings illustrate how social psychology can address urgent societal questions while equally offering an opportunity for new theoretical developments.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1280-1295"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141365516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived general similarity and relationship satisfaction: The role of attributional confidence 感知的一般相似性与关系满意度:归因信心的作用
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3085
Ting Hin Lee, Ting Kin Ng

Even though many studies have examined the link between individuals' perceived general similarity to their romantic partners and their own relationship satisfaction, there are not many studies focusing on the impact of perceived general similarity reported by the partner. The present work uncovers the potential mediating roles of one's and the partner's attributional confidence in the effects of one's and the partner's perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction. To investigate the mechanisms, two studies were conducted at individual and dyadic levels. A total of 395 individuals (51.4% female) aged 17–67 (= 22.06, SD = 4.68) in unmarried opposite-gender relationships were recruited in Study 1 and 227 unmarried opposite-gender couples were employed in Study 2. Across two studies, the indirect effect of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction via attributional confidence was significant. In particular, the actor effects of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction were mediated by the actor effects of attributional confidence. Moreover, the partner effects of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction were mediated by partner attributional confidence (i.e., one's partner's perceived general similarity on one's relationship satisfaction via one's partner's attributional confidence). The present findings shed light on the mechanisms through which perceived general similarity fosters relationship satisfaction among opposite-gender relationships.

尽管许多研究都探讨了个人感知到的与其恋爱伴侣的总体相似性与其自身关系满意度之间的联系,但关注伴侣报告的感知到的总体相似性的影响的研究并不多。本研究揭示了个人和伴侣的归因信心在个人和伴侣感知到的总体相似性对关系满意度的影响中的潜在中介作用。为了探究其中的机制,我们分别从个人和伴侣两个层面进行了研究。研究一共招募了 395 名年龄在 17-67 岁(M=22.06,SD=4.68)的未婚异性关系中的个体(51.4% 为女性),研究二则招募了 227 对未婚异性伴侣。在两项研究中,感知到的一般相似性通过归因信心对关系满意度的间接影响是显著的。特别是,感知到的总体相似性对关系满意度的行为者效应是由归因信心的行为者效应中介的。此外,感知到的一般相似性对关系满意度的伴侣效应是由伴侣的归因信心所中介的(即一个人的伴侣感知到的一般相似性通过其伴侣的归因信心对其关系满意度产生影响)。本研究结果揭示了感知到的一般相似性促进异性关系满意度的机制。
{"title":"Perceived general similarity and relationship satisfaction: The role of attributional confidence","authors":"Ting Hin Lee,&nbsp;Ting Kin Ng","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3085","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3085","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Even though many studies have examined the link between individuals' perceived general similarity to their romantic partners and their own relationship satisfaction, there are not many studies focusing on the impact of perceived general similarity reported by the partner. The present work uncovers the potential mediating roles of one's and the partner's attributional confidence in the effects of one's and the partner's perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction. To investigate the mechanisms, two studies were conducted at individual and dyadic levels. A total of 395 individuals (51.4% female) aged 17–67 (<i>M </i>= 22.06, <i>SD </i>= 4.68) in unmarried opposite-gender relationships were recruited in Study 1 and 227 unmarried opposite-gender couples were employed in Study 2. Across two studies, the indirect effect of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction via attributional confidence was significant. In particular, the actor effects of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction were mediated by the actor effects of attributional confidence. Moreover, the partner effects of perceived general similarity on relationship satisfaction were mediated by partner attributional confidence (i.e., one's partner's perceived general similarity on one's relationship satisfaction via one's partner's attributional confidence). The present findings shed light on the mechanisms through which perceived general similarity fosters relationship satisfaction among opposite-gender relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1266-1279"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3085","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141374463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The meaning of touch: Relational and individual variables shape emotions and intentions associated with imagined social touch 触摸的意义:关系和个体变量决定了与想象中的社会接触相关的情感和意图
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-04 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3076
Charlotte Krahé, Aikaterini Fotopoulou, Claudia Hammond, Michael J. Banissy, Athanasios Koukoutsakis, Paul M. Jenkinson

Touch is a key channel for conveying meaning in social interactions. The affective quality of touch and its effects on well-being are shaped by relational context (relationship between touch giver vs. recipient) and person variables (e.g. adult attachment style). Yet, such effects have not been explored in relation to the meaning ascribed to touch. We used data from the Touch Test, the world's largest touch survey, which included questions on the degree to which people felt and related specific emotions and intentions to imagined gentle stroking touch and hugs. In N = 23,428, we examined how relational context (imagined source of touch) and person variables (gender, recalled positive childhood touch and adult attachment style) were associated with positive (e.g. love, desire, support) and negative (e.g. fear, anger, warning) emotions and intentions related to imagined touch. Love, desire and support were endorsed more when participants had had their partner (vs. someone else) in mind, and women (vs. men) gave lower ratings for desire overall. Gentle stroking touch was most linked with arousal when participants had had their partner in mind. Further, more positive childhood touch and secure and anxious attachment scores were associated with more positive emotions and intentions, while the opposite was found for avoidant attachment scores. Lastly, positive childhood touch and higher anxious attachment scores were related to greater discrimination between distinct emotion and intention categories, while higher attachment avoidance was associated with reduced discriminability. Thus, contextual and person variables matter in shaping the meaning of social touch.

