首页 > 最新文献

Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Inhibiting orofacial mimicry affects authenticity perception in vocal emotions. 抑制口型模仿会影响声乐情感的真实性感知。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001361
Ricardo F Vilaverde, Oleksandr V Horchak, Ana P Pinheiro, Sophie K Scott, Sebastian Korb, César F Lima

Although emotional mimicry is ubiquitous in social interactions, its mechanisms and roles remain disputed. A prevalent view is that imitating others' expressions facilitates emotional understanding, but the evidence is mixed and almost entirely based on facial emotions. In a preregistered study, we asked whether inhibiting orofacial mimicry affects authenticity perception in vocal emotions. Participants listened to authentic and posed laughs and cries, while holding a pen between the teeth and lips to inhibit orofacial responses (n = 75), or while responding freely without a pen (n = 75). They made authenticity judgments and rated how much they felt the conveyed emotions (emotional contagion). Mimicry inhibition decreased the accuracy of authenticity perception in laughter and crying, and in posed and authentic vocalizations. It did not affect contagion ratings, however, nor performance in a cognitive control task, ruling out the effort of holding the pen as an explanation for the decrements in authenticity perception. Laughter was more contagious than crying, and authentic vocalizations were more contagious than posed ones, regardless of whether mimicry was inhibited or not. These findings confirm the role of mimicry in emotional understanding and extend it to auditory emotions. They also imply that perceived emotional contagion can be unrelated to mimicry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管情绪模仿在社会交往中无处不在,但其机制和作用仍存在争议。一种普遍的观点认为,模仿他人的表情能促进情感理解,但证据不一,而且几乎完全基于面部情绪。在一项预先登记的研究中,我们询问了抑制口面部模仿是否会影响声音情绪的真实性感知。参与者在用笔夹住牙齿和嘴唇以抑制口面部反应(n = 75)或不用笔自由反应(n = 75)的情况下,聆听了真实的和装模作样的笑声和哭声。他们进行真实性判断,并评定自己对所传达情绪的感受程度(情绪传染)。模仿抑制降低了对笑声和哭声、假声和真声的真实性感知的准确性。然而,模仿抑制并不影响传染评级,也不影响认知控制任务中的表现,这就排除了因握笔费力而导致真实性感知下降的可能性。无论模仿是否受到抑制,笑声都比哭声更有传染性,真实的发声也比摆拍的发声更有传染性。这些发现证实了模仿在情绪理解中的作用,并将其扩展到听觉情绪。这些发现还暗示,感知到的情绪感染可能与模仿无关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Inhibiting orofacial mimicry affects authenticity perception in vocal emotions.","authors":"Ricardo F Vilaverde, Oleksandr V Horchak, Ana P Pinheiro, Sophie K Scott, Sebastian Korb, César F Lima","doi":"10.1037/emo0001361","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001361","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although emotional mimicry is ubiquitous in social interactions, its mechanisms and roles remain disputed. A prevalent view is that imitating others' expressions facilitates emotional understanding, but the evidence is mixed and almost entirely based on facial emotions. In a preregistered study, we asked whether inhibiting orofacial mimicry affects authenticity perception in vocal emotions. Participants listened to authentic and posed laughs and cries, while holding a pen between the teeth and lips to inhibit orofacial responses (<i>n</i> = 75), or while responding freely without a pen (<i>n</i> = 75). They made authenticity judgments and rated how much they felt the conveyed emotions (emotional contagion). Mimicry inhibition decreased the accuracy of authenticity perception in laughter and crying, and in posed and authentic vocalizations. It did not affect contagion ratings, however, nor performance in a cognitive control task, ruling out the effort of holding the pen as an explanation for the decrements in authenticity perception. Laughter was more contagious than crying, and authentic vocalizations were more contagious than posed ones, regardless of whether mimicry was inhibited or not. These findings confirm the role of mimicry in emotional understanding and extend it to auditory emotions. They also imply that perceived emotional contagion can be unrelated to mimicry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1376-1385"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is all anger created equal? A meta-analytic assessment of anger elicitation in persuasion research. 所有的愤怒都是平等的吗?对说服研究中激发愤怒的元分析评估。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001360
Stefanie Z Demetriades, Callie S Kalny, Monique M Turner, Nathan Walter

Although veritable libraries have been written about anger, the practical and theoretical understanding of its effects has been somewhat hampered by the difficulty of experimentally manipulating this emotion. Thus, key questions related to methodological precision and theoretical clarity remain, specifically with regard to whether and how anger induction techniques may interact with various moderators and elicit other co-occurring emotions in the process. Addressing this gap, a meta-analysis of 31 experimental studies in persuasion offers insights regarding the effect of anger elicitation on felt anger and its sensitivity to a host of theoretically meaningful moderators, as well as the relationship between anger induction and the arousal of other incidental emotions. Findings broadly affirm the complexity of anger as a contested emotion and offer new insight into methodological considerations and theoretical nuances of anger elicitations to be taken into account in persuasion research. These results should be interpreted with the caveat of an exclusive focus on persuasion and a Western-centric corpus of studies, further accenting the need to diversify and expand research into emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管有关愤怒的文章已经多如牛毛,但对其影响的实践和理论理解却因难以在实验中操纵这种情绪而受到一定影响。因此,与方法的精确性和理论的清晰性相关的关键问题仍然存在,特别是关于愤怒诱导技术是否以及如何与各种调节因素相互作用,并在此过程中引发其他并发情绪。为了弥补这一不足,我们对 31 项说服实验研究进行了荟萃分析,就愤怒诱导对感受到的愤怒的影响及其对一系列理论上有意义的调节因素的敏感性,以及愤怒诱导与唤起其他偶发情绪之间的关系提供了见解。研究结果广泛肯定了愤怒作为一种有争议的情绪的复杂性,并为说服研究中需要考虑的愤怒诱发方法和理论上的细微差别提供了新的见解。在解释这些结果时,应注意这些研究只关注劝说和以西方为中心的研究语料库,这进一步强调了情绪研究多样化和扩展的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is all anger created equal? A meta-analytic assessment of anger elicitation in persuasion research.","authors":"Stefanie Z Demetriades, Callie S Kalny, Monique M Turner, Nathan Walter","doi":"10.1037/emo0001360","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001360","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although veritable libraries have been written about anger, the practical and theoretical understanding of its effects has been somewhat hampered by the difficulty of experimentally manipulating this emotion. Thus, key questions related to methodological precision and theoretical clarity remain, specifically with regard to whether and how anger induction techniques may interact with various moderators and elicit other co-occurring emotions in the process. Addressing this gap, a meta-analysis of 31 experimental studies in persuasion offers insights regarding the effect of anger elicitation on felt anger and its sensitivity to a host of theoretically meaningful moderators, as well as the relationship between anger induction and the arousal of other incidental emotions. Findings broadly affirm the complexity of anger as a contested emotion and offer new insight into methodological considerations and theoretical nuances of anger elicitations to be taken into account in persuasion research. These results should be interpreted with the caveat of an exclusive focus on persuasion and a Western-centric corpus of studies, further accenting the need to diversify and expand research into emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1428-1441"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People in Ecuador and the United States conceptualize compassion differently: The role of avoided negative affect. 厄瓜多尔人和美国人对同情心的理解不同:避免负面情绪的作用
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001356
Sofía Sandoval Larco, María Gabriela Romo, María Sol Garcés, Birgit Koopmann-Holm

Even people from frequently studied cultural contexts differ in how they conceptualize compassion, partly because of differences in how much they want to avoid feeling negative. To broaden this past work, we include participants from an understudied cultural context and start to examine the process through which culture shapes compassion. Based on ethnographic and empirical studies that include Ecuadorians, we predicted that Ecuadorians would want to avoid feeling negative less compared to U.S. Americans. Furthermore, we hypothesized that because of these differences in avoided negative affect, compared to U.S. Americans, for Ecuadorians, a compassionate response would contain more emotion sharing, which in turn would be associated with conceptualizing a compassionate face as one that mirrors sadness more and expresses happiness (e.g., a kind smile) less. Using a reverse correlation task, participants in the United States and Ecuador selected the stimuli that most resembled a compassionate face. They also reported how much they wanted to avoid feeling negative and described what a compassionate response would entail. As predicted, compared to U.S. Americans, Ecuadorians wanted to avoid feeling negative less, they conceptualized a compassionate response as one that focused more on emotion sharing, and visualized a compassionate face as one that contained more sadness and less happiness. Furthermore, exploratory analyses suggest that wanting to avoid feeling negative and conceptualizations of a compassionate response as emotion sharing partly sequentially explained the cultural differences in conceptualizations of a compassionate face. What people regard as compassionate differs across cultures, which has important implications for cross-cultural counseling. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