触摸是社会交往中传递意义的一个重要渠道。触摸的情感质量及其对幸福感的影响受关系背景(触摸给予者与接受者之间的关系)和个人变量(如成人依恋风格)的影响。然而,这些影响尚未与触摸的意义相关联。我们使用了世界上最大的触摸调查 "触摸测试"(Touch Test)的数据,其中包括人们对想象中的轻柔抚摸和拥抱的感受程度以及与之相关的特定情绪和意图的问题。在 N = 23,428 人中,我们研究了关系背景(想象中的触摸来源)和个人变量(性别、回忆中的积极童年触摸和成人依恋风格)如何与想象中的触摸相关的积极(如爱、渴望、支持)和消极(如恐惧、愤怒、警告)情绪和意向相关联。当参与者想到自己的伴侣(而不是其他人)时,爱、渴望和支持会得到更多的认可,而女性(而不是男性)对渴望的总体评价较低。当参与者想到自己的伴侣时,温柔的抚摸与唤醒的关系最为密切。此外,童年时期更积极的抚摸以及安全型和焦虑型依恋得分与更积极的情绪和意图相关,而回避型依恋得分则与之相反。最后,积极的童年抚摸和较高的焦虑依恋得分与不同情感和意图类别之间较高的辨别能力有关,而较高的依恋回避则与较低的辨别能力有关。因此,情境和个人变量在塑造社会接触的意义方面非常重要。
{"title":"The meaning of touch: Relational and individual variables shape emotions and intentions associated with imagined social touch","authors":"Charlotte Krahé,&nbsp;Aikaterini Fotopoulou,&nbsp;Claudia Hammond,&nbsp;Michael J. Banissy,&nbsp;Athanasios Koukoutsakis,&nbsp;Paul M. Jenkinson","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3076","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3076","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Touch is a key channel for conveying meaning in social interactions. The affective quality of touch and its effects on well-being are shaped by relational context (relationship between touch giver vs. recipient) and person variables (e.g. adult attachment style). Yet, such effects have not been explored in relation to the <i>meaning</i> ascribed to touch. We used data from the Touch Test, the world's largest touch survey, which included questions on the degree to which people felt and related specific emotions and intentions to imagined gentle stroking touch and hugs. In <i>N =</i> 23,428, we examined how relational context (imagined source of touch) and person variables (gender, recalled positive childhood touch and adult attachment style) were associated with positive (e.g. love, desire, support) and negative (e.g. fear, anger, warning) emotions and intentions related to imagined touch. Love, desire and support were endorsed more when participants had had their partner (vs. someone else) in mind, and women (vs. men) gave lower ratings for desire overall. Gentle stroking touch was most linked with arousal when participants had had their partner in mind. Further, more positive childhood touch and secure and anxious attachment scores were associated with more positive emotions and intentions, while the opposite was found for avoidant attachment scores. Lastly, positive childhood touch and higher anxious attachment scores were related to greater discrimination between distinct emotion and intention categories, while higher attachment avoidance was associated with reduced discriminability. Thus, contextual and person variables matter in shaping the meaning of social touch.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1247-1265"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3076","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141386950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Keeping score: Past victimization reduces offenders’ conciliatory sentiments for their present transgressions 记分:过去的受害经历会减少犯罪者对当前违法行为的和解情绪
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3075
Michael Thai, Michael Wenzel, Blake Quinney, Lydia Woodyatt, Tyler G. Okimoto

Many relationships within which interpersonal transgressions transpire often involve histories of reciprocal wrongdoing, where each party has occupied both the role of victim and offender. We investigate whether past incidents of being wronged by the victim of a present transgression may dampen offenders’ conciliatory sentiments for their present wrongdoing. Across four studies (combined N = 1037), we find evidence that past victimization experiences within the context of an interpersonal relationship can blur offenders’ construal of their role as offender and elicit feelings of victimhood, allowing them to exonerate themselves and feel less guilt for their present wrongdoing, display less empathy for the present victim, and reduce their willingness to reconcile with the present victim. These findings highlight the importance of taking into account historical transgressions within a relationship as a determinant of relationship repair in the aftermath of present wrongdoings.