即使是来自经常被研究的文化背景的人,他们对同情心的理解也不尽相同,部分原因是他们希望避免负面情绪的程度不同。为了扩展过去的研究,我们将来自未被充分研究的文化背景的参与者纳入研究范围,并开始研究文化塑造同情心的过程。根据包含厄瓜多尔人的人种学和实证研究,我们预测厄瓜多尔人与美国人相比,更不希望产生负面情绪。此外,我们还假设,与美国人相比,由于在避免负面情绪方面存在这些差异,厄瓜多尔人的同情反应会包含更多的情绪分享,这反过来又会与将同情面孔概念化为更多反映悲伤和较少表达快乐(如善意的微笑)有关。通过反向相关任务,美国和厄瓜多尔的参与者选择了最像同情脸的刺激物。他们还报告了自己希望避免负面情绪的程度,并描述了同情的反应会带来什么结果。正如预测的那样,与美国人相比,厄瓜多尔人更不希望产生负面情绪,他们将同情反应概念化为更注重情感分享的反应,并将同情面孔视觉化为包含更多悲伤和较少快乐的面孔。此外,探索性分析表明,想要避免消极情绪和将同情反应概念化为情感分享在一定程度上解释了同情面孔概念化的文化差异。不同文化背景下人们所认为的同情心是不同的,这对跨文化心理咨询具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"People in Ecuador and the United States conceptualize compassion differently: The role of avoided negative affect.","authors":"Sofía Sandoval Larco, María Gabriela Romo, María Sol Garcés, Birgit Koopmann-Holm","doi":"10.1037/emo0001356","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001356","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Even people from frequently studied cultural contexts differ in how they conceptualize compassion, partly because of differences in how much they want to avoid feeling negative. To broaden this past work, we include participants from an understudied cultural context and start to examine the process through which culture shapes compassion. Based on ethnographic and empirical studies that include Ecuadorians, we predicted that Ecuadorians would want to avoid feeling negative less compared to U.S. Americans. Furthermore, we hypothesized that because of these differences in avoided negative affect, compared to U.S. Americans, for Ecuadorians, a compassionate response would contain more emotion sharing, which in turn would be associated with conceptualizing a compassionate face as one that mirrors sadness more and expresses happiness (e.g., a kind smile) less. Using a reverse correlation task, participants in the United States and Ecuador selected the stimuli that most resembled a compassionate face. They also reported how much they wanted to avoid feeling negative and described what a compassionate response would entail. As predicted, compared to U.S. Americans, Ecuadorians wanted to avoid feeling negative less, they conceptualized a compassionate response as one that focused more on emotion sharing, and visualized a compassionate face as one that contained more sadness and less happiness. Furthermore, exploratory analyses suggest that wanting to avoid feeling negative and conceptualizations of a compassionate response as emotion sharing partly sequentially explained the cultural differences in conceptualizations of a compassionate face. What people regard as compassionate differs across cultures, which has important implications for cross-cultural counseling. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1456-1467"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multidimensional signal detection modeling reveals Gestalt-like perceptual integration of face emotion and identity. 多维信号检测建模揭示了类似于 Gestalt 的面部情绪和身份的感知整合。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001352
S Sanaz Hosseini, Fabian A Soto

Numerous studies have tested the hypothesis that facial identity and emotional expression are independently processed, but a solid conclusion has been difficult to reach, with the literature showing contradictory results. We argue that this is partly due to different researchers using different definitions of perceptual integration and independence, usually vague and/or simply operational, and also due to lack of proper stimulus control. Here, we performed a study using three-dimensional realistic computer-generated faces for which the discriminability of identities and expressions, the intensity of the expressions, and low-level features of the faces were controlled. A large number of participants, distributed across twelve experimental groups, performed identification tasks for the six basic emotional expressions and the neutral expression (between 2018 and 2019). A multidimensional signal detection model was utilized to analyze the data, which allowed us to dissociate between multiple formally defined notions of independence and holism. Results showed strong and robust violations of perceptual independence that were consistent across all experiments and suggest Gestalt-like perceptual integration of face identity and expression. To date, our results provide the strongest evidence for holistic/Gestalt processing found among face perception studies that have used formal definitions of independence and holism. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多研究都对面部特征和情绪表达是独立处理的这一假设进行了测试,但很难得出可靠的结论,文献显示的结果相互矛盾。我们认为,造成这种情况的部分原因是不同的研究人员对知觉整合和独立使用了不同的定义,这些定义通常比较模糊和/或只是简单的操作性定义,另外还因为缺乏适当的刺激控制。在这里,我们使用计算机生成的三维真实人脸进行了一项研究,该研究控制了身份和表情的可辨别性、表情的强度以及人脸的低级特征。大量参与者分布在十二个实验小组中,对六种基本情绪表情和中性表情(2018 年至 2019 年)进行了识别任务。我们利用多维信号检测模型对数据进行了分析,从而将多个正式定义的独立性和整体性概念区分开来。结果显示,在所有实验中都存在强烈而稳健的违反知觉独立性的情况,这表明对人脸身份和表情的知觉整合类似于盖世太保。迄今为止,我们的研究结果为使用独立性和整体性正式定义的人脸感知研究中发现的整体/格式塔处理提供了最有力的证据。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Multidimensional signal detection modeling reveals Gestalt-like perceptual integration of face emotion and identity.","authors":"S Sanaz Hosseini, Fabian A Soto","doi":"10.1037/emo0001352","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001352","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Numerous studies have tested the hypothesis that facial identity and emotional expression are independently processed, but a solid conclusion has been difficult to reach, with the literature showing contradictory results. We argue that this is partly due to different researchers using different definitions of perceptual integration and independence, usually vague and/or simply operational, and also due to lack of proper stimulus control. Here, we performed a study using three-dimensional realistic computer-generated faces for which the discriminability of identities and expressions, the intensity of the expressions, and low-level features of the faces were controlled. A large number of participants, distributed across twelve experimental groups, performed identification tasks for the six basic emotional expressions and the neutral expression (between 2018 and 2019). A multidimensional signal detection model was utilized to analyze the data, which allowed us to dissociate between multiple formally defined notions of independence and holism. Results showed strong and robust violations of perceptual independence that were consistent across all experiments and suggest Gestalt-like perceptual integration of face identity and expression. To date, our results provide the strongest evidence for holistic/Gestalt processing found among face perception studies that have used formal definitions of independence and holism. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1494-1502"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140337314","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social class, schadenfreude, and children's prosocial behavior in moral contexts. 社会阶层、幸灾乐祸和儿童在道德情境中的亲社会行为。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001363
Zuo-Jun Wang, Ya-Meng Wang, Ying Wei, Ting-Ting Zhang, Fei Wang, Kai Qin Chan

Previous research has shown mixed results regarding the relationship between social class and children's prosocial behavior. The current study aims to further our understanding of these findings by exploring the relationship between social class and children's prosocial behavior in a moral context. Study 1 (N = 833) found that when a target child pursued a morally negative goal and subsequently experienced misfortune, children from higher social class, compared to those from lower social class, experienced greater schadenfreude and exhibited less prosocial behavior. The relation between social class and prosocial behavior was mediated by schadenfreude. Study 2 (N = 389) investigated whether the greater schadenfreude experienced by children from higher social class was due to a weaker empathic response to misfortune or a stronger sense of deservingness. The results revealed a sequential mediation effect of social class on prosocial behavior through deservingness and schadenfreude. These findings provide insight into the impact of social class on the development of children's moral judgment, emotions, and behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于社会阶层与儿童亲社会行为之间的关系,以往的研究结果不一。本研究旨在通过探讨道德背景下社会阶层与儿童亲社会行为之间的关系,进一步加深我们对这些研究结果的理解。研究一(N = 833)发现,当目标儿童追求道德上的负面目标并随后遭遇不幸时,与社会阶层较低的儿童相比,社会阶层较高的儿童会有更多的幸灾乐祸,表现出的亲社会行为也较少。社会阶层与亲社会行为之间的关系是由幸灾乐祸中介的。研究 2(N = 389)调查了社会阶层较高的儿童所经历的更大的幸灾乐祸是由于对不幸的移情反应较弱还是由于应得感较强。结果显示,社会阶层通过应得感和幸灾乐祸对亲社会行为产生了连续的中介效应。这些研究结果让我们深入了解了社会阶层对儿童道德判断、情感和行为发展的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social class, schadenfreude, and children's prosocial behavior in moral contexts.","authors":"Zuo-Jun Wang, Ya-Meng Wang, Ying Wei, Ting-Ting Zhang, Fei Wang, Kai Qin Chan","doi":"10.1037/emo0001363","DOIUrl":"10.1037/emo0001363","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has shown mixed results regarding the relationship between social class and children's prosocial behavior. The current study aims to further our understanding of these findings by exploring the relationship between social class and children's prosocial behavior in a moral context. Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 833) found that when a target child pursued a morally negative goal and subsequently experienced misfortune, children from higher social class, compared to those from lower social class, experienced greater schadenfreude and exhibited less prosocial behavior. The relation between social class and prosocial behavior was mediated by schadenfreude. Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 389) investigated whether the greater schadenfreude experienced by children from higher social class was due to a weaker empathic response to misfortune or a stronger sense of deservingness. The results revealed a sequential mediation effect of social class on prosocial behavior through deservingness and schadenfreude. These findings provide insight into the impact of social class on the development of children's moral judgment, emotions, and behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1417-1427"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177254","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamics between affect and social acceptance as a function of social anxiety: A person-specific network approach. 作为社交焦虑的函数,情感与社会接受之间的动态关系:特定人群网络方法。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001370
Kelley E Gunther, Audrey Edelman, Daniel Petrie, Hedy Kober, Dylan G Gee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack

Social acceptance and rejection are salient experiences, especially during adolescence. Acceptance and rejection relate to changes in positive and negative affect, although directionality of the relation remains unclear. The ability to regulate affect following social experiences may be part of the etiology of social anxiety disorder. With the importance of social cues in adolescence, as well as adolescence as a key window for the onset of social anxiety, we used daily diary data collected in a sample ranging from 9 to 18 years to examine daily changes in acceptance, rejection, positive affect, and negative affect. Taking a person-centered approach, we constructed networks directionally linking social experiences and affect, which served as behaviors of interest ("nodes") in the network for each individual. From these networks, we extracted recovery times from different nodes, that is, the number of days it took for a node to return to baseline when (a) the node itself was perturbed and (b) when a connected node was perturbed. We examined associations between network metrics and social anxiety, age, gender, and their interaction. We found that the recovery time of positive affect when social acceptance was perturbed was inversely related with social anxiety and age, suggesting benefits of acceptance may be shorter lasting for those with more (vs. less) social anxiety symptoms and for older (vs. younger) adolescents. We conclude that positive affect may be a critical yet understudied piece in understanding why adolescence is a developmental period of increased risk for psychopathology and for understanding the etiology of social anxiety disorder. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会接纳和拒绝是一种突出的经历,尤其是在青春期。接受和拒绝与积极和消极情绪的变化有关,但这种关系的方向性仍不明确。社交经历后的情绪调节能力可能是社交焦虑症的病因之一。鉴于社交线索在青春期的重要性,以及青春期是社交焦虑症发病的关键窗口期,我们利用从 9 岁到 18 岁的样本中收集的每日日记数据,研究了接受、拒绝、积极情绪和消极情绪的每日变化。我们采用以人为本的方法,构建了将社交经历和情感定向联系起来的网络,作为每个人在网络中感兴趣的行为("节点")。从这些网络中,我们提取了不同节点的恢复时间,即当(a)节点本身受到干扰和(b)连接节点受到干扰时,节点恢复到基线所需的天数。我们研究了网络指标与社交焦虑、年龄、性别及其交互作用之间的关联。我们发现,当社会接纳受到干扰时,积极情绪的恢复时间与社会焦虑和年龄成反比,这表明对于社会焦虑症状较多(与较少)的人和年龄较大(与较小)的青少年来说,接纳带来的益处可能持续时间较短。我们的结论是,积极情绪可能是理解青春期为何是心理病理学风险增加的发育期以及理解社交焦虑症病因的一个关键但未被充分研究的部分。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Dynamics between affect and social acceptance as a function of social anxiety: A person-specific network approach.","authors":"Kelley E Gunther, Audrey Edelman, Daniel Petrie, Hedy Kober, Dylan G Gee, Jutta Joormann, Reuma Gadassi-Polack","doi":"10.1037/emo0001370","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social acceptance and rejection are salient experiences, especially during adolescence. Acceptance and rejection relate to changes in positive and negative affect, although directionality of the relation remains unclear. The ability to regulate affect following social experiences may be part of the etiology of social anxiety disorder. With the importance of social cues in adolescence, as well as adolescence as a key window for the onset of social anxiety, we used daily diary data collected in a sample ranging from 9 to 18 years to examine daily changes in acceptance, rejection, positive affect, and negative affect. Taking a person-centered approach, we constructed networks directionally linking social experiences and affect, which served as behaviors of interest (\"nodes\") in the network for each individual. From these networks, we extracted recovery times from different nodes, that is, the number of days it took for a node to return to baseline when (a) the node itself was perturbed and (b) when a connected node was perturbed. We examined associations between network metrics and social anxiety, age, gender, and their interaction. We found that the recovery time of positive affect when social acceptance was perturbed was inversely related with social anxiety and age, suggesting benefits of acceptance may be shorter lasting for those with more (vs. less) social anxiety symptoms and for older (vs. younger) adolescents. We conclude that positive affect may be a critical yet understudied piece in understanding why adolescence is a developmental period of increased risk for psychopathology and for understanding the etiology of social anxiety disorder. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal symptoms of prenatal depression predict context-incongruent negative emotion in infants. 母亲产前抑郁症状可预测婴儿的情境一致性负面情绪。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001413
Katherine E Jennings, Madeleine E Piper, Jennifer L Kling, Amanda Bruner, Rebecca J Brooker

Prenatal symptoms of depression in mothers are associated with infants' emotional reactivity. Context-incongruent reactivity, comprising mismatches between the eliciting context and emotional reactions, predicts negative long-term socioemotional outcomes in children. However, the etiology of context-incongruent reactivity is largely unknown, obscuring a full understanding of its potential role as a vulnerability in models outlining the transmission of risk for emotion difficulties from mothers to offspring. We tested mothers' (N = 92) prenatal depressive symptoms as prospective predictors of infants' context-incongruent emotion. Greater prenatal symptoms predicted more context-incongruent negativity in infants even when controlling for context-congruent affect. Findings demonstrate a novel utility of context-incongruent emotion as one possible vulnerability linking mothers' prenatal depression to socioemotional difficulties in offspring. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