许多发生人际越轨行为的人际关系往往都有相互不法行为的历史,在这种关系中,每一方都同时扮演着受害者和犯罪者的角色。我们研究了过去被当前违法行为的受害者冤枉的事件是否会削弱违法者对其当前不法行为的和解情绪。在四项研究中(总人数=1037),我们发现有证据表明,在人际关系中,过去的受害经历会模糊犯罪者对其犯罪者角色的理解,并引发受害情绪,使他们为自己开脱,减少对当前不法行为的内疚感,减少对当前受害者的同情,并降低他们与当前受害者和解的意愿。这些研究结果突出表明,在当前错误行为发生后,考虑关系中的历史过失作为关系修复的决定因素非常重要。
{"title":"Keeping score: Past victimization reduces offenders’ conciliatory sentiments for their present transgressions","authors":"Michael Thai,&nbsp;Michael Wenzel,&nbsp;Blake Quinney,&nbsp;Lydia Woodyatt,&nbsp;Tyler G. Okimoto","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3075","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3075","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many relationships within which interpersonal transgressions transpire often involve histories of reciprocal wrongdoing, where each party has occupied both the role of victim <i>and</i> offender. We investigate whether past incidents of being wronged by the victim of a present transgression may dampen offenders’ conciliatory sentiments for their present wrongdoing. Across four studies (combined <i>N</i> = 1037), we find evidence that past victimization experiences within the context of an interpersonal relationship can blur offenders’ construal of their role as offender and elicit feelings of victimhood, allowing them to exonerate themselves and feel less guilt for their present wrongdoing, display less empathy for the present victim, and reduce their willingness to reconcile with the present victim. These findings highlight the importance of taking into account historical transgressions within a relationship as a determinant of relationship repair in the aftermath of present wrongdoings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1228-1246"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3075","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141148724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral commitment to gender equality increases (mis)perceptions of gender bias in hiring 对性别平等的道德承诺增加了对招聘中性别偏见的(错误)认知
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.3071
Hualin Xiao, Antoine Marie, Brent Strickland

Exploring what modulates people's trust in evidence of hiring discrimination is crucial to the deployment of corrective policies. Here, we explore one powerful source of variation in such judgments: moral commitment to gender equality (MCGE), that is, perceptions of the issue as a moral imperative and as identity-defining. Across seven experiments (N = 3579), we examined folk evaluations of scientific reports of hiring discrimination in academia. Participants who were more morally committed to gender equality were more likely to trust rigorous, experimental evidence of gender discrimination against women. This association between moral commitment and research evaluations was not reducible to prior beliefs, and largely explained a sex difference in people's evaluations on the issue. On a darker note, however, MCGE was associated with increased chances of fallaciously inferring discrimination against women from contradictory evidence. Overall, our results suggest that moral convictions amplify people's myside bias, bringing about both benefits and costs in the public consumption of science.

探索是什么影响了人们对雇佣歧视证据的信任,这对制定纠正政策至关重要。在此,我们探讨了此类判断中一个强大的变异来源:对性别平等的道德承诺(MCGE),即对该问题作为道德义务和身份界定的看法。在七个实验中(N = 3579),我们考察了民间对学术界雇佣歧视科学报告的评价。在道德上更致力于性别平等的参与者更有可能相信严谨的、实验性的女性性别歧视证据。道德承诺与研究评价之间的这种关联并不能归因于先前的信念,它在很大程度上解释了人们对这一问题评价的性别差异。然而,从更阴暗的角度来看,道德承诺与研究评价之间的联系也增加了人们从相互矛盾的证据中谬误地推断出对女性歧视的几率。总之,我们的研究结果表明,道德信念会放大人们的 "我的偏见",从而给公众的科学消费带来好处和代价。
{"title":"Moral commitment to gender equality increases (mis)perceptions of gender bias in hiring","authors":"Hualin Xiao,&nbsp;Antoine Marie,&nbsp;Brent Strickland","doi":"10.1002/ejsp.3071","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ejsp.3071","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Exploring what modulates people's trust in evidence of hiring discrimination is crucial to the deployment of corrective policies. Here, we explore one powerful source of variation in such judgments: moral commitment to gender equality (MCGE), that is, perceptions of the issue as a moral imperative and as identity-defining. Across seven experiments (N = 3579), we examined folk evaluations of scientific reports of hiring discrimination in academia. Participants who were more morally committed to gender equality were more likely to trust rigorous, experimental evidence of gender discrimination against women. This association between moral commitment and research evaluations was not reducible to prior beliefs, and largely explained a sex difference in people's evaluations on the issue. On a darker note, however, MCGE was associated with increased chances of fallaciously inferring discrimination against women from contradictory evidence. Overall, our results suggest that moral convictions amplify people's myside bias, bringing about both benefits and costs in the public consumption of science.</p>","PeriodicalId":48377,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"54 6","pages":"1211-1227"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ejsp.3071","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141148743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
European Journal of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1