母亲的产前抑郁症状与婴儿的情绪反应有关。情境不一致反应(包括诱发情境与情绪反应之间的不匹配)可预测儿童长期的负面社会情感结果。然而,情境不一致反应性的病因在很大程度上是未知的,这使得人们无法全面了解它在概述母亲向后代传递情绪障碍风险的模型中可能扮演的脆弱角色。我们测试了母亲(N = 92)产前抑郁症状对婴儿情境一致性情绪的前瞻性预测作用。即使在控制情境一致性情感的情况下,产前抑郁症状越严重,预测婴儿的情境一致性消极性就越高。研究结果表明,作为母亲产前抑郁与后代社会情感障碍之间的一种可能的脆弱性,情境一致性情绪具有新的实用性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Maternal symptoms of prenatal depression predict context-incongruent negative emotion in infants.","authors":"Katherine E Jennings, Madeleine E Piper, Jennifer L Kling, Amanda Bruner, Rebecca J Brooker","doi":"10.1037/emo0001413","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001413","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prenatal symptoms of depression in mothers are associated with infants' emotional reactivity. Context-incongruent reactivity, comprising mismatches between the eliciting context and emotional reactions, predicts negative long-term socioemotional outcomes in children. However, the etiology of context-incongruent reactivity is largely unknown, obscuring a full understanding of its potential role as a vulnerability in models outlining the transmission of risk for emotion difficulties from mothers to offspring. We tested mothers' (<i>N</i> = 92) prenatal depressive symptoms as prospective predictors of infants' context-incongruent emotion. Greater prenatal symptoms predicted more context-incongruent negativity in infants even when controlling for context-congruent affect. Findings demonstrate a novel utility of context-incongruent emotion as one possible vulnerability linking mothers' prenatal depression to socioemotional difficulties in offspring. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judging emotion in natural images of crowds. 判断人群自然图像中的情绪
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001358
Susan Hao, David Whitney, Sonia J Bishop

It has been suggested that humans use summary statistics such as the average of the emotion of individual faces when they rapidly judge group emotion. Previous studies have mainly used faces of actors posing basic emotions, and morphed versions of these faces, against a plain background. In the present study, photographs taken in real-world settings were used to investigate the influence of mean facial emotion, maximal facial emotion, and background context on judgments of group emotion, assessed using dimensional ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance. Background context explained a significant amount of unique variance in group ratings for each dimension. Mean emotion explained additional unique variance for valence ratings, whereas maximal emotion explained additional unique variance for arousal, with dominance showing more mixed results. Removing background context and disrupting the contextual and spatial relationship between faces by randomly replacing faces with ones from other images within the stimulus set increased reliance on mean emotion. However, under all conditions, the maximally arousing face continued to exert an influence on ratings of group arousal, in line with theoretical accounts arguing for a unique bottom-up effect of emotional arousal on attentional competition and postattentive perceptual processing. Together these findings suggest that individuals' reliance on average emotion when judging crowd scenes differs as a function of the dimension of affect. In addition, the presence of background context both directly impacts judgments of crowd emotion and modulates the relative influence of maximal versus mean emotion on these judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有人认为,人类在快速判断群体情绪时,会使用诸如单个人脸情绪平均值之类的汇总统计数据。以前的研究主要使用的是演员在普通背景下摆出基本情绪的脸部表情和这些脸部表情的变形版本。在本研究中,我们使用在真实世界环境中拍摄的照片来研究平均面部情绪、最大面部情绪和背景环境对群体情绪判断的影响。在每个维度的群体评分中,背景情境都能解释大量的独特变异。平均情绪解释了情绪评分的额外独特方差,而最大情绪解释了唤醒评分的额外独特方差,主导地位的结果则较为复杂。通过随机替换刺激集中其他图像中的人脸来移除背景并破坏人脸之间的上下文和空间关系,会增加对平均情绪的依赖。然而,在所有条件下,唤醒程度最高的面孔都会继续影响对群体唤醒程度的评价,这与情绪唤醒对注意竞争和注意后知觉加工的独特自下而上效应的理论观点是一致的。这些研究结果共同表明,个体在判断人群场景时对平均情绪的依赖因情绪维度的不同而不同。此外,背景情境的存在既会直接影响对人群情绪的判断,也会调节最大情绪与平均情绪对这些判断的相对影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Judging emotion in natural images of crowds.","authors":"Susan Hao, David Whitney, Sonia J Bishop","doi":"10.1037/emo0001358","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001358","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been suggested that humans use summary statistics such as the average of the emotion of individual faces when they rapidly judge group emotion. Previous studies have mainly used faces of actors posing basic emotions, and morphed versions of these faces, against a plain background. In the present study, photographs taken in real-world settings were used to investigate the influence of mean facial emotion, maximal facial emotion, and background context on judgments of group emotion, assessed using dimensional ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance. Background context explained a significant amount of unique variance in group ratings for each dimension. Mean emotion explained additional unique variance for valence ratings, whereas maximal emotion explained additional unique variance for arousal, with dominance showing more mixed results. Removing background context and disrupting the contextual and spatial relationship between faces by randomly replacing faces with ones from other images within the stimulus set increased reliance on mean emotion. However, under all conditions, the maximally arousing face continued to exert an influence on ratings of group arousal, in line with theoretical accounts arguing for a unique bottom-up effect of emotional arousal on attentional competition and postattentive perceptual processing. Together these findings suggest that individuals' reliance on average emotion when judging crowd scenes differs as a function of the dimension of affect. In addition, the presence of background context both directly impacts judgments of crowd emotion and modulates the relative influence of maximal versus mean emotion on these judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Affective control in adolescence: The influence of age and depressive symptomatology on working memory. 青春期的情感控制:年龄和抑郁症状对工作记忆的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001390
Kirsty Griffiths, Darren L Dunning, Jenna Parker, Marc Bennett, Susanne Schweizer, Lucy Foulkes, Saz Ahmed, Jovita T Leung, Cait Griffin, Ashok Sakhardande, Willem Kuyken, J Mark G Williams, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Tim Dalgleish, Jason Stretton

People exhibit marked individual variation in their ability to exercise cognitive control in affectively charged situations. Affective control is typically assessed in laboratory settings by comparing performance in carefully constructed executive tasks performed in both affectively neutral and affectively charged contexts. There is some evidence that affective control undergoes significant improvement throughout adolescence, though it is unclear how adolescents deemed at risk of developing depression exercise affective control despite poor affective control being identified as a contributing factor to ongoing mental ill health in adulthood. The present study therefore investigated affective control in a large (n = 425) sample of adolescents (aged 11-18 years) collected from 2016 to 2018. A simultaneous visuospatial search and written storage working memory (WM) capacity task was carried out to examine affective control, using affectively neutral and affectively negative social images as the task-irrelevant distractors. Overall, WM capacity increased as a function of age across both affective conditions. Moreover, we report a significant difference between affective conditions, with WM capacity slightly lower during trials with affectively negative social scenes, relative to neutral. Performance in each condition and the performance "cost" for completing the task in negative relative to neutral conditions was not modulated by depressive symptoms. Furthermore, age did not predict performance cost, irrespective of depressive symptoms. These findings suggest that WM capacity is relatively robust against socioaffective contexts and mood in adolescents. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们在情绪紧张的情况下进行认知控制的能力存在明显的个体差异。情感控制能力通常是在实验室环境中通过比较在情感中性和情感强烈的情境下执行精心设计的执行任务的表现来进行评估的。有证据表明,情感控制能力在整个青春期都会得到显著提高,尽管情感控制能力差被认为是导致成年后心理健康持续不良的一个因素,但被认为有患抑郁症风险的青少年是如何进行情感控制的,目前尚不清楚。因此,本研究调查了2016年至2018年收集的大量青少年(11-18岁)样本(n = 425)的情感控制能力。研究人员使用情感中性和情感负面的社会图像作为任务无关的分心物,同时进行了视觉空间搜索和书面存储工作记忆(WM)能力任务,以考察情感控制能力。总体而言,在两种情绪条件下,工作记忆容量随着年龄的增长而增加。此外,我们还报告了不同情绪条件下的显著差异,在情绪消极的社交场景下,相对于中性情绪,WM 能力略低。在消极情绪条件下相对于中性情绪条件下,每个条件下的表现和完成任务的表现 "成本 "都不受抑郁症状的影响。此外,无论抑郁症状如何,年龄并不能预测成绩成本。这些研究结果表明,青少年的WM能力对社会情感环境和情绪的影响相对较小。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Affective control in adolescence: The influence of age and depressive symptomatology on working memory.","authors":"Kirsty Griffiths, Darren L Dunning, Jenna Parker, Marc Bennett, Susanne Schweizer, Lucy Foulkes, Saz Ahmed, Jovita T Leung, Cait Griffin, Ashok Sakhardande, Willem Kuyken, J Mark G Williams, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Tim Dalgleish, Jason Stretton","doi":"10.1037/emo0001390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001390","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People exhibit marked individual variation in their ability to exercise cognitive control in affectively charged situations. Affective control is typically assessed in laboratory settings by comparing performance in carefully constructed executive tasks performed in both affectively neutral and affectively charged contexts. There is some evidence that affective control undergoes significant improvement throughout adolescence, though it is unclear how adolescents deemed at risk of developing depression exercise affective control despite poor affective control being identified as a contributing factor to ongoing mental ill health in adulthood. The present study therefore investigated affective control in a large (<i>n</i> = 425) sample of adolescents (aged 11-18 years) collected from 2016 to 2018. A simultaneous visuospatial search and written storage working memory (WM) capacity task was carried out to examine affective control, using affectively neutral and affectively negative social images as the task-irrelevant distractors. Overall, WM capacity increased as a function of age across both affective conditions. Moreover, we report a significant difference between affective conditions, with WM capacity slightly lower during trials with affectively negative social scenes, relative to neutral. Performance in each condition and the performance \"cost\" for completing the task in negative relative to neutral conditions was not modulated by depressive symptoms. Furthermore, age did not predict performance cost, irrespective of depressive symptoms. These findings suggest that WM capacity is relatively robust against socioaffective contexts and mood in adolescents. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113501","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is emotion perception altered by gaze direction, gender appearance, and gender identity of the perceived face? 情绪感知是否会因注视方向、性别外观和所感知人脸的性别特征而改变?
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/emo0001406
Robrecht P R D van der Wel, Yana Prodanova, Jason Snyder, Timothy N Welsh, Anne Böckler

The purpose of the present study was to examine how gaze and emotion processing may change due to differences in gender appearance and gender identity of the perceived face. We manipulated gender appearance (male or female), gender identity (cisgender or transgender), gaze direction (direct or averted), and expressed emotions (anger, fear, or neutral) of face models in an emotion rating task. We replicate several previous findings, including a direct gaze advantage, an emotion effect, and an interaction between gaze direction and expressed emotion. In line with previous findings on the influence of facial morphology for face processing, we found that male faces were more quickly and intensely perceived for displays of anger, while female faces were more quickly and intensely perceived for displays of fear. Of key interest, gender identity influenced face perception for different emotion expressions and gaze directions for ratings and reaction times in a variety of ways. For example, transgender male faces were seen as angrier and less fearful than cisgender male faces, while the opposite effect occurred for female faces. These results suggest that face perception is systematically shaped by morphological differences as well as more abstract social constructs related to gender identity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究旨在探讨凝视和情绪处理会如何因所感知人脸的性别外观和性别认同的不同而发生变化。我们在情绪评级任务中操纵了人脸模型的性别外观(男性或女性)、性别身份(顺性别或跨性别)、注视方向(直视或背视)和表达的情绪(愤怒、恐惧或中性)。我们重复了之前的一些发现,包括直接注视优势、情绪效应以及注视方向与表达情绪之间的交互作用。与之前关于面部形态对人脸加工影响的研究结果一致,我们发现男性人脸对愤怒表现的感知更快、更强烈,而女性人脸对恐惧表现的感知更快、更强烈。最令人感兴趣的是,性别认同以多种方式影响着对不同情绪表达的人脸感知以及对评分和反应时间的注视方向。例如,变性男性面孔比同性男性面孔更愤怒、更不恐惧,而女性面孔则相反。这些结果表明,人脸感知受到形态差异以及与性别认同相关的更抽象的社会建构的系统性影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is emotion perception altered by gaze direction, gender appearance, and gender identity of the perceived face?","authors":"Robrecht P R D van der Wel, Yana Prodanova, Jason Snyder, Timothy N Welsh, Anne Böckler","doi":"10.1037/emo0001406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001406","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of the present study was to examine how gaze and emotion processing may change due to differences in gender appearance and gender identity of the perceived face. We manipulated gender appearance (male or female), gender identity (cisgender or transgender), gaze direction (direct or averted), and expressed emotions (anger, fear, or neutral) of face models in an emotion rating task. We replicate several previous findings, including a direct gaze advantage, an emotion effect, and an interaction between gaze direction and expressed emotion. In line with previous findings on the influence of facial morphology for face processing, we found that male faces were more quickly and intensely perceived for displays of anger, while female faces were more quickly and intensely perceived for displays of fear. Of key interest, gender identity influenced face perception for different emotion expressions and gaze directions for ratings and reaction times in a variety of ways. For example, transgender male faces were seen as angrier and less fearful than cisgender male faces, while the opposite effect occurred for female faces. These results suggest that face perception is systematically shaped by morphological differences as well as more abstract social constructs related to gender identity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48417,"journal":{"name":"Emotion","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